Lilac chaser: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
Alter: template type. Add: pmc, doi-broken-date, doi-access, bibcode, page, issue, volume, year, journal, pmid, doi, pages. Removed proxy/dead URL that duplicated identifier. Removed access-date with no URL. Removed parameters. Upgrade ISBN10 to 13. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Headbomb | Linked from Wikipedia:WikiProject_Academic_Journals/Journals_cited_by_Wikipedia/Sandbox | #UCB_webform_linked 267/530
fix doi and links
Line 16: Line 16:


==Explanation==
==Explanation==
The lilac chaser illusion combines three simple, well-known effects, as described, for example, by Bertamini (2017):<ref name="Bertamini2017">{{cite journal |last1=Bertamini |first1=M. |title=Lilac chaser illusion |journal=Vision, Illusion and Perception |date=2017 |pages=153–161 |doi=10.1007/978-3-319-64066-2_142 |doi-broken-date=2022-10-30 |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-64066-2_142}}</ref>
The lilac chaser illusion combines three simple, well-known effects, as described, for example, by Bertamini (2017):<ref name="Bertamini2017">{{cite journal |last1=Bertamini |first1=M. |title=Lilac chaser illusion |journal=Vision, Illusion and Perception |date=2017 |pages=153–161 |doi=10.1007/978-3-319-64066-2 |url=https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007/978-3-319-64066-2|chapter-url=https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-319-64066-2_14}}</ref>


#The [[phi phenomenon]] is the optical illusion of perceiving continuous motion between separate objects viewed rapidly in succession. The phenomenon was defined by Max Wertheimer in the Gestalt psychology in 1912 and along with persistence of vision formed a part of the base of the theory of cinema, applied by Hugo Münsterberg in 1916. The visual events in the lilac chaser initially are the disappearances of the lilac discs. The visual events then become the appearances of green afterimages (see next).
#The [[phi phenomenon]] is the optical illusion of perceiving continuous motion between separate objects viewed rapidly in succession. The phenomenon was defined by Max Wertheimer in the Gestalt psychology in 1912 and along with persistence of vision formed a part of the base of the theory of cinema, applied by Hugo Münsterberg in 1916. The visual events in the lilac chaser initially are the disappearances of the lilac discs. The visual events then become the appearances of green afterimages (see next).

Revision as of 13:43, 31 October 2022

Stare at the center cross for at least 30 seconds to experience the phi phenomena of the illusion

The lilac chaser is a visual illusion, also known as the Pac-Man illusion.[1] It consists of 12 lilac (or pink, rose, or magenta), blurred discs arranged in a circle (like the numbers on a clock), around a small black, central cross on a grey background. One of the discs disappears briefly (for about 0.1 seconds), then the next (about 0.125 seconds later), and the next, and so on, in a clockwise direction. When one stares at the cross for at least 30 seconds, one sees three different illusions:

  1. A gap running around the circle of lilac discs;
  2. A green disc running around the circle of lilac discs in place of the gap;
  3. The green disc running around on the grey background, with the lilac discs having disappeared in sequence.

The illusion was created by Jeremy Hinton some time before 2005. It then spread widely over the internet.[2] It has been praised for being a striking visual illusion,[3] and has been the subject of academic research.[4]

The chaser effect results from the phi phenomenon illusion, combined with an afterimage effect in which an opposite color, or complementary color – green – appears when each lilac spot disappears (if the discs were blue, one would see yellow), and Troxler's fading of the lilac discs.

History

The illusion was created by Jeremy Hinton some time before 2005. He stumbled across the configuration while devising stimuli for visual motion experiments. In one version of a program to move a disc around a central point, he mistakenly neglected to erase the preceding disc, which created the appearance of a moving gap. On noticing the moving green-disc afterimage, he adjusted foreground and background colours, number of discs, and timing to optimise the effect.

In 2005 Hinton blurred the discs, allowing them to disappear when a viewer looks steadily at the central cross. Hinton entered the illusion in the European Conference on Visual Perception's Visual Illusion Contest,[5] but was disqualified for not being registered for that year's conference. Hinton approached Michael Bach, who placed an animated GIF of the illusion on his web page of illusions, naming it the "Lilac Chaser", and later presenting a configurable Java version.[6] The illusion became popular on the Internet in 2005.

Explanation

The lilac chaser illusion combines three simple, well-known effects, as described, for example, by Bertamini (2017):[7]

  1. The phi phenomenon is the optical illusion of perceiving continuous motion between separate objects viewed rapidly in succession. The phenomenon was defined by Max Wertheimer in the Gestalt psychology in 1912 and along with persistence of vision formed a part of the base of the theory of cinema, applied by Hugo Münsterberg in 1916. The visual events in the lilac chaser initially are the disappearances of the lilac discs. The visual events then become the appearances of green afterimages (see next).
  2. When a lilac stimulus that is presented to a particular region of the visual field for a long time (say 10 seconds or so) disappears, a green afterimage will appear. The afterimage lasts only a short time, and in this case is effaced by the reappearance of the lilac stimulus. The afterimage is a consequence of neural adaptation of the cells that carry signals from the retina of the eye to the rest of the brain, the retinal ganglion cells.[8] According to opponent process theory, the human visual system interprets color information by processing signals from the retinal ganglion cells in three opponent channels: red versus green, blue versus yellow, and black versus white. Responses to one color of an opponent channel are antagonistic to those of the other color. Therefore, a lilac image (a combination of red and blue) will produce a green afterimage from adaptation of the red and the blue channels, so they produce weaker signals. Anything resulting in less lilac is interpreted as a combination of the other primary colors, which are green and yellow.[9]
  3. When a blurry stimulus is presented to a region of the visual field, and we keep our eyes still, that stimulus will disappear even though it is still physically presented. This is called Troxler fading.

These effects combine to yield the remarkable sight of a green spot running around in a circle on a grey background when only stationary, flashing lilac spots have been presented.

Psychophysics

Psychophysical research has used lilac chaser's properties.[4] Hinton optimised the conditions for all three aspects of the illusion before releasing it. He also noted that the colour of the green disc could be outside the colour gamut of the monitor on which it was created (because the monitor never displays the green disc, only lilac ones). Michael Bach's version of the illusion allows viewers to adjust some aspects of the illusion. It is simple to confirm that the illusion occurs with other colours.

Other effects

It is not necessary to fixate on the black cross for the effects to occur. As long as the eyes are held steadily on any point of the figure, even the centre of one of the discs, the illusion will occur.

If instead of fixating on the black cross, one follows the moving gap with one's eyes, one will see only a moving gap and 12 lilac discs rather than a single green disc. This is because the green disc arises as an afterimage, requiring the eyes to be held steadily to occur.

If after looking at the effect for 5 minutes or so, one moves one's eyes elsewhere (e.g., to another point on the figure or to a blank sheet of white paper), one will see a stationary ring of 12 green discs that will fade after a short time. These green discs are the afterimages of the 12 lilac discs.

If one watches the illusion for long enough to see only the green disc and then moves away from the computer screen while keeping the eyes on the cross, one sees larger green spots outside a ring of lilac spots with a smaller green disc running around them. The smaller green disc may merge briefly with the outer green spots, making the spots appear to be radial blobs. The outer green spots soon fade. These outer green spots are afterimages that appear larger because of Emmert's law: the size of an afterimage becomes larger as its viewing distance is increased. They are outside because moving away from the computer screen has decreased the visual angle of the lilac spots. They fade because the lilac discs that constantly refresh the green afterimages are now projected onto a different part of the retina. If one moves towards the screen, the effects are opposite.

If one closes the right eye and moves close to the stimulus so that the nine-o'clock disc falls in the blind spot, one sees that the movement is no longer smooth. There is a noticeable pause when the disappearance of the disc occurs on the region of the retina having no rods or cones. This suggests there are limits to the filling-in that normally prevents us from noticing a black hole in our visual fields at the location of the blind spot.

See also

References

  1. ^ "The Lilac Chaser aka Pac-Man Illusion". Archived from the original on April 16, 2008.
  2. ^ Bach, Michael (2014). "Visual illusions on the internet: 15 years of change in technology and user behaviour". Perception. 43 (9): 873–880. doi:10.1068/p7708. PMID 25420328. Retrieved 16 Oct 2022.
  3. ^ DeValois, K. K., & Webster, M. A. (2011). "Color vision". Scholarpedia. 6 (4): 3073. Bibcode:2011SchpJ...6.3073D. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.3073.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. ^ a b Patricia Winkler; Kyle C. McDermott; Gideon Caplovitz; Michael Webster (2011). "Figural chasers". Journal of Vision. 11 (11): 1014. doi:10.1167/11.11.1014. Retrieved 16 Oct 2022. {{cite journal}}: Check |url= value (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  5. ^ "European Conference on Visual Perception". Retrieved 16 Oct 2022.
  6. ^ Michael Bach. "Lilac Chaser". Michaelbach.de. Retrieved 2010-11-18.
  7. ^ Bertamini, M. (2017). "Lilac chaser illusion". Vision, Illusion and Perception: 153–161. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-64066-2. {{cite journal}}: |chapter-url= ignored (help)
  8. ^ Zaidi, Q., Ennis, R., Cao, D., & Lee, B. (2012). "Neural locus of color afterimages. ". Current Biology. 22 (3): 220–224. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.021. PMC 3562597. PMID 22264612. Retrieved 17 October 2022.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  9. ^ Horner, David. T. (2013). "Demonstrations of Color Perception and the Importance of Colors". In Ware, Mark E.; Johnson, David E. (eds.). Handbook of Demonstrations and Activities in the Teaching of Psychology. Vol. II: Physiological-Comparative, Perception, Learning, Cognitive, and Developmental. Psychology Press. pp. 94–96. ISBN 978-1-134-99757-2. Retrieved 2019-12-06. Originally published as: Horner, David T. (1997). "Demonstrations of Color Perception and the Importance of Contours". Teaching of Psychology. 24 (4): 267–268. doi:10.1207/s15328023top2404_10. ISSN 0098-6283. S2CID 145364769.

External links