Jump to content

Stereotype embodiment theory

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 203.217.150.68 (talk) at 02:37, 13 December 2013 (cardiovascular events as much as 38 years later than individuals with more positive age stereotypes.[30] does not make sense. Should be: earlier). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Stereotype Embodiment Theory (SET) is a theoretical model first posited by Psychologist Becca Levy to explain the process by which age stereotypes influence the health of older adults.[1]

The effects of age stereotypes on a number of cognitive and physical outcomes (including memory, cardiovascular reactivity, and longevity) are well-documented[2][3][4][5]

SET explains these findings according to a three-step process:

  1. Age stereotypes are internalized from the host culture at a young age.
  2. At some point these age stereotype become “self stereotypes” about oneself as an aging individual.
  3. These self-stereotypes are then consciously and unconsciously activated to exert their effects on individual health.

Underlying these three steps are SET’s four main theoretical premises. According to Levy (2009):

“The theory has four components: The stereotypes (a) become internalized across the lifespan, (b) can operate unconsciously, (c) gain salience from self-relevance, and (d) utilize multiple pathways.”[1]

Although this theory was developed to explain the operation of age stereotypes across the lifepsan, it may also explain how other types of self-stereotypes operate, such as race stereotypes among African Americans and gender stereotypes among women.

Theoretical Premises of Stereotype Embodiment Theory

Internalization of Stereotypes Across the Life Span

Aging stereotypes are internalized starting in early childhood.[6] This process of early internalization is facilitated by the lack of resistances that are usually present when stereotypes are relevant to the personal identity of those exposed to them.[7] Unfortunately, in North America and Europe these stereotypes tend largely to be negative.[3][8][9][10][11]

This process continues on into early adulthood where the acceptance and invocation of negative age stereotypes may represent short-term benefits in the form of greater social and economic resources being allocated to younger, rather than older, adults. Hence, younger adult workers tend to assume that older adult workers are less productive than their younger counterparts even when regular contact with older adult workers proves these assumptions inaccurate.[12][13]

Unconscious Operation of Age Stereotypes

Age stereotypes have been shown to operate unconsciously. For instance, in 1990, a study by Purdue and Gurtman demonstrated that the associations made by their college-aged participants between certain negative traits and old age had an “automatic” component, such that when their participants were subliminally primed (i.e. presented with stimuli at speeds sufficient for perception, but not recognition) with the word “old,” they made associations with negative traits significantly faster than when they were subliminally primed with the word “young.”[14]

Unconscious operation of age stereotypes in older individuals was demonstrated for the first time by Levy (1996) in an experimental study that showed age stereotypes can impact memory of older individuals . These age stereotype-memory findings have been replicated in a number of different laboratories.[15] Additional subliminal priming experiments have further linked the unconscious activation of age stereotypes to an individual’s “will to live,” such that older adults subliminally exposed to positive age stereotypes tended to accept life-prolonging interventions, while those exposed to negative age stereotypes tended to reject such interventions.[16] Additionally, research conducted using the Implicit Association Test (IAT) method has found further evidence for the presence of robust age stereotypes that operate independently of explicitly held age stereotypes.[17][18]

Becca Levy and Mahzarin Banaji are credited with coining the term “implicit ageism” to describe the unconscious operation of age stereotypes and age prejudice.

Salience Gain from Self-Relevance

Levy (2003) in paraphrasing Snyder & Miene (1994) notes that “the old is the only outgroup that inevitably becomes an ingroup for individuals who live long enough” (pp. 33–54).[19] In similar fashion, age stereotypes that at one point were directed outwardly at others who were perceived to be “old” are eventually directed inwardly at the self as age self-stereotypes when that self is recognized to be “old.”[19][20]

Unfortunately, these age self-stereotypes continue to be predominantly negative[18] and exert similarly negative influences on cognitive and physical health (as discussed previously). It is worth noting that positive age stereotypes do exist, and where they predominate they exert similarly positive effects on individual health.[9]

Utilization of Multiple Pathways

The ways in which age stereotypes exert their influence on individual health can be quite varied. However in general, it is hypothesized that these stereotypes exert their influence according to three primary mechanisms: psychological, behavioral, and physiological.[1]

The psychological mechanism operates via self-fulfilling expectations. For example, in a large longitudinal study of twenty years, Levy and colleagues found that those with more positive self-perceptions of aging at baseline tended to have better functional health and greater longevity.[4][7] Another study, to better establish the causal relationship between beliefs about one’s own aging process and subsequent health outcomes, measured these outcomes as a product of randomly assigned, experimental primes. This study found that individuals performed better on cognitive and physical tasks when first subliminally primed with positive age stereotypes, and performed worse when first subliminally primed with negative age stereotypes.[21] Additionally, this study demonstrated a “stereotype-matching effect” whereby the impact of positive and negative age stereotypes on physical and mental health was most greatly manifest when the content of the stereotypes corresponded to the particular health outcome under observation.

The behavioral mechanism operates via health practices. Specifically, when health problems are seen as inevitable consequences of growing old, such perceptions can lead older individuals to consider healthy behaviors as futile [22] and may lead to reductions in self-efficacy.[16] Conversely, Levy and Myers (2004) found that older adults with more positive self-perceptions of aging were significantly more likely to engage in health practices over time than those with more negative self-perceptions of aging .

The physiological mechanism operates via the autonomic nervous system. Cardiovascular reactivity, a measure of the autonomic nervous system’s response to stress, is heightened in the presence of subliminally primed negative age stereotypes and reduced in the presence of subliminally primed positive age stereotypes.[3] While occasional stress is not harmful, repeated elevation of cardiovascular response to stress can be quite detrimental to cardiac health. In fact, Levy and colleagues (2009) found that possessing negative age stereotypes in younger life can double the risk of having an adverse cardiovascular event after the age of 60.[21]

History of Stereotype Embodiment Theory Development

Aging has traditionally been explained in terms of physiological processes that lead to inevitable decline.[23] However, more recent findings suggest that aging is a more subjective experience with health outcomes tied as intimately to social mores and behavior as they are to human biology.[21] Additionally, when age stereotypes have been examined, such examinations have focused on the “targeters” (usually younger adults) rather than the targets (older adults). Stereotype Embodiment Theory has emerged in response to and as a result of these gaps in the field of health and aging.

Until the emergence of Stereotype Embodiment Theory (SET), Stereotype Threat Theory (STT) has provided the primary means of explaining how stereotypes impact targeted individuals. Unlike SET, STT attempts to explain these outcomes solely as a result of individuals’ reactions against negative stereotypes directed at them from external sources. Stereotype threat, thus, does not allow for the possibility of stereotype internalization and only operates in response to negative stereotypes directed against an aware target.[24] Finally, STT assumes the stereotype process is limited to short-term situations, as opposed to SET that assumes a dynamic process that occurs across the lifespan.

In contrast to the theoretical assumptions laid out by STT, several lines of research have produced findings that support SET suggest that age stereotypes are in fact internalized, can exert an effect regardless of the target’s awareness, are effective in both negative and positive formulations, and can operate across the lifespan. O’Brien and Hummert (2006) compared SET and STT in a memory study that examined 2 divergent hypotheses – one predicted by SET and one predicted by STT – and found support for the one predicted by SET.[25]

Other relevant results that have laid the foundation for the development of SET are summarized here:

  • Age Stereotypes are Internalized: In a study by Donlon, Ashman, & Levy, (2005), it was demonstrated that older individuals with greater lifetime exposure to television (a source of predominantly negative age stereotypes) possessed more negative age stereotypes, thus suggesting that age stereotypes are internalized from the individual’s host culture.[26]
  • Positive and Negative Age Stereotypes Operate: In a cross-cultural study by Levy and Langer (1994), memory performance was compared among three groups with varying degrees of negative versus positive age stereotypes. In order of increasing negativity they were: a) mainland Chinese; b) American Deaf; and c) American hearing. As predicted, the mainland Chinese elderly group outperformed the American deaf elderly group that, in turn, outperformed the American hearing elderly group.[9] This finding suggests that cognitive outcomes are indeed sensitive to the ratio of negative to positive stereotypes. Additional confirmatory studies have further demonstrated the independent and opposite effects of positive versus negative age stereotypes.[2][27][28]
  • Age Stereotypes can Operate Unconsciously: In a study by Levy (1996) it was found that age stereotypes can operate without awareness among older individuals. It was shown that by activating age stereotypes subliminally, negative age stereotypes can worsen older individuals memory performance whereas positive age stereotypes can improve their memory performance. Nosek and Banaji (2002) demonstrated that explicit and implicit beliefs each operate independently of the other. In fact, of the 15 belief categories examined (including race and gender), age was found to have the lowest correlation between explicit and implicit beliefs.[29] Additionally, a follow-up study by Nosek et al. (2002) found that while both explicit and implicit attitudes towards aging were negative, implicitly held attitudes tended to be even more negative than explicitly held attitudes.[18]
  • Age Stereotypes are Internalized and Operate across the Lifespan: In a longitudinal study by Levy, Zonderman, Slade, and Ferrucci (2009) it was demonstrated that age stereotypes internalized across the life course could have real health consequences. In their study, it was found that individuals who held negative age stereotypes earlier in life had a greater likelihood of experiencing cardiovascular events as much as 38 years earlier than individuals with more positive age stereotypes.[30]

Summary

Stereotype Embodiment Theory is a novel theory that can help explain how an individual experiences aging in a particular society. It demands sensitivity for the changing circumstances of an individual’s life as he or she grows older and thus allows for the integration of social psychological research with a life course perspective. While SET has been used primarily to explore the influence of age stereotypes on health and functioning, given its flexibility, it seems likely that future research will expand the use of SET to encompass a wider range of stereotype domains most relevant to our cognitive, physical and mental well being.

References

  1. ^ a b c Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A psychosocial approach to aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 332-336.
  2. ^ a b Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age by implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1092–1107.
  3. ^ a b c Levy, B., Hausdorff, J., Hencke, R., & Wei, J. Y. (2000). Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 55B, P205-P213.
  4. ^ a b Levy, B., Slade M. D., & Kasl, S. V. (2002). Increased longevity by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 261-270.
  5. ^ Wurm, S., Tesch-Römer, C., & Tomasik, M.J. (2007). Longitudinal findings on aging-related cognitions, control beliefs and health in later life. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, P156–P164.
  6. ^ De Pallo, M., Thomas, R., Thompson, J., Fox, R. J., Goyer, A., & Abrams, T. (1995). Images of aging in America. Washington, DC: AARP.
  7. ^ a b Levy, B.R., & Banaji, M.R. (2002). Implicit ageism. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  8. ^ Hummert, M. L. (2011). Age stereotypes and aging. In L. L. Carstensen, & T. A. Rando (Ed.), Handbook of the psychology of aging. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  9. ^ a b c Levy, B., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in China and among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 989–997.
  10. ^ Palmore, E. B. (1999). Ageism: Negative and positive. New York: Springer.
  11. ^ Harrop, A. (2011). Agenda for later life 2011: Public policy and an ageing society. ageuk.org.uk (retrieved 4/2/2011).
  12. ^ Finkelstein, L. M., Burke, M. J., & Raju, N. S. (1995). Age discrimination in simulated employment contexts: An integrative analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 652–663.
  13. ^ McCann, R., & Giles, H. (2002). Ageism in the workplace: A communication perspective. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons (pp. 163–199). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  14. ^ Perdue, C. W., & Gurtman, M. B. (1990). Evidence for the automaticity of ageism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 199–216.
  15. ^ Horton, S., Baker, J., & Deakin, J. M. (2007). Stereotypes of aging: their effects on the health of seniors in North American society. Educational Gerontology, 33, 1021-35.
  16. ^ a b Levy, B.R., Ashman, O., & Dror, I. (2000). To be or not to be: The effects of aging stereotypes on the will to live. Omega, 40, 409–420.
  17. ^ Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math=Male, Me=Female, Therefore Math ≠ Me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 44–59.
  18. ^ a b c Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting intergroup attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration website. Group Dynamics, 6, 101–115.
  19. ^ a b Levy, B.R. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, P203–P211.
  20. ^ Rothermund, K. (2005). Effects of age stereotypes on self-views and adaptation. In W. Greve, K. Rothermund, & D. Wentura (Eds.), The adaptive self. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe & Huber.
  21. ^ a b c Levy, B.R., & Leifheit-Limson, E. (2009). The stereotype matching effect: Greater influence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes. Psychology and Aging, 24, 230-233.
  22. ^ Levy, B.R., & Myers, L.M. (2004). Preventive health behaviors influenced by self-perceptions of aging. Preventive Medicine, 39, 625-629.
  23. ^ Masoro, E. J. (2006). Are age-associated diseases an integral part of aging? In E. J. Masoro & S. N. Austad (Eds.), Handbook of the biology of aging. (6th ed., pp. 43-62) New York: Academic Press.
  24. ^ Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectualtest performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.
  25. ^ O’Brien, L. T., & Hummert, M. L. (2006). Memory performance of late middle-aged adults: contrasting self-stereotyping and stereotype threat accounts of assimilation to age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 24, 338-58.
  26. ^ Donlon, M., Ashman, O., & Levy, B.R. (2005). Re-vision of older television characters: A stereotype-awareness intervention. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 307–319.
  27. ^ Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S. J., & Rahhal, T. A. (2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, P3–P11.
  28. ^ Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, J. A., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 638–647.
  29. ^ Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2002). (At least) two factors moderate the relationship between implicit and explicit attitudes. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University, New Haven, CT
  30. ^ Levy, B. R., Zonderman, A. B., Slade, M. D. and Ferrucci, L. (2009). Age Stereotypes Held Earlier in Life Predict Cardiovascular Events in Later Life. Psychological Science. 20, 296-298.