User:Plotkinm/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Although there are many physiological and psychological gender differences in humans, memory, in general, is fairly stable across the sexes. By studying the specific instances in which males and females demonstrate differences in memory, we are able to further understand the brain structures and functions associated with memory.

It is within specific experimental trials that differences appear, such as methods of recalling past events, explicit facial emotion recognition tasks, and neuroimaging studies regarding size and activation of different brain regions. Research seems to focus especially on gender differences in explicit memory. Like many other nuances of the human pysche, these differences are studied with the goal of lending insight to a greater understanding of the human brain.

History of research[edit]

Perceptions of gender differences in cognitive abilities date back to ancient Greece, when the early physician Hippocrates dubbed the term ‘hysteria’ or ‘wandering womb’ to account for emotional instability and mental illness in women.[1] This diagnosis survived up until the mid-19th century and the beginning of the women's suffrage movement, and was used as evidence for women’s inability to handle intellectual work.[1] Prominent physicians of this era, including neurologist Sigmund Freud, argued that women were biologically suited to homemaking and housework, as they did not have enough blood to power both the brain and the uterus. When women began attending university in the late 19th and early 20th centuries, opponents asserted that the high demands of post-secondary education on the female brain would render women sterile.

The mass entrance of women into the workplace during World War I to replace the conscripted men fighting overseas, provided a turning point for views on women’s cognitive abilities. Having demonstrated that they were capable of functioning in the workplace, women gained the right to vote in post-war United States, Canada and the United Kingdom. Though women were able to vote and hold paid employment, they were still not regarded as intellectually equal to men. The development of the encephalization quotient by Harry Jerison in 1973 seemed to confirm popular beliefs and about women’s cognitive abilities; this quotient was one of the first means of indirectly measuring brain size, and it demonstrated that women have, on average, smaller brain areas than men.[2] Modern neuroscience has since demonstrated that women compensate for their smaller brains with increased neuronal density, and there are no significant differences in mean cognitive abilities between men and women. Recent advancements in neuropsychology and cognitive psychology have shown, however, that specific differences in cognition - including memory - do exist. There is an ongoing debate about the causes of those differences, with biology, genetics, culture, and environmental factors all likely contributing.[citation needed]

Explicit Memory[edit]

When participating in a facial emotion recognition task, explicit memory is used. The knowledge of what a face looks like in various emotional states is something that is learned and stored in memory. It is found that women are typically more sensitive to emotional recognition tasks than men.

In a study which assessed identification of emotions on faces[3] (happiness, sadness, fear, anger, disgust, or neutral) females excelled at the explicit identification of emotions, especially fear and sadness. Women are better than men in general at explicit emotional recognition, but especially so with the negative emotions.

Based on brain imaging studies, women also show heightened neural sensitivity to negative emotions compared to men. In addition, women are postulated to have larger orbitofrontal regions which are involved in emotional regulation. This may contribute to heightened accuracy in the facial emotion recognition task, as well as more accurate identification of emotionally laden content.[3]

However, in another study, females showed no difference in remembering details from affective passages versus neutral passages, while men showed more recall for the affective passage.[4] Female recall was stable, and consistent to men’s overall levels, which indicates that women are generally more attentive to remembering verbal passages, and men only become more attentive when the passage has highly emotional content.

Lastly, women show an own sex bias in remembering gendered faces. Females exceed males at facial recognition for other female faces, but not for male faces.[5] A study suggests that women appropriately identify facial expressions in a more timely manner than men do. In this study, participants took the Eyes Test and mood VASs, and their results were recorded for each. Participants were shown images on a screen, and the participants were asked to identify the facial expression they could see. The same study proposes women are able to identify facial expressions more quickly than men because women study the eyes of other people more intently than men. Men tend to set their focus on the mouth rather than the eyes. Fixations on the eyes led to more accurate identification than fixations on the mouth. Women take more time focusing on the eyes than men, supporting the notion of female advantage in facial expression recognition. [6]

Science seems to agree that in general, the small memory differences between genders that do exist, favor female explicit memory.

Semantic verbal fluency is another aspect of explicit memory. A verbal fluency test checks ability to recall facts about the world, and general knowledge such as vocabulary. When asked to list words that start with the same letter or are in the same semantic category, women are able to produce more words than men. This is most likely due to differing styles of recall. Women tend to have a more even balance between clustering (generating words within subcategories) and switching (shifting between clusters) which allows them to come up with more words. Men switch categories less often and tend to make clusters with more words in them. This is not as efficient a strategy as the one generally employed by women.[7] This provides evidence that while there are differences in the sexes verbal fluency abilities, it could be due to differing recall strategies as opposed to major differences in actual semantic knowledge.

Recall strategies and memory[edit]

The examinations of the differences in recall strategies between males and females originated with studies of sexual behavior. In some studies, men reported, on average, to having had more heterosexual sexual partners than women had. [Citation Needed] As this is a statistical impossibility, this phenomenon then became of the focus of studies, some of which examined the hypothesis that this was due to a gender-based deficiency in memory, and recall into gender based recall strategies followed. [Citation Needed]

One experiment into the recall strategy of the number of a persons sexual partners has found differences, between the genders. Males were observed to most often attempt to estimate their number of sexual partners, which in some cases led to overestimation, while the women studied generally attempted to list all of the partners they have had, which due to the potential of forgetting an incident, in some cases led to underestimation. [8]

Differences may also arise due to opposite sexes having diverse interests and motivations.[9] For example, in a study testing recalls of sexual versus non-sexual television advertisements, men were found to recall sexual advertisements better than non-sexual ones. This effect increased when sexual advertisements were embedded in sexual programming. Women, however, were equally good at remembering sexual and nonsexual advertisements. Differing levels of interest in the two types of commercials may explain the gender baises in remembering.[10] This, unfortunately, is not a distinct gender difference, so further investigation into gender biased recall must be made

Short-Term Memory[edit]

Females have been shown to have consistently stronger short-term or working memory than men. Women are thought to be able to hold more items of verbal information in short-term storage at once. This advantage in short-term memory is thought to be linked to women’s superior ability to attend to more than one task at once, or ‘multitask’.[11]

Recent research suggests that men have advantages in specific subtypes of short term memory, specifically those pertaining to visuo-spatial information.

In a brain activation study,[12] working memory tasks showed more bilateral activation in male brains versus overall left hemisphere activation in female brains. This provides evidence that different brain structures may be responsible for short term memory differences in males versus females. In an fMRI study designed to discover the sex differences in working memory, it was shown that women demonstrate more activations in the middle, inferior, and orbital prefrontal regions than men. For women, there was activation in both hemispheres of the brain, while there was only activation in the left hemisphere for men. Even though different areas of the brain were activated as shown in the fMRI, both men and women had the same responses. However, women tend to have more activations in the prefrontal regions than men, even with the same behavioral results. This suggests that there are gender differences in brain activity. [13]

A gender-based stereotype threat study was conducted in hopes to determine if gender-based stereotypes actually effect the performance of men and women in mathematics and memory tasks. . In this study, participants received two tests in which were both separated into two different parts. Participants took a math pre-test, and then they were exposed to the gender-based stereotypes. Then, the participants took a math post-test. The results of each test were recorded and compared by gender. The stereotype is that men are better in mathematical performances than women are. This study’s results showed that the gender-based stereotype threat may have some effect on working memory. While women did show improvements throughout the study, men outperformed women in their improvements. This study suggests that gender-based stereotypes and performance rates are related to the working memory, just not in math. [14]

Memory Loss[edit]

There may be gendered differences in rates of memory decline. Though research on the subject has not always been consistent, it is known that women experience much higher rates of Alzheimer’s disease. This difference in rates was initially attributed to women’s typically longer lifespans, but the relatively small difference in years of life has been found to be insufficient to explain the incidence of a disease that occurs over decades. Recent research has suggested a link between menopausal declines in estrogen and inefficiencies in brain metabolism. A lack of female hormones may decrease the energy efficiency of brain cells, causing the brain to have inadequate fuel and experience subsequent cognitive decline. Clinical trials of hormone replacement therapy have not shown to be effective at preventing the disease. This area requires further investigation in order to understand the differing Alzheimer's rates between genders.[citation needed]

The prognosis of Alzheimer’s disease also differs between men and woman. Though women tend to experience a much sharper decline in grey matter at the onset of the disease, men catch up and eventually overtake women in grey matter loss as the disease progresses.

General age-related memory decline also varies by gender. When all factors, such as age, education, socio-economic status and geographic location are held constant, men are found to be at a 50% increased risk of experiencing significant age-related memory decline.[15]

A study conducted to determine gender differences in memory loss across different age groups suggests that women tend to perform better than men at verbal tasks, while men perform better at visuospatial tasks. The results of this study showed a female advantage during adolescence in memory tasks over males. This study proposes the female advantage in memory tasks is due to their hormonal balance; women seem to outperform men in memory tasks when estrogen levels are high. Women, however, show a decrease in achievement of memory tasks when going through menopause. In adolescence and in older age, men outperform women in visual working memory tasks. The most major differences in gender when it comes to memory are in young adulthood. [16]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b Tasca, Cecilia; Rapetti, M; Carta, MG; Fadda, B (2012). "Women and Hysteria in the History of Mental Health". Clinical Practice & Epidemiology in Mental Health. 8: 110–9. doi:10.2174/1745017901208010110. PMC 3480686. PMID 23115576.
  2. ^ Carne, Ross P.; Vogrin, Simon; Litewka, Lucas; Cook, Mark J. (2006). "Cerebral cortex: An MRI-based study of volume and variance with age and sex". Journal of Clinical Neuroscience. 13 (1): 60–72. doi:10.1016/j.jocn.2005.02.013. PMID 16410199.
  3. ^ a b Williams, Leanne M.; Mathersul, Danielle; Palmer, Donna M.; Gur, Ruben C.; Gur, Raquel E.; Gordon, Evian (2009). "Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: I. Age effects in males and females across 10 decades". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 31 (3): 257–77. doi:10.1080/13803390802255635. PMID 18720177.
  4. ^ Burton, Leslie A.; Rabin, Laura; Vardy, Susan Bernstein; Frohlich, Jonathan; Wyatt, Gwinne; Dimitri, Diana; Constante, Shimon; Guterman, Elan (2004). "Gender differences in implicit and explicit memory for affective passages". Brain and Cognition. 54 (3): 218–24. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.011. PMID 15050778.
  5. ^ Wang, Bo (2013). "Gender difference in recognition memory for neutral and emotional faces". Memory. 21 (8): 991–1003. doi:10.1080/09658211.2013.771273. PMID 23432017.
  6. ^ Hall, Jessica K. (2010). "Sex differences in scanning faces: Does attention to the eyes explain female superiority in facial expression recognition?". Psychology Press. 24 (4): 629-637.
  7. ^ Weiss, Elisabeth M.; Ragland, J. Daniel; Brensinger, Colleen M.; Bilker, Warren B.; Deisenhammer, Eberhard A.; Delazer, Margarete (2006). "Sex differences in clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 12 (4): 502–9. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060656. PMID 16981602.
  8. ^ Bogart, Laura M.; Walt, Lisa C.; Pavlovic, Jelena D.; Ober, Allison J.; Brown, Norman; Kalichman, Seth C. (2007). "Cognitive strategies affecting recall of sexual behavior among high-risk men and women". Health Psychology. 26 (6): 787–93. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.26.6.787. PMID 18020852.
  9. ^ Loftus, Elizabeth F.; Banaji, Mahzarin R.; Schooler, Jonathan W.; Foster, Rachael A. (1987). "Who remembers what?: Gender differences in memory" (PDF). Michigan Quarterly Review. 26: 64–85.
  10. ^ Leka, Jona; McClelland, Alastair; Furnham, Adrian (2013). "Memory for Sexual and Nonsexual Television Commercials as a Function of Viewing Context and Viewer Gender". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 27 (5): 584–92. doi:10.1002/acp.2939.
  11. ^ http://andrewd.ces.clemson.edu/courses/cpsc412/fall03/teams/reports/group5.pdf[full citation needed][unreliable source?]
  12. ^ Speck, Oliver; Ernst, Thomas; Braun, Jochen; Koch, Christoph; Miller, Eric; Chang, Linda (2000). "Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory". NeuroReport. 11 (11): 2581–5. doi:10.1097/00001756-200008030-00046. PMID 10943726.
  13. ^ Goldstein, Jill M. "Sex Differences in Prefrontal Cortical Brain Activity During fMRI of Auditory Verbal Working Memory". Neuropsychology. 19 (4): 500–519.
  14. ^ Tine, Michele; Gotlieb, Rebecca (2013). "Gender-, race-, and income-based stereotype threat: the effects of multiple stigmatized aspects of identity on math performance and working memory function". Soc Psych Educ. 16: 353-376. doi:10.1007/s11218-013-9224-8.
  15. ^ http://www.reuters.com/article/2010/09/07/us-memories-men-idUSTRE68603N20100907[full citation needed]
  16. ^ Pauls, Franz; Petermann, Franz; Lepach, Anja Christina (2013). "Gender differences in episodic memory and visual working memory including effects of age". Memory. 21 (7): 857-874.

Category:Psychology [1]

[2]


[3]

[4]

  1. ^ Goldstein, Jill M. "Sex Differences in Prefrontal Cortical Brain Activity During fMRI of Auditory Verbal Working Memory". Neuropsychology. 19 (4): 500–519.
  2. ^ Hall, Jessica K. (2010). "Sex differences in scanning faces: Does attention to the eyes explain female superiority in facial expression recognition?". Psychology Press. 24 (4): 629-637.
  3. ^ Pauls, Franz; Petermann, Franz; Lepach, Anja Christina (2013). "Gender differences in episodic memory and visual working memory including effects of age". Memory. 21 (7): 857-874.
  4. ^ Tine, Michele; Gotlieb, Rebecca (2013). "Gender-, race-, and income-based stereotype threat: the effects of multiple stigmatized aspects of identity on math performance and working memory function". Soc Psych Educ. 16: 353-376. doi:10.1007/s11218-013-9224-8.