Jump to content

Physical attractiveness stereotype: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
not a valid doi
Ref cleanup
Line 1: Line 1:
The '''physical attractiveness stereotype''' is the tendency to assume that people who are physically attractive, based on [[Beauty standards|social beauty standards]], also possess other desirable personality traits. Research has shown that those who are physically attractive are viewed as more intelligent, competent, and socially desirable. The target benefits from what has been coined as pretty privilege, namely social, economic, and political advantages or benefits. Physical attractiveness can have a significant effect on how people are judged in terms of employment or social opportunities, friendship, sexual behavior, and marriage.
The '''physical attractiveness stereotype''' is the tendency to assume that people who are physically attractive, based on [[Beauty standards|social beauty standards]], also possess other desirable [[personality]] traits. Research has shown that those who are physically attractive are viewed as more [[Intelligence|intelligent]], competent, and socially desirable. The target benefits from what has been coined as pretty privilege, namely social, economic, and political advantages or benefits. Physical attractiveness can have a significant effect on how people are judged in terms of employment or social opportunities, [[friendship]], [[sexual behavior]], and [[marriage]].


The physical attractiveness stereotype will [[bias]] an observer's opinions and decisions when comparing people of different attractiveness levels. There is evidence of this stereotype affecting decision making within social settings, but also within the workplace and the judicial system.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal |last1=Hosoda |first1=Megumi |last2=Stone-Romero |first2=Eugene F. |last3=Coats |first3=Gwen |title=The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Job-Related Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Studies |date=June 2003 |journal=Personnel Psychology |language=en |volume=56 |issue=2 |pages=431–462 |doi=10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00157.x |issn=0031-5826}}</ref><ref name=":3">{{Cite journal |last1=Mazzella |first1=Ronald |last2=Feingold |first2=Alan |date=August 1994 |title=The Effects of Physical Attractiveness, Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Gender of Defendants and Victims on Judgments of Mock Jurors: A Meta-Analysis1 |journal=Journal of Applied Social Psychology |language=en |volume=24 |issue=15 |pages=1315–1338 |doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01552.x |issn=0021-9029}}</ref>
The physical attractiveness stereotype will [[bias]] an observer's opinions and decisions when comparing people of different attractiveness levels. There is evidence of this stereotype affecting decision making within social settings, but also within the workplace and the [[judicial system]].<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal |last1=Hosoda |first1=Megumi |last2=Stone-Romero |first2=Eugene F. |last3=Coats |first3=Gwen |date=June 2003 |title=The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Job-Related Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Studies |journal=[[Personnel Psychology (journal)|Personnel Psychology]] |language=en |volume=56 |issue=2 |pages=431–462 |doi=10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00157.x |issn=0031-5826}}</ref><ref name=":3">{{Cite journal |last1=Mazzella |first1=Ronald |last2=Feingold |first2=Alan |date=August 1994 |title=The Effects of Physical Attractiveness, Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Gender of Defendants and Victims on Judgments of Mock Jurors: A Meta-Analysis1 |journal=[[Journal of Applied Social Psychology]] |language=en |volume=24 |issue=15 |pages=1315–1338 |doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01552.x |issn=0021-9029}}</ref>


== History ==
== History ==
The physical attractiveness stereotype was first formally observed in a study done by Karen Dion, [[Ellen S. Berscheid|Ellen Berscheid]], and Elaine Walster in 1972. Study participants, all university students, were told by the experimenters that they would be tested on how well they could "read" a person after seeing a single photo of them, and that their performance would be compared to individuals that had been trained in reading a person based on body language and other interpersonal skills. The subjects were then given three envelopes that contained a photo of either a male or female near the subjects' age, who the researchers had categorized as either attractive, average, or unattractive. The goal of this study was to ascertain whether physical attractiveness affected how individuals were perceived, specifically whether they were seen as having more socially desirable personality traits and perceived to live a better life. The researchers found that overall attractive individuals were rated as having more socially desirable personality traits. Moreover, physically attractive individuals were expected to get a better job, be better spouses, have happier marriages, have better social and professional lives, and be married earlier. The only dimension that was not rated positively was parenting, where physically attractive individuals were not rated higher on the expectation of being a better parent. '''<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last1=Dion |first1=Karen |last2=Berscheid |first2=Ellen |last3=Walster |first3=Elaine |date=1972 |title=What is beautiful is good. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0033731 |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |language=en |volume=24 |issue=3 |pages=285–290 |doi=10.1037/h0033731 |pmid=4655540 |s2cid=10152052 |issn=1939-1315}}</ref>'''
The physical attractiveness stereotype was first formally observed in a study done by Karen Dion, [[Ellen S. Berscheid|Ellen Berscheid]], and Elaine Walster in 1972.'''<ref name=":0" />''' Study participants, all university students, were told by the experimenters that they would be tested on how well they could "read" a person after seeing a single photo of them, and that their performance would be compared to individuals that had been trained in reading a person based on body language and other interpersonal skills. The subjects were then given three envelopes that contained a photo of either a male or female near the subjects' age, who the researchers had categorized as either attractive, average, or unattractive. The goal of this study was to ascertain whether physical attractiveness affected how individuals were perceived, specifically whether they were seen as having more socially desirable personality traits and perceived to live a better life. The researchers found that overall attractive individuals were rated as having more socially desirable personality traits. Moreover, physically attractive individuals were expected to get a better job, be better spouses, have happier marriages, have better social and professional lives, and be married earlier. The only dimension that was not rated positively was [[parenting]], where physically attractive individuals were not rated higher on the expectation of being a better parent.'''<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last1=Dion |first1=Karen |last2=Berscheid |first2=Ellen |last3=Walster |first3=Elaine |date=1972 |title=What is beautiful is good. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0033731 |journal=[[Journal of Personality and Social Psychology]] |language=en |volume=24 |issue=3 |pages=285–290 |doi=10.1037/h0033731 |issn=1939-1315 |pmid=4655540 |s2cid=10152052}}</ref>'''


== Experimental Evidence ==
== Experimental evidence ==
In the years since the publication of the original study, further research has bolstered the physical attractiveness stereotype and expanded its influence into other areas.
In the years since the publication of the original study, further research has bolstered the physical attractiveness stereotype and expanded its influence into other areas.


=== Intelligence Ratings ===
=== Intelligence ratings ===
Physical attraction also has a strong relationship with how intelligent one is perceived. For both adults and children, attractive individuals are expected to be more intellectually competent than unattractive individuals. This effect is stronger in adults and also stronger in males. Between equally attractive males and females, the males will be perceived as more intelligent. This perception exists despite little to no evidence that attractiveness is correlated with actual competence.<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal |last1=Jackson |first1=Linda A. |last2=Hunter |first2=John E. |last3=Hodge |first3=Carole N. |date=June 1995 |title=Physical Attractiveness and Intellectual Competence: A Meta-Analytic Review |jstor=2787149 |journal=Social Psychology Quarterly |volume=58 |issue=2 |pages=108–122 |doi=10.2307/2787149 |issn=0190-2725}}</ref>
Physical attraction also has a strong relationship with how intelligent one is perceived. For both adults and children, attractive individuals are expected to be more intellectually competent than unattractive individuals. This effect is stronger in adults and also stronger in males. Between equally attractive males and females, the males will be perceived as more intelligent. This perception exists despite little to no evidence that attractiveness is correlated with actual competence.<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal |last1=Jackson |first1=Linda A. |last2=Hunter |first2=John E. |last3=Hodge |first3=Carole N. |date=June 1995 |title=Physical Attractiveness and Intellectual Competence: A Meta-Analytic Review |journal=[[Social Psychology Quarterly]] |volume=58 |issue=2 |pages=108–122 |doi=10.2307/2787149 |issn=0190-2725 |jstor=2787149}}</ref>


=== Workplace ===
=== Workplace ===
Research has shown that this stereotype exists in the workplace as well. A meta-analysis looking at how one's level of physical attractiveness can affect various job-related outcomes showed a strong relationship between attractive individuals and better job outcomes. The report accumulated over 60 study results and showed that attractive individuals were perceived as better employees. They are more likely to be hired and promoted, as well as ranked higher in performance evaluations and employment potential than unattractive counterparts. This stereotype is present for and affects both men and women as neither the gender of the attractive individual nor the gender of the observer influences the relationship.<ref name=":1"/>
Research has shown that this stereotype exists in the workplace as well. A meta-analysis looking at how one's level of physical attractiveness can affect various job-related outcomes showed a strong relationship between attractive individuals and better job outcomes. The report accumulated over 60 study results and showed that attractive individuals were perceived as better employees. They are more likely to be hired and promoted, as well as ranked higher in performance evaluations and employment potential than unattractive counterparts. This stereotype is present for and affects both men and women as neither the gender of the attractive individual nor the gender of the observer influences the relationship.<ref name=":1"/>


=== Judicial System ===
=== Judicial system ===
Based on an extensive review of [[jury research]], where a fake jury is presented with a [[mock trial]] to better understand how their decisions are made, it was found that the physical attractiveness stereotype does have an effect on jury rulings. Juries are more likely to find a physically unattractive defendant guilty than an attractive defendant. Additionally, for certain crimes, juries recommend lesser punishments for attractive individuals. In the case of robbery, rape, and cheating, the attractiveness of the defendant contributes to a lesser sentence. However, in the case of negligent homicide, attractive individuals are actually given a greater punishment than their unattractive counterparts. Researchers posit that this occurs because attractive individuals are expected to be better than unattractive individuals, they are held to a higher standard. Therefore, they are treated harsher when they make a mistake, such as in the case of negligent homicide.<ref name=":3"/>
Based on an extensive review of [[jury research]], where a fake jury is presented with a [[mock trial]] to better understand how their decisions are made, it was found that the physical attractiveness stereotype does have an effect on jury rulings. Juries are more likely to find a physically unattractive defendant guilty than an attractive defendant. Additionally, for certain crimes, juries recommend lesser punishments for attractive individuals. In the case of [[Robbery (Lime Cordiale song)|robbery]], [[rape]], and [[Infidelity|cheating]], the attractiveness of the defendant contributes to a lesser sentence. However, in the case of [[negligent homicide]], attractive individuals are actually given a greater punishment than their unattractive counterparts. Researchers posit that this occurs because attractive individuals are expected to be better than unattractive individuals, they are held to a higher standard. Therefore, they are treated harsher when they make a mistake, such as in the case of negligent homicide.<ref name=":3"/>


=== Education ===
=== Education ===
In a review of studies done that examine perception of students in the education system, it was found that attractive students were treated more favorably by their teachers than unattractive students. Consistent with the perception that attractive individuals are more [[#Intelligence Ratings|intelligent]], research shows that teachers have higher expectations for attractive students. They expect these students to be more intelligent, earn better grades, and have better social attributes than other, more unattractive students. Furthermore, various studies done between 1960-1985 show that attractive students actually earned higher scores on standardized tests. Researchers believe this is an example of a [[self-fulfilling prophecy]], where the teacher's higher expectations for the attractive students cause them to work harder and perform better.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Ritts |first1=Vicki |last2=Patterson |first2=Miles L. |last3=Tubbs |first3=Mark E. |date=Winter 1992 |title=Expectations, Impressions, and Judgments of Physically Attractive Students: A Review |doi=10.3102/00346543062004413 |journal=Review of Educational Research |volume=62 |issue=4 |pages=413–426|s2cid=145563047 }}</ref>
In a review of studies done that examine perception of students in the education system, it was found that attractive students were treated more favorably by their teachers than unattractive students. Consistent with the perception that attractive individuals are more intelligent, research shows that teachers have higher expectations for attractive students. They expect these students to be more intelligent, earn better grades, and have better social attributes than other, less attractive students. Furthermore, various studies done between 1960–1985 show that attractive students actually earned higher scores on standardized tests. Researchers believe this is an example of a [[self-fulfilling prophecy]], where the teacher's higher expectations for the attractive students cause them to work harder and perform better.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Ritts |first1=Vicki |last2=Patterson |first2=Miles L. |last3=Tubbs |first3=Mark E. |date=Winter 1992 |title=Expectations, Impressions, and Judgments of Physically Attractive Students: A Review |journal=[[Review of Educational Research]] |volume=62 |issue=4 |pages=413–426 |doi=10.3102/00346543062004413 |s2cid=145563047}}</ref>


== Theories ==
== Theories ==
There are a couple proposed theoretical underpinnings for the physical attractiveness stereotype.
There are a couple proposed theoretical underpinnings for the physical attractiveness stereotype.


=== Implicit Personality Theory ===
=== Implicit personality theory ===
The [[implicit personality theory]] refers to the unconscious assumptions one makes about another's personality based on their characteristics.<ref>{{Cite web |title=implicit personality theory |url=https://www.oxfordreference.com/display/10.1093/oi/authority.20110803095959427 |website=Oxford Reference |language=en }}</ref> These assumptions can be based on other personality traits but in the context of the physical attractiveness stereotype, they are based on physical traits. Using this theory, researchers explain the physical attractiveness stereotype in that attractive physical features are linked with positive assumptions of personality and unattractive physical features are linked with negative assumptions of personality.<ref name=":1"/> These unconscious linkages can explain why those seen as more physically attractive are treated and perceived differently.
The [[implicit personality theory]] refers to the unconscious assumptions one makes about another's personality based on their characteristics.<ref>{{Cite web |title=Implicit personality theory |url=https://www.oxfordreference.com/display/10.1093/oi/authority.20110803095959427 |access-date=2023-06-06 |website=[[Oxford Reference]] |language=en}}</ref> These assumptions can be based on other personality traits but in the context of the physical attractiveness stereotype, they are based on physical traits. Using this theory, researchers explain the physical attractiveness stereotype in that attractive physical features are linked with positive assumptions of personality and unattractive physical features are linked with negative assumptions of personality.<ref name=":1"/> These unconscious linkages can explain why those seen as more physically attractive are treated and perceived differently.


=== Evolution ===
=== Evolution ===
The principle of [[evolutionary biology]] is that, in case of [[genetic variation]] within a population in a characteristic, the form which improves the individual’s chance of survival and reproduction will be selected over other forms and becomes more frequent within the population. [[Evolutionary psychology|Evolutionary psychologists]] suggest that the physical attractiveness stereotype has evolved for individuals to assess potential mates and reproductive partners and as a mean to assess our status ranking among same-sex members.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Senior C |date=May 2003 |title=Beauty in the brain of the beholder |journal=Neuron |language=English |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=525–528 |doi=10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00293-9 |pmid=12765605 |s2cid=15749275}}</ref>
The principle of [[evolutionary biology]] is that, in case of [[genetic variation]] within a population in a characteristic, the form which improves the individual's chance of survival and reproduction will be selected over other forms and becomes more frequent within the population. [[Evolutionary psychology|Evolutionary psychologists]] suggest that the physical attractiveness stereotype has evolved for individuals to assess potential mates and reproductive partners and as a means to assess our status ranking among same-sex members.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Senior |first=Carl |date=2003-05-22 |title=Beauty in the Brain of the Beholder |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627303002939 |journal=[[Neuron (journal)|Neuron]] |language=en |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=525–528 |doi=10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00293-9}}</ref>


The [[Reproduction|reproductive strategy]] of women and men differ; however, both include advertising to potential mates and competing with same-sex members to demonstrate one’s value.<ref>{{Cite web |title=APA PsycNet |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1993-97913-002 |access-date=2023-03-24 |website=psycnet.apa.org |language=en}}</ref> Attractiveness or beauty is the display of these traits and one of the most important predictors of reproductive success. Physical attractiveness may have evolved as a signal of good health, fitness, and genetic quality. Certain physical features, including symmetry, clear skin, and waist-to-hip ratio, signal reproductive health. Individuals with these features are perceived as more attractive because they possess genes which they could pass on to the next generation.
The [[Reproduction|reproductive strategy]] of women and men differ; however, both include advertising to potential mates and competing with same-sex members to demonstrate one's value.<ref>{{Citation |last=Spencer |first=Steven J. |title=Low Self-Esteem: The Uphill Struggle for Self-Integrity |date=1993 |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4684-8956-9_2 |work=Self-Esteem |pages=21–36 |editor-last=Baumeister |editor-first=Roy F. |access-date= |place=Boston, MA |publisher=[[Springer US]] |doi=10.1007/978-1-4684-8956-9_2 |isbn=978-1-4684-8958-3 |last2=Josephs |first2=Robert A. |last3=Steele |first3=Claude M.}}</ref> Attractiveness or beauty is the display of these traits and one of the most important predictors of reproductive success. Physical attractiveness may have evolved as a signal of good health, fitness, and genetic quality. Certain physical features, including symmetry, clear skin, and [[waist-to-hip ratio]], signal reproductive health. Individuals with these features are perceived as more attractive because they possess genes which they could pass on to the next generation.


The physical attractiveness stereotype may have also evolved as a result of [[natural selection]]. Attractive individuals may have a greater chance of mating and passing on desirable traits and are therefore preferred as mates over others based on their physical attractiveness.
The physical attractiveness stereotype may have also evolved as a result of [[natural selection]]. Attractive individuals may have a greater chance of mating and passing on desirable traits and are therefore preferred as mates over others based on their physical attractiveness.


Physical attractiveness, therefore, provides the target with direct benefits where they gain directly for themselves and their offspring, and indirect benefits whereby the target gains genetic benefits to the offspring.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Thornhill |first1=Randy |url=https://global.oup.com/academic/product/the-evolutionary-biology-of-human-female-sexuality-9780195340990?cc=gb&lang=en |title=The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality |last2=Gangestad |first2=Steven W. |last3=Thornhill |first3=Randy |last4=Gangestad |first4=Steven W. |date=2008-09-25 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-534099-0 |location=Oxford, New York}}</ref>
Physical attractiveness, therefore, provides the target with direct benefits where they gain directly for themselves and their offspring, and indirect benefits whereby the target gains genetic benefits to the offspring.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Thornhill |first1=Randy |url=https://global.oup.com/academic/product/the-evolutionary-biology-of-human-female-sexuality-9780195340990?cc=gb&lang=en |title=The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality |last2=Gangestad |first2=Steven W. |last3=Thornhill |first3=Randy |last4=Gangestad |first4=Steven W. |date=2008-09-25 |publisher=[[Oxford University Press]] |isbn=978-0-19-534099-0 |location=Oxford, New York}}</ref>


=== Neuroscience ===
=== Neuroscience ===
Here only the brain regions used in assessing facial beauty will be discussed, since there is little research of how the brain processes body judgements.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallam M, Smoot M |date=May 2000 |title=Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=126 |issue=3 |pages=390–423 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390 |pmid=10825783 |s2cid=18665543}}</ref>
Here only the [[Human brain|brain]] regions used in assessing facial beauty will be discussed, since there is little research of how the brain processes body judgements.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Langlois |first=Judith H. |last2=Kalakanis |first2=Lisa |last3=Rubenstein |first3=Adam J. |last4=Larson |first4=Andrea |last5=Hallam |first5=Monica |last6=Smoot |first6=Monica |date=2000 |title=Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390 |journal=[[Psychological Bulletin]] |language=en |volume=126 |issue=3 |pages=390–423 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390 |issn=1939-1455}}</ref>


The [[brain]] uses at least three cognitive domains to decide the value of attractiveness.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Ramsey JL, Langlois JH, Hoss RA, Rubenstein AJ, Griffin AM |date=April 2004 |title=Origins of a stereotype: categorization of facial attractiveness by 6-month-old infants |journal=Developmental Science |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=201–211 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00339.x |pmid=15320380}}</ref> At first, the [[Occipital lobe|occipital]] and [[Temporal lobe|temporal]] regions of the cortex process face views.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Rodgers RF, Campagna J, Attawala R |date=December 2019 |title=Stereotypes of physical attractiveness and social influences: The heritage and vision of Dr. Thomas Cash |journal=Body Image |volume=31 |pages=273–279 |doi=10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.01.010 |pmid=30713132 |s2cid=73447614}}</ref> The information about facial features is then passed on to the [[fusiform face area]] of the [[fusiform gyrus]] (FG) for facial recognition.<ref>{{Citation |title=The evolution of feminine beauty |date=2018-08-21 |url=http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=85063209810&partnerID=8YFLogxK |work=Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences |pages=327–357 |access-date=2023-03-24 |place=Cham |publisher=Springer |isbn=978-3-319-76053-7 |vauthors=Bovet J |veditors=Kapoula Z, Volle E, Renoult J, Andreatta M}}</ref> When judging an unfamiliar face, the FG responds more strongly to attractive faces than unattractive ones, suggesting that the recognition of attractive features occurs even before the rest of the brain is included in the evaluation.<ref>{{cite journal |display-authors=6 |vauthors=Bzdok D, Langner R, Caspers S, Kurth F, Habel U, Zilles K, Laird A, Eickhoff SB |date=January 2011 |title=ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness |journal=Brain Structure & Function |volume=215 |issue=3–4 |pages=209–223 |doi=10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4 |pmc=4020344 |pmid=20978908}}</ref>
The brain uses at least three cognitive domains to decide the value of attractiveness.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ramsey |first=Jennifer L. |last2=Langlois |first2=Judith H. |last3=Hoss |first3=Rebecca A. |last4=Rubenstein |first4=Adam J. |last5=Griffin |first5=Angela M. |date=April 2004 |title=Origins of a stereotype: categorization of facial attractiveness by 6-month-old infants |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00339.x |journal=[[Developmental Science]] |language=en |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=201–211 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00339.x |issn=1363-755X}}</ref> At first, the [[Occipital lobe|occipital]] and [[Temporal lobe|temporal]] regions of the cortex process face views.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rodgers |first=Rachel F. |last2=Campagna |first2=Jenna |last3=Attawala |first3=Raihaan |date=December 2019 |title=Stereotypes of physical attractiveness and social influences: The heritage and vision of Dr. Thomas Cash |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1740144518305102 |journal=[[Body Image (journal)|Body Image]] |language=en |volume=31 |pages=273–279 |doi=10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.01.010}}</ref> The information about facial features is then passed on to the [[fusiform face area]] of the [[fusiform gyrus]] (FG) for facial recognition.<ref>{{Citation |last=Bovet |first=Jeanne |title=The Evolution of Feminine Beauty |date=2018 |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_17 |work=Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences |pages=327–357 |editor-last=Kapoula |editor-first=Zoï |access-date= |place=Cham |publisher=[[Springer International Publishing]] |language=en |doi=10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_17 |isbn=978-3-319-76053-7 |editor2-last=Volle |editor2-first=Emmanuelle |editor3-last=Renoult |editor3-first=Julien |editor4-last=Andreatta |editor4-first=Moreno}}</ref> When judging an unfamiliar face, the FG responds more strongly to attractive faces than unattractive ones, suggesting that the recognition of attractive features occurs even before the rest of the brain is included in the evaluation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Bzdok |first=D. |last2=Langner |first2=R. |last3=Caspers |first3=S. |last4=Kurth |first4=F. |last5=Habel |first5=U. |last6=Zilles |first6=K. |last7=Laird |first7=A. |last8=Eickhoff |first8=Simon B. |date=January 2011 |title=ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4 |journal=[[Brain Structure and Function]] |language=en |volume=215 |issue=3-4 |pages=209–223 |doi=10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4 |issn=1863-2653}}</ref>


The second module interprets facial movements and then interacts with other brain regions such as the [[amygdala]], [[Insular cortex|insula]], and [[limbic system]] for the emotional content of facial expressions and movements.
The second module interprets facial movements and then interacts with other brain regions such as the [[amygdala]], [[Insular cortex|insula]], and [[limbic system]] for the emotional content of facial expressions and movements.


Information is then passed on to the third module, the [[orbitofrontal cortex]] (OFC) which makes judgments of beauty and produces the neurological rewards, namely dopamine and other neurotransmitters, for finding the face's beauty.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Kirsch LP, Urgesi C, Cross ES |date=March 2016 |title=Shaping and reshaping the aesthetic brain: Emerging perspectives on the neurobiology of embodied aesthetics |journal=Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews |volume=62 |pages=56–68 |doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.12.005 |pmid=26698020 |s2cid=3401618}}</ref> The OFC is more active when viewing an attractive face versus an unattractive face.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Little AC, Jones BC, DeBruine LM |date=June 2011 |title=Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences |volume=366 |issue=1571 |pages=1638–1659 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2010.0404 |pmc=3130383 |pmid=21536551}}</ref> These areas of the bran are also associated with reward processing and regulating experiences of pleasure motivation.<ref>{{Cite web |title=APA PsycNet |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-18683-011 |access-date=2023-03-24 |website=psycnet.apa.org |language=en}}</ref> Researchers suggest that our brains find attractive faces rewarding which could be part of the reason more attractive people benefit from pretty privilege.
Information is then passed on to the third module, the [[orbitofrontal cortex]] (OFC) which makes judgments of beauty and produces the neurological rewards, namely dopamine and other neurotransmitters, for finding the face's beauty.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kirsch |first=Louise P. |last2=Urgesi |first2=Cosimo |last3=Cross |first3=Emily S. |date=March 2016 |title=Shaping and reshaping the aesthetic brain: Emerging perspectives on the neurobiology of embodied aesthetics |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0149763415301007 |journal=[[Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews]] |language=en |volume=62 |pages=56–68 |doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.12.005}}</ref> The OFC is more active when viewing an attractive face versus an unattractive face.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Little |first=Anthony C. |last2=Jones |first2=Benedict C. |last3=DeBruine |first3=Lisa M. |date=2011-06-12 |title=Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research |url=https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.2010.0404 |journal=[[Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences]] |language=en |volume=366 |issue=1571 |pages=1638–1659 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2010.0404 |issn=0962-8436 |pmc=3130383 |pmid=21536551}}</ref> These areas of the bran are also associated with reward processing and regulating experiences of pleasure motivation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Park |first=Lora E. |last2=Maner |first2=Jon K. |date=2009 |title=Does self-threat promote social connection? The role of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0013933 |journal=[[Journal of Personality and Social Psychology]] |language=en |volume=96 |issue=1 |pages=203–217 |doi=10.1037/a0013933 |issn=1939-1315}}</ref> Researchers suggest that our brains find attractive faces rewarding which could be part of the reason more attractive people benefit from pretty privilege.


==Criticism==
==Criticism==
There are recent studies that indicate that the physical attractiveness stereotype can also be a negative bias and disadvantage the target.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Agthe M, Spörrle M, Maner JK | title = Does being attractive always help? Positive and negative effects of attractiveness on social decision making | journal = Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin | volume = 37 | issue = 8 | pages = 1042–1054 | date = August 2011 | pmid = 21636731 | doi = 10.1177/0146167211410355 | s2cid = 9854865 | url = https://epub.ub.uni-muenchen.de/15549/ }}</ref> Research suggests that there might exist an exception to pretty privilege when the viewer and the target are of the same sex. In the study, targets were less likely to be recommended for a job and admission into university compared to average-looking individuals. This might stem from a desire to avoid perceived self-threats posed by attractive same-sex targets.<ref>{{Cite web |title=APA PsycNet |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2012-02198-010 |access-date=2023-03-24 |website=psycnet.apa.org |language=en}}</ref> Especially individuals who lack self-esteem are more likely to avoid these threats than those with high self-esteem.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Yarosh DB | title = Perception and Deception: Human Beauty and the Brain | journal = Behavioral Sciences | volume = 9 | issue = 4 | pages = 34 | date = March 2019 | pmid = 30934856 | pmc = 6523404 | doi = 10.3390/bs9040034 | doi-access = free }}</ref>
There are recent studies that indicate that the physical attractiveness stereotype can also be a negative bias and disadvantage the target.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Agthe |first=Maria |last2=Spörrle |first2=Matthias |last3=Maner |first3=Jon K. |date=August 2011 |title=Does Being Attractive Always Help? Positive and Negative Effects of Attractiveness on Social Decision Making |url=http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0146167211410355 |journal=[[Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin]] |language=en |volume=37 |issue=8 |pages=1042–1054 |doi=10.1177/0146167211410355 |issn=0146-1672}}</ref> Research suggests that there might exist an exception to pretty privilege when the viewer and the target are of the same sex. In the study, targets were less likely to be recommended for a job and admission into university compared to average-looking individuals. This might stem from a desire to avoid perceived self-threats posed by attractive same-sex targets.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Wiese |first=Holger |last2=Kloth |first2=Nadine |last3=Güllmar |first3=Daniel |last4=Reichenbach |first4=Jürgen R. |last5=Schweinberger |first5=Stefan R. |date=2012 |title=Perceiving age and gender in unfamiliar faces: An fMRI study on face categorization |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0278262611002016 |journal=[[Brain and Cognition]] |language=en |volume=78 |issue=2 |pages=163–168 |doi=10.1016/j.bandc.2011.10.012}}</ref> Especially individuals who lack [[self-esteem]] are more likely to avoid these threats than those with high self-esteem.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Yarosh |first=Daniel |date=2019-03-29 |title=Perception and Deception: Human Beauty and the Brain |url=https://www.mdpi.com/2076-328X/9/4/34 |journal=Behavioral Sciences |language=en |publisher=[[MDPI]] |volume=9 |issue=4 |pages=34 |doi=10.3390/bs9040034 |issn=2076-328X |pmc=6523404 |pmid=30934856}}</ref>


== See also ==
== See also ==

Revision as of 21:07, 6 June 2023

The physical attractiveness stereotype is the tendency to assume that people who are physically attractive, based on social beauty standards, also possess other desirable personality traits. Research has shown that those who are physically attractive are viewed as more intelligent, competent, and socially desirable. The target benefits from what has been coined as pretty privilege, namely social, economic, and political advantages or benefits. Physical attractiveness can have a significant effect on how people are judged in terms of employment or social opportunities, friendship, sexual behavior, and marriage.

The physical attractiveness stereotype will bias an observer's opinions and decisions when comparing people of different attractiveness levels. There is evidence of this stereotype affecting decision making within social settings, but also within the workplace and the judicial system.[1][2]

History

The physical attractiveness stereotype was first formally observed in a study done by Karen Dion, Ellen Berscheid, and Elaine Walster in 1972.[3] Study participants, all university students, were told by the experimenters that they would be tested on how well they could "read" a person after seeing a single photo of them, and that their performance would be compared to individuals that had been trained in reading a person based on body language and other interpersonal skills. The subjects were then given three envelopes that contained a photo of either a male or female near the subjects' age, who the researchers had categorized as either attractive, average, or unattractive. The goal of this study was to ascertain whether physical attractiveness affected how individuals were perceived, specifically whether they were seen as having more socially desirable personality traits and perceived to live a better life. The researchers found that overall attractive individuals were rated as having more socially desirable personality traits. Moreover, physically attractive individuals were expected to get a better job, be better spouses, have happier marriages, have better social and professional lives, and be married earlier. The only dimension that was not rated positively was parenting, where physically attractive individuals were not rated higher on the expectation of being a better parent.[3]

Experimental evidence

In the years since the publication of the original study, further research has bolstered the physical attractiveness stereotype and expanded its influence into other areas.

Intelligence ratings

Physical attraction also has a strong relationship with how intelligent one is perceived. For both adults and children, attractive individuals are expected to be more intellectually competent than unattractive individuals. This effect is stronger in adults and also stronger in males. Between equally attractive males and females, the males will be perceived as more intelligent. This perception exists despite little to no evidence that attractiveness is correlated with actual competence.[4]

Workplace

Research has shown that this stereotype exists in the workplace as well. A meta-analysis looking at how one's level of physical attractiveness can affect various job-related outcomes showed a strong relationship between attractive individuals and better job outcomes. The report accumulated over 60 study results and showed that attractive individuals were perceived as better employees. They are more likely to be hired and promoted, as well as ranked higher in performance evaluations and employment potential than unattractive counterparts. This stereotype is present for and affects both men and women as neither the gender of the attractive individual nor the gender of the observer influences the relationship.[1]

Judicial system

Based on an extensive review of jury research, where a fake jury is presented with a mock trial to better understand how their decisions are made, it was found that the physical attractiveness stereotype does have an effect on jury rulings. Juries are more likely to find a physically unattractive defendant guilty than an attractive defendant. Additionally, for certain crimes, juries recommend lesser punishments for attractive individuals. In the case of robbery, rape, and cheating, the attractiveness of the defendant contributes to a lesser sentence. However, in the case of negligent homicide, attractive individuals are actually given a greater punishment than their unattractive counterparts. Researchers posit that this occurs because attractive individuals are expected to be better than unattractive individuals, they are held to a higher standard. Therefore, they are treated harsher when they make a mistake, such as in the case of negligent homicide.[2]

Education

In a review of studies done that examine perception of students in the education system, it was found that attractive students were treated more favorably by their teachers than unattractive students. Consistent with the perception that attractive individuals are more intelligent, research shows that teachers have higher expectations for attractive students. They expect these students to be more intelligent, earn better grades, and have better social attributes than other, less attractive students. Furthermore, various studies done between 1960–1985 show that attractive students actually earned higher scores on standardized tests. Researchers believe this is an example of a self-fulfilling prophecy, where the teacher's higher expectations for the attractive students cause them to work harder and perform better.[5]

Theories

There are a couple proposed theoretical underpinnings for the physical attractiveness stereotype.

Implicit personality theory

The implicit personality theory refers to the unconscious assumptions one makes about another's personality based on their characteristics.[6] These assumptions can be based on other personality traits but in the context of the physical attractiveness stereotype, they are based on physical traits. Using this theory, researchers explain the physical attractiveness stereotype in that attractive physical features are linked with positive assumptions of personality and unattractive physical features are linked with negative assumptions of personality.[1] These unconscious linkages can explain why those seen as more physically attractive are treated and perceived differently.

Evolution

The principle of evolutionary biology is that, in case of genetic variation within a population in a characteristic, the form which improves the individual's chance of survival and reproduction will be selected over other forms and becomes more frequent within the population. Evolutionary psychologists suggest that the physical attractiveness stereotype has evolved for individuals to assess potential mates and reproductive partners and as a means to assess our status ranking among same-sex members.[7]

The reproductive strategy of women and men differ; however, both include advertising to potential mates and competing with same-sex members to demonstrate one's value.[8] Attractiveness or beauty is the display of these traits and one of the most important predictors of reproductive success. Physical attractiveness may have evolved as a signal of good health, fitness, and genetic quality. Certain physical features, including symmetry, clear skin, and waist-to-hip ratio, signal reproductive health. Individuals with these features are perceived as more attractive because they possess genes which they could pass on to the next generation.

The physical attractiveness stereotype may have also evolved as a result of natural selection. Attractive individuals may have a greater chance of mating and passing on desirable traits and are therefore preferred as mates over others based on their physical attractiveness.

Physical attractiveness, therefore, provides the target with direct benefits where they gain directly for themselves and their offspring, and indirect benefits whereby the target gains genetic benefits to the offspring.[9]

Neuroscience

Here only the brain regions used in assessing facial beauty will be discussed, since there is little research of how the brain processes body judgements.[10]

The brain uses at least three cognitive domains to decide the value of attractiveness.[11] At first, the occipital and temporal regions of the cortex process face views.[12] The information about facial features is then passed on to the fusiform face area of the fusiform gyrus (FG) for facial recognition.[13] When judging an unfamiliar face, the FG responds more strongly to attractive faces than unattractive ones, suggesting that the recognition of attractive features occurs even before the rest of the brain is included in the evaluation.[14]

The second module interprets facial movements and then interacts with other brain regions such as the amygdala, insula, and limbic system for the emotional content of facial expressions and movements.

Information is then passed on to the third module, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) which makes judgments of beauty and produces the neurological rewards, namely dopamine and other neurotransmitters, for finding the face's beauty.[15] The OFC is more active when viewing an attractive face versus an unattractive face.[16] These areas of the bran are also associated with reward processing and regulating experiences of pleasure motivation.[17] Researchers suggest that our brains find attractive faces rewarding which could be part of the reason more attractive people benefit from pretty privilege.

Criticism

There are recent studies that indicate that the physical attractiveness stereotype can also be a negative bias and disadvantage the target.[18] Research suggests that there might exist an exception to pretty privilege when the viewer and the target are of the same sex. In the study, targets were less likely to be recommended for a job and admission into university compared to average-looking individuals. This might stem from a desire to avoid perceived self-threats posed by attractive same-sex targets.[19] Especially individuals who lack self-esteem are more likely to avoid these threats than those with high self-esteem.[20]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c Hosoda, Megumi; Stone-Romero, Eugene F.; Coats, Gwen (June 2003). "The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Job-Related Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Studies". Personnel Psychology. 56 (2): 431–462. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00157.x. ISSN 0031-5826.
  2. ^ a b Mazzella, Ronald; Feingold, Alan (August 1994). "The Effects of Physical Attractiveness, Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Gender of Defendants and Victims on Judgments of Mock Jurors: A Meta-Analysis1". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 24 (15): 1315–1338. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01552.x. ISSN 0021-9029.
  3. ^ a b Dion, Karen; Berscheid, Ellen; Walster, Elaine (1972). "What is beautiful is good". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 24 (3): 285–290. doi:10.1037/h0033731. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 4655540. S2CID 10152052.
  4. ^ Jackson, Linda A.; Hunter, John E.; Hodge, Carole N. (June 1995). "Physical Attractiveness and Intellectual Competence: A Meta-Analytic Review". Social Psychology Quarterly. 58 (2): 108–122. doi:10.2307/2787149. ISSN 0190-2725. JSTOR 2787149.
  5. ^ Ritts, Vicki; Patterson, Miles L.; Tubbs, Mark E. (Winter 1992). "Expectations, Impressions, and Judgments of Physically Attractive Students: A Review". Review of Educational Research. 62 (4): 413–426. doi:10.3102/00346543062004413. S2CID 145563047.
  6. ^ "Implicit personality theory". Oxford Reference. Retrieved 2023-06-06.
  7. ^ Senior, Carl (2003-05-22). "Beauty in the Brain of the Beholder". Neuron. 38 (4): 525–528. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00293-9.
  8. ^ Spencer, Steven J.; Josephs, Robert A.; Steele, Claude M. (1993), Baumeister, Roy F. (ed.), "Low Self-Esteem: The Uphill Struggle for Self-Integrity", Self-Esteem, Boston, MA: Springer US, pp. 21–36, doi:10.1007/978-1-4684-8956-9_2, ISBN 978-1-4684-8958-3
  9. ^ Thornhill, Randy; Gangestad, Steven W.; Thornhill, Randy; Gangestad, Steven W. (2008-09-25). The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-534099-0.
  10. ^ Langlois, Judith H.; Kalakanis, Lisa; Rubenstein, Adam J.; Larson, Andrea; Hallam, Monica; Smoot, Monica (2000). "Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (3): 390–423. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390. ISSN 1939-1455.
  11. ^ Ramsey, Jennifer L.; Langlois, Judith H.; Hoss, Rebecca A.; Rubenstein, Adam J.; Griffin, Angela M. (April 2004). "Origins of a stereotype: categorization of facial attractiveness by 6-month-old infants". Developmental Science. 7 (2): 201–211. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00339.x. ISSN 1363-755X.
  12. ^ Rodgers, Rachel F.; Campagna, Jenna; Attawala, Raihaan (December 2019). "Stereotypes of physical attractiveness and social influences: The heritage and vision of Dr. Thomas Cash". Body Image. 31: 273–279. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.01.010.
  13. ^ Bovet, Jeanne (2018), Kapoula, Zoï; Volle, Emmanuelle; Renoult, Julien; Andreatta, Moreno (eds.), "The Evolution of Feminine Beauty", Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences, Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 327–357, doi:10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_17, ISBN 978-3-319-76053-7
  14. ^ Bzdok, D.; Langner, R.; Caspers, S.; Kurth, F.; Habel, U.; Zilles, K.; Laird, A.; Eickhoff, Simon B. (January 2011). "ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness". Brain Structure and Function. 215 (3–4): 209–223. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4. ISSN 1863-2653.
  15. ^ Kirsch, Louise P.; Urgesi, Cosimo; Cross, Emily S. (March 2016). "Shaping and reshaping the aesthetic brain: Emerging perspectives on the neurobiology of embodied aesthetics". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 62: 56–68. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.12.005.
  16. ^ Little, Anthony C.; Jones, Benedict C.; DeBruine, Lisa M. (2011-06-12). "Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 366 (1571): 1638–1659. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0404. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 3130383. PMID 21536551.
  17. ^ Park, Lora E.; Maner, Jon K. (2009). "Does self-threat promote social connection? The role of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (1): 203–217. doi:10.1037/a0013933. ISSN 1939-1315.
  18. ^ Agthe, Maria; Spörrle, Matthias; Maner, Jon K. (August 2011). "Does Being Attractive Always Help? Positive and Negative Effects of Attractiveness on Social Decision Making". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (8): 1042–1054. doi:10.1177/0146167211410355. ISSN 0146-1672.
  19. ^ Wiese, Holger; Kloth, Nadine; Güllmar, Daniel; Reichenbach, Jürgen R.; Schweinberger, Stefan R. (2012). "Perceiving age and gender in unfamiliar faces: An fMRI study on face categorization". Brain and Cognition. 78 (2): 163–168. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.10.012.
  20. ^ Yarosh, Daniel (2019-03-29). "Perception and Deception: Human Beauty and the Brain". Behavioral Sciences. 9 (4). MDPI: 34. doi:10.3390/bs9040034. ISSN 2076-328X. PMC 6523404. PMID 30934856.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)