Stereotype content model

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Jump to: navigation, search

The stereotype content model (SCM) is a psychological theory that hypothesizes that stereotypes possess two dimensions: warmth and competence. Social groups are perceived as warm if they do not compete with the ingroup for the same resources (e.g., college space) and they are considered competent if they are high in status (e.g., economically or educationally successful). Thus, lack of competition predicts perceived warmth and status predicts perceived competence.[1][2] The model was first proposed by social psychologist Susan Fiske and her colleagues Amy Cuddy, Peter Glick and Jun Xu.[3]

Dimensions[edit]

Stereotype content model, adapted from Fiske et al. (2002): Four types of stereotypes resulting from combinations of perceived warmth and competence.

The SCM postulates that all social groups (e.g., older people, the homeless, drug addicts) fit within each of the four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence.[1][4] Contradicting earlier theories of stereotype content which assumed that stereotypes reflected unidimensional and uniformly negative attitudes,[5][6] the stereotype content model theorizes that stereotypes are often mixed or ambivalent: groups perceived to be high in one dimension, but low in the other (e.g., old people as rated high on warmth, but low on competence).[1] The groups within each of the four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence elicit distinct emotions:[1][4][7]

  1. Noncompetitive, low-status outgroups are perceived as warm but incompetent (e.g., housewives, disabled and older people) and are usually liked and pitied but disrespected.
  2. Feelings of pride and admiration are aroused by groups considered both competent and warm (e.g., the ingroup, close allies).
  3. Groups regarded as incompetent and not warm (e.g., welfare recipients, poor people) elicit feelings of contempt and pity.
  4. High-status, competitive outgroups are perceived as high on competence but low on warmth (e.g., Asians, Jews, feminists, rich people) and are subject to an envious stereotype which is accompanied by feelings of admiration and resentment.

The ingroup, i.e., the group to which an observer personally belongs, close allies, and societal reference groups (e.g., cultural default groups such as the middle class, heterosexuals) tend to be rated as high on both dimensions.[1] However, there are differences between ingroup perceptions between Western and Eastern cultures, with only Western cultures displaying this ingroup favoritism.[8]

The stereotype content model was empirically tested on a variety of national and international samples and was found to reliably predict stereotype content in different cultural contexts[2][8] and affective reactions toward a variety of different groups.[7] The model has also received support in such domains as interpersonal perception.[9]

Behavioral outcomes[edit]

Four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence and corresponding types of behavior.

The SCM posits that intergroup emotions and stereotypes predict distinct behaviors which can be active, passive, facilitative, and harmful.[10][11] Members of a social group considered incompetent but warm elicit active behavior and the desire to assist them. A passive behavior is one in which the observer perceives the individual as low in both warmth and competence; therefore, they do not engage with the individual at all. A facilitative behavior stems from the observer's belief that the individual is high in both competence and warmth; therefore, they provide them with necessary assistance, but allow for independence. A harmful behavior is implemented when an observer views an individual as high in competence but low in warmth; therefore, they engage in negative behaviors against the individual.

An example of this model would be an observer might perceive a homeless person on low in warmth and low in competence. The combination of these two dimensions creates an emotion of disgust in the observer and will cause them to behave passively.

Implicit stereotypes[edit]

So far most researchers in this area have relied on explicit measures of stereotypes (i.e., self-reports). In fact, there have been very few attempts to systematically study mixed stereotype content with implicit measures. Implicit measures have two main qualities that make them important complements to traditional explicit measures of stereotypes. First, implicit measures are much less susceptible to social desirability bias. Second, implicit measures may tap biases that people cannot accurately identify by means of normal introspection and thus cannot express explicitly even if they are truly motivated to do so. Hence, implicit measures may prove to be valuable tools in the study of mixed stereotype content as predicted by the CSM. Using the implicit-association test, Carlsson and Björklund (2010) demonstrated the existence of implicit mixed stereotypes of two occupational groups, preschool teachers and lawyers. The former were considered warm but incompetent whereas the latter where regarded as cold and competent.[12]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c d e Fiske, Susan T.; Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Glick, Peter; Xu, Jun (2002). "A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (American Psychological Association) 82 (6): 878–902. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.878. 
  2. ^ a b Kassin, Saul M.; Fein, Steven; Markus, Hazel Rose (2011). Social psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. pp. 177–178. ISBN 978-0-495-81240-1. 
  3. ^ Whitley, Bernard E.; Kite, Mary E. (2010). The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning. p. 226. ISBN 978-0-495-59964-7. 
  4. ^ a b Dovidio, John F.; Gaertner, Samuel L. (2010). "Intergroup Bias". In Susan T., Fiske; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Lindzey, Gardner. Handbook of Social Psychology. Volume Two (5th ed.). Hooboken, N.J.: John Wiley. p. 1085. ISBN 978-0-470-13747-5. 
  5. ^ Operario, Don; Fiske, Susan T. (2003). "Stereotypes: Content, Structures, Processes, and Context". In Brown, Rupert; Gaertner, Samuel L. Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Malden, MA: Blackwell. pp. 22–44. ISBN 978-1-4051-0654-2. 
  6. ^ Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T. (2002). "Doddering But Dear: Process, Content, and Function in Stereotyping of Older Persons". In Nelson, Todd D. Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. 7–8. ISBN 978-0-262-14077-5. 
  7. ^ a b Cuddy, Amy J.C.; Fiske, Susan T.; Glick, Peter (2008). "Warmth and Competence as Universal Dimensions of Social Perception: The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS Map". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Elsevier) 40 (1): 61–149. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(07)00002-0. 
  8. ^ a b Cuddy, Amy J. C.; et al. (2009). "Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences". British Journal of Social Psychology (The British Psychological Society) 48 (1): 1–33. doi:10.1348/014466608X314935. 
  9. ^ Russel, Ann Marie; Fiske, Susan T. (2008). "It's All Relative: Social Position and Interpersonal Position". European Journal of Social Psychology 38 (7): 1193–1201. doi:10.1002/ejsp.539. 
  10. ^ Gervais, Sarah J. (2011). "A social psychological perspective of disability justice". In Wiener, Richard L.; Willborn, Steven L. Disability and Aging Discrimination: Perspectives in Law and Psychology. New York: Springer. p. 256. ISBN 978-1-4419-6293-5. 
  11. ^ Uleman, James S.; Saribay, S. Adil (2012). "Initial Impressions of Others". In Deaux, Kay; Snyder, Mark. The Oxford Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 342. ISBN 978-0-19-539899-1. 
  12. ^ Carlsson, Rickard; Björklund, Fredrik (2010). "Implicit stereotype content: Mixed stereotypes can be measured with the implicit association test". Social Psychology 41 (4): 213–222. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000029. Retrieved December 12, 2014. 

External links[edit]