Jump to content

Placebo analgesia: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
Add: s2cid. | You can use this bot yourself. Report bugs here. | Suggested by Abductive | Category:Analgesics | via #UCB_Category
KekeCl12 (talk | contribs)
Addition of Psychological Mechanisms, specifically behaviourism in analgesic responses
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|Ability of placebos to reduce pain}}
{{short description|Ability of placebos to reduce pain}}
'''Placebo analgesia''' occurs when the administration of [[placebo]]s leads to [[pain relief]]. Because placebos by definition lack active ingredients, the effect of placebo analgesia is considered to result from the patient's belief that they are receiving an [[analgesic]] drug or other medical intervention. It has been shown that, in some cases, the [[endogenous opioid]] system is critical for mediating placebo analgesia, as evidenced by the ability of such analgesia to be reduced by the [[opioid antagonist]] [[naloxone]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C | title = Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia | journal = Neuron | volume = 63 | issue = 4 | pages = 533–43 | date = August 2009 | pmid = 19709634 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014 | s2cid = 2828147 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL | title = The mechanism of placebo analgesia | journal = Lancet | volume = 2 | issue = 8091 | pages = 654–7 | date = September 1978 | pmid = 80579 | doi = 10.1016/S0140-6736(78)92762-9 | s2cid = 45403755 }}</ref> However, it is also possible for placebo analgesia to be mediated by non-opioid mechanisms, in which case it would not be affected by naloxone.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK | title = Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 25 | issue = 45 | pages = 10390–402 | date = November 2005 | pmid = 16280578 | pmc = 6725834 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3458-05.2005 }}</ref> Other research has indicated that the human [[spinal cord]],<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, Bingel U, Büchel C | title = Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia | journal = Science | volume = 326 | issue = 5951 | pages = 404 | date = October 2009 | pmid = 19833962 | doi = 10.1126/science.1180142 | bibcode = 2009Sci...326..404E | s2cid = 206522822 }}</ref> [[prefrontal cortex]],<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G, Schedlowski M, Schönbächler G | title = Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia | journal = Pain | volume = 148 | issue = 3 | pages = 368–74 | date = March 2010 | pmid = 19875233 | doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.033 | s2cid = 41113917 }}</ref> and [[rostral (anatomical term)|rostral]] [[anterior cingulate cortex]]<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M | title = Placebo and opioid analgesia-- imaging a shared neuronal network | journal = Science | volume = 295 | issue = 5560 | pages = 1737–40 | date = March 2002 | pmid = 11834781 | doi = 10.1126/science.1067176 | bibcode = 2002Sci...295.1737P | hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0013-2552-D | s2cid = 45262927 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> also play a role in placebo analgesia.
'''Placebo analgesia''' occurs when the administration of [[placebo]]s leads to [[pain relief]]. Because placebos by definition lack active ingredients, the effect of placebo analgesia is considered to result from the patient's belief that they are receiving an [[analgesic]] drug or other medical intervention. It has been shown that, in some cases, the [[endogenous opioid]] system is critical for mediating placebo analgesia, as evidenced by the ability of such analgesia to be reduced by the [[opioid antagonist]] [[naloxone]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C | title = Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia | journal = Neuron | volume = 63 | issue = 4 | pages = 533–43 | date = August 2009 | pmid = 19709634 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014 | s2cid = 2828147 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL | title = The mechanism of placebo analgesia | journal = Lancet | volume = 2 | issue = 8091 | pages = 654–7 | date = September 1978 | pmid = 80579 | doi = 10.1016/S0140-6736(78)92762-9 | s2cid = 45403755 }}</ref> However, it is also possible for placebo analgesia to be mediated by non-opioid mechanisms, in which case it would not be affected by naloxone.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK | title = Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 25 | issue = 45 | pages = 10390–402 | date = November 2005 | pmid = 16280578 | pmc = 6725834 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3458-05.2005 }}</ref> Other research has indicated that the human [[spinal cord]],<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, Bingel U, Büchel C | title = Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia | journal = Science | volume = 326 | issue = 5951 | pages = 404 | date = October 2009 | pmid = 19833962 | doi = 10.1126/science.1180142 | bibcode = 2009Sci...326..404E | s2cid = 206522822 }}</ref> [[prefrontal cortex]],<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G, Schedlowski M, Schönbächler G | title = Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia | journal = Pain | volume = 148 | issue = 3 | pages = 368–74 | date = March 2010 | pmid = 19875233 | doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.033 | s2cid = 41113917 }}</ref> and [[rostral (anatomical term)|rostral]] [[anterior cingulate cortex]]<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M | title = Placebo and opioid analgesia-- imaging a shared neuronal network | journal = Science | volume = 295 | issue = 5560 | pages = 1737–40 | date = March 2002 | pmid = 11834781 | doi = 10.1126/science.1067176 | bibcode = 2002Sci...295.1737P | hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0013-2552-D | s2cid = 45262927 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> also play a role in placebo analgesia.


== Psychological Mechanisms: Behaviourism ==
'''Pavlovian Classical Conditioning'''

[[Classical conditioning]] has been identified as a means by which we can induce placebo analgesia.
Recent research <ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Ober K, Benson S, Vogelsang M, Bylica A, Günther D, Witzke O, Kribben A, Engler H, Schedlowski M| title = Plasma Noradrenaline and State Anxiety
Levels Predict Placebo Response in Learned
Immunosuppression| journal = Clinical Pharmacology & Therapeutics | volume = 91 | issue = 2| pages = 220-226 | date = 2017 | doi = 10.1038/clpt.2011.214}}</ref> demonstrates how a neutral stimulus, experienced with an unconditioned stimulus, can generate a conditioned response, i.e., analgesic effects, after a short conditioning period. In this study, an experimental group was given [[cyclosporine]] with a drink.The drink acts as the neutral stimulus and cyclosporine the unconditioned stimulus. When the two are ingested concurrently, an association is learnt, generating a conditioned stimulus of the drink. This results in a conditioned response of pain-relief. In this particular study, [[analgesia]] was observed during the evocation stage, where participants were exposed to the drink and a placebo, then displayed a decreased [[lymphocyte]] activity.

Classical conditioning has been identified as a means to induce analgesia without drugs also<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Bąbel P, Bajcar E.A, Adamczyk W, Kicman P, Lisińska N, Świder K, Colloca L | title = Classical conditioning without verbal
suggestions elicits placebo analgesia and
nocebo hyperalgesia| journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 12 | issue = 7| pages = e0181856 | date = 2017 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0181856}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Bąbel P, Adamczyk W, Świder K, Bajcar E.A, Kicman P, Lisińska N| title = How Classical Conditioning Shapes Placebo
Analgesia: Hidden versus Open Conditioning| journal = Pain Medicine| volume = 19 | issue = 6| pages = 1156-1169| date = 2017 | doi = 10.1093/pm/pnx177}}</ref>. Common to-be conditioned stimuli in studies investigating analgesia are various coloured lights, e.g., blue, and orange and a common unconditioned stimulus, electric shocks.
During conditioning phases of experiments, one light is displayed at the same time as a shock is administered. The shock comes from electrodes secured to the hands, where the intensity alternates between two extremes, generally high and non-painful, so participants can identify the difference and therefore bridge an association between the lights and shocks given, e.g., blue = not painful, and orange = very painful.

The shocks act as unconditioned stimuli, producing unconditioned responses, i.e., pain, and the neutral stimuli are the different coloured lights. In the experimental phase, participants are exposed to the same lights, but this time the shocks are uncoordinated; orange no longer signifies a painful shock, instead a weaker, non-painful shock and blue, now signifies pain. This is where the analgesic effect occurs. Participants witnessing both lights in a randomised sequence, state that the shock administered with the blue light is less painful than that of the one with the orange. This happens because the association bridged between the lights and shock intensity, gives participants an idea of what the shocks will feel like, when a certain light is shown. Due to this, high intensity shocks that intend to cause pain, won’t generate intense pain, because the pain level associated with that specific light is low.

'''Clinical Applications'''

The applicability to [[pharmacotherapy]] is extensive. On one hand, the theory of classical conditioning suggests that medication dosage could be reduced, by associating it with another substance, like a nice tasting drink, therefore allowing people to slowly reduce and stop taking the medication, whilst maintaining the symptom-relieving effects. Theoretically, it should work, however in practice may not be feasible, as effects of conditioning last for a short period of time, approximately 4-7 days <ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Colloca L, Benedetti F | title = How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia| journal = Pain | volume = 124 | issue = 1 | pages = 126-133 | date = 2006 | doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.005}}</ref>. This would mean constant classical conditioning using the medication would need to be conducted, which is likely to have reservations from an ethical viewpoint. This durability can be affected by a person’s previous experience with therapy. Given a person has undergone therapy before, whether a negative or positive experience, the staggered reduction in stimuli affectivity, to induce an analgesic effect, will create an effect that’s larger, prior the trial and smaller, after it<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Klinger R, Colloca L, Bingel U, Flor H | title = Placebo analgesia: Clinical applications | journal = Pain | volume = 155 | issue = 6 | pages = 1055-1058 | date = 2014 | doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2013.12.007}}</ref>. This in clinical application would be inutile as therapy shouldn’t lose its’ efficacy over an extended period of time.

On the other hand, research on the role that both expectancy, alongside conditioning play in inducing the analgesic effect, have prompted ideas to better the practice of pharmacotherapy. Expectancy in the analgesic effect comes from the conditioning prior, where the analgesic is associated with physiological pain relief. In practice<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Lückemann L, Unteroberdörster M, Kirchog J, Schedlowski M, Hadamitzky M | title = Applications and limitations of behaviourally conditioned immunopharmacological responses | journal = Neurobiology of Learning and Memory | volume = 142| pages = 91-98 | date = July 2017| doi = 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.012}}</ref>, this knowledge can inform how specialists explain medicative therapy to their patients. Specialists could exercise emphasis on the symptom-relieving effect of the tablets, as opposed to the side effects. This would create an expectation in the patient’s mind, of how they should feel upon taking the medication, as well as provide them with confidence it’s going to be beneficial for them. This could be especially useful for treating those with chronic pain.

Classical conditioning can inform pharmacotherapy, in encouraging clinicians to help their patients to create associations between the analgesic medication and between pain relieving techniques<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Lückemann L, Unteroberdörster M, Kirchog J, Schedlowski M, Hadamitzky M | title = Applications and limitations of behaviourally conditioned immunopharmacological responses | journal = Neurobiology of Learning and Memory | volume = 142| pages = 91-98 | date = July 2017| doi = 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.012}}</ref>. By developing these techniques, even if the medication dosage were reduced, to act as a single-blind placebo, those patients would still be able to experience some pain relief. This supports the idea of using behavioural conditioning to relieve patients of their pain and not need to continue medication at the same time, despite the durability constraints.

''' Social Observational Learning'''

The social learning theory, as part of [[behaviourism]], is considered a way by which analgesic effects can be induced<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Colloca L, Benedetti F| title = Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning | journal = Pain | volume = 144| issue = 1 | pages = 28-34 | date = 2009| doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033}}</ref>. Where a demonstrator sits with participants and responds to different coloured shocks administered when different lights appear, like above, the participants observing report the same answers as reported by the demonstrator. Separately, whilst the associations constructed around shock intensity through social observation were true based on statistical tests, the perception of the experience differ from that of the demonstrator. Alongside classical conditioning and social observational learning are verbal suggestions, tested as ways in which to engender analgesic effects. Of the three, classical conditioning and social observational learning are the most effective, regarding the strength of analgesic effects.
An additional contributor to the analgesic effect, is empathy<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Colloca L, Benedetti F| title = Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning | journal = Pain | volume = 144| issue = 1 | pages = 28-34 | date = 2009| doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033}}</ref>. This can be measured using the [[Interpersonal Reactivity Index]], containing subscales of which Empathetic Concern was the aspect positively correlated with social observational learning. This is suggestive of the idea that prosocial behaviour may be linked to placebo effects, as empathy scores tend to be higher where social learning ‘gains’ were greater.

The obtaining of empathy whilst learning how to respond to pain stimuli, is a stark contrast to the first-hand experience of pain, and how empathy is affected by that. Meta-analytical research <ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T| title = Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain | journal = Neuroimage | volume = 54 | issue = 3 | pages = 2492-2502 | date = 1 February 2011| doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033}}</ref>demonstrates shared regions between the experience of pain and empathy. The dynamics of this relatability, having been investigated, show that both functions cannot be at their most active, at the same time<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Rütgen M, Seidel E.-M, Riečanský I, Lamm C | title = Reduction of Empathy for Pain by Placebo Analgesia
Suggests Functional Equivalence of Empathy and First-Hand
Emotion Experience | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience| volume = 35 | issue = 23 | pages = 8938-8947 |date = June 10 2015| doi = 10.1523/jneurosci.3936-14.2015}}</ref>.. Instead, they are negatively correlated, when placebo analgesia is being experienced, the ability to empathise with oneself or others decreases greatly. This has implications for medication, specifically for painkillers, with possible evocation of anti-social attitudes amongst people, as was found with [[acetaminophen]]<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors = Mischkowski D, Crocker J, Way B.M| title = A Social Analgesic? Acetaminophen(Paracetamol) Reduces Positive Empathy | journal = Frontiers in Psychology | volume = 10 | issue = 3 | date = 2019| doi = 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00538}}</ref>.



== References ==
== References ==
{{Reflist|30em}}
{{Reflist|30em}}


{{Neuroscience-stub}}


[[Category:Analgesics]]
[[Category:Analgesics]]

Revision as of 11:24, 17 February 2021

Placebo analgesia occurs when the administration of placebos leads to pain relief. Because placebos by definition lack active ingredients, the effect of placebo analgesia is considered to result from the patient's belief that they are receiving an analgesic drug or other medical intervention. It has been shown that, in some cases, the endogenous opioid system is critical for mediating placebo analgesia, as evidenced by the ability of such analgesia to be reduced by the opioid antagonist naloxone.[1][2] However, it is also possible for placebo analgesia to be mediated by non-opioid mechanisms, in which case it would not be affected by naloxone.[3] Other research has indicated that the human spinal cord,[4] prefrontal cortex,[5] and rostral anterior cingulate cortex[6] also play a role in placebo analgesia.


Psychological Mechanisms: Behaviourism

Pavlovian Classical Conditioning

Classical conditioning has been identified as a means by which we can induce placebo analgesia. Recent research [7] demonstrates how a neutral stimulus, experienced with an unconditioned stimulus, can generate a conditioned response, i.e., analgesic effects, after a short conditioning period. In this study, an experimental group was given cyclosporine with a drink.The drink acts as the neutral stimulus and cyclosporine the unconditioned stimulus. When the two are ingested concurrently, an association is learnt, generating a conditioned stimulus of the drink. This results in a conditioned response of pain-relief. In this particular study, analgesia was observed during the evocation stage, where participants were exposed to the drink and a placebo, then displayed a decreased lymphocyte activity.

Classical conditioning has been identified as a means to induce analgesia without drugs also[8][9]. Common to-be conditioned stimuli in studies investigating analgesia are various coloured lights, e.g., blue, and orange and a common unconditioned stimulus, electric shocks. During conditioning phases of experiments, one light is displayed at the same time as a shock is administered. The shock comes from electrodes secured to the hands, where the intensity alternates between two extremes, generally high and non-painful, so participants can identify the difference and therefore bridge an association between the lights and shocks given, e.g., blue = not painful, and orange = very painful.

The shocks act as unconditioned stimuli, producing unconditioned responses, i.e., pain, and the neutral stimuli are the different coloured lights. In the experimental phase, participants are exposed to the same lights, but this time the shocks are uncoordinated; orange no longer signifies a painful shock, instead a weaker, non-painful shock and blue, now signifies pain. This is where the analgesic effect occurs. Participants witnessing both lights in a randomised sequence, state that the shock administered with the blue light is less painful than that of the one with the orange. This happens because the association bridged between the lights and shock intensity, gives participants an idea of what the shocks will feel like, when a certain light is shown. Due to this, high intensity shocks that intend to cause pain, won’t generate intense pain, because the pain level associated with that specific light is low.

Clinical Applications

The applicability to pharmacotherapy is extensive. On one hand, the theory of classical conditioning suggests that medication dosage could be reduced, by associating it with another substance, like a nice tasting drink, therefore allowing people to slowly reduce and stop taking the medication, whilst maintaining the symptom-relieving effects. Theoretically, it should work, however in practice may not be feasible, as effects of conditioning last for a short period of time, approximately 4-7 days [10]. This would mean constant classical conditioning using the medication would need to be conducted, which is likely to have reservations from an ethical viewpoint. This durability can be affected by a person’s previous experience with therapy. Given a person has undergone therapy before, whether a negative or positive experience, the staggered reduction in stimuli affectivity, to induce an analgesic effect, will create an effect that’s larger, prior the trial and smaller, after it[11]. This in clinical application would be inutile as therapy shouldn’t lose its’ efficacy over an extended period of time.

On the other hand, research on the role that both expectancy, alongside conditioning play in inducing the analgesic effect, have prompted ideas to better the practice of pharmacotherapy. Expectancy in the analgesic effect comes from the conditioning prior, where the analgesic is associated with physiological pain relief. In practice[12], this knowledge can inform how specialists explain medicative therapy to their patients. Specialists could exercise emphasis on the symptom-relieving effect of the tablets, as opposed to the side effects. This would create an expectation in the patient’s mind, of how they should feel upon taking the medication, as well as provide them with confidence it’s going to be beneficial for them. This could be especially useful for treating those with chronic pain.

Classical conditioning can inform pharmacotherapy, in encouraging clinicians to help their patients to create associations between the analgesic medication and between pain relieving techniques[13]. By developing these techniques, even if the medication dosage were reduced, to act as a single-blind placebo, those patients would still be able to experience some pain relief. This supports the idea of using behavioural conditioning to relieve patients of their pain and not need to continue medication at the same time, despite the durability constraints.

Social Observational Learning

The social learning theory, as part of behaviourism, is considered a way by which analgesic effects can be induced[14]. Where a demonstrator sits with participants and responds to different coloured shocks administered when different lights appear, like above, the participants observing report the same answers as reported by the demonstrator. Separately, whilst the associations constructed around shock intensity through social observation were true based on statistical tests, the perception of the experience differ from that of the demonstrator. Alongside classical conditioning and social observational learning are verbal suggestions, tested as ways in which to engender analgesic effects. Of the three, classical conditioning and social observational learning are the most effective, regarding the strength of analgesic effects. An additional contributor to the analgesic effect, is empathy[15]. This can be measured using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, containing subscales of which Empathetic Concern was the aspect positively correlated with social observational learning. This is suggestive of the idea that prosocial behaviour may be linked to placebo effects, as empathy scores tend to be higher where social learning ‘gains’ were greater.

The obtaining of empathy whilst learning how to respond to pain stimuli, is a stark contrast to the first-hand experience of pain, and how empathy is affected by that. Meta-analytical research [16]demonstrates shared regions between the experience of pain and empathy. The dynamics of this relatability, having been investigated, show that both functions cannot be at their most active, at the same time[17].. Instead, they are negatively correlated, when placebo analgesia is being experienced, the ability to empathise with oneself or others decreases greatly. This has implications for medication, specifically for painkillers, with possible evocation of anti-social attitudes amongst people, as was found with acetaminophen[18].


References

  1. ^ Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C (August 2009). "Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia". Neuron. 63 (4): 533–43. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014. PMID 19709634. S2CID 2828147.
  2. ^ Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL (September 1978). "The mechanism of placebo analgesia". Lancet. 2 (8091): 654–7. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(78)92762-9. PMID 80579. S2CID 45403755.
  3. ^ Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK (November 2005). "Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect". The Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (45): 10390–402. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3458-05.2005. PMC 6725834. PMID 16280578.
  4. ^ Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, Bingel U, Büchel C (October 2009). "Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia". Science. 326 (5951): 404. Bibcode:2009Sci...326..404E. doi:10.1126/science.1180142. PMID 19833962. S2CID 206522822.
  5. ^ Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G, Schedlowski M, Schönbächler G (March 2010). "Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia". Pain. 148 (3): 368–74. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.033. PMID 19875233. S2CID 41113917.
  6. ^ Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M (March 2002). "Placebo and opioid analgesia-- imaging a shared neuronal network". Science. 295 (5560): 1737–40. Bibcode:2002Sci...295.1737P. doi:10.1126/science.1067176. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-2552-D. PMID 11834781. S2CID 45262927.
  7. ^ Ober K, Benson S, Vogelsang M, Bylica A, Günther D, Witzke O, Kribben A, Engler H, Schedlowski M (2017). "Plasma Noradrenaline and State Anxiety Levels Predict Placebo Response in Learned Immunosuppression". Clinical Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 91 (2): 220–226. doi:10.1038/clpt.2011.214. {{cite journal}}: line feed character in |title= at position 39 (help)
  8. ^ Bąbel P, Bajcar E.A, Adamczyk WE, Kicman P, Lisińska N, Świder K, Colloca L (2017). "Classical conditioning without verbal suggestions elicits placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia". PLOS ONE. 12 (7): e0181856. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0181856. {{cite journal}}: Vancouver style error: punctuation in name 2 (help); line feed character in |title= at position 38 (help)CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  9. ^ Bąbel P, Adamczyk W, Świder K, Bajcar E.A, Kicman PE, Lisińska N (2017). "How Classical Conditioning Shapes Placebo Analgesia: Hidden versus Open Conditioning". Pain Medicine. 19 (6): 1156–1169. doi:10.1093/pm/pnx177. {{cite journal}}: Vancouver style error: punctuation in name 4 (help); line feed character in |title= at position 42 (help)
  10. ^ Colloca L, Benedetti F (2006). "How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia". Pain. 124 (1): 126–133. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.005.
  11. ^ Klinger R, Colloca L, Bingel U, Flor H (2014). "Placebo analgesia: Clinical applications". Pain. 155 (6): 1055–1058. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.12.007.
  12. ^ Lückemann L, Unteroberdörster M, Kirchog J, Schedlowski M, Hadamitzky M (July 2017). "Applications and limitations of behaviourally conditioned immunopharmacological responses". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 142: 91–98. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.012.
  13. ^ Lückemann L, Unteroberdörster M, Kirchog J, Schedlowski M, Hadamitzky M (July 2017). "Applications and limitations of behaviourally conditioned immunopharmacological responses". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 142: 91–98. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.012.
  14. ^ Colloca L, Benedetti F (2009). "Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning". Pain. 144 (1): 28–34. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033.
  15. ^ Colloca L, Benedetti F (2009). "Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning". Pain. 144 (1): 28–34. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033.
  16. ^ Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T (1 February 2011). "Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain". Neuroimage. 54 (3): 2492–2502. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033.
  17. ^ Rütgen M, Seidel E.-M, Riečanský IE, Lamm C (June 10 2015). "Reduction of Empathy for Pain by Placebo Analgesia Suggests Functional Equivalence of Empathy and First-Hand Emotion Experience". The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (23): 8938–8947. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3936-14.2015. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Vancouver style error: punctuation in name 2 (help); line feed character in |title= at position 51 (help)
  18. ^ Mischkowski D, Crocker J, Way B.M (2019). "A Social Analgesic? Acetaminophen(Paracetamol) Reduces Positive Empathy". Frontiers in Psychology. 10 (3). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00538. {{cite journal}}: Vancouver style error: punctuation in name 3 (help)CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)