Jump to content

Shared intentionality: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Grammatical errors
fmt
Line 6: Line 6:
The concept is slightly close to [[Collective intentionality|Collective Intentionality]]. The philosophical notion of Collective intentionality defines the capability of collectives to form co-intentions when individuals become jointly directed at objects, matters of fact, states of affairs, goals, or values. This co-intention occurs when two or more individuals undertake an aware task together. The attribute of the Collective Intentionality is defined in the object’s awareness of a common intention, in the conscious power of minds to be jointly directed at a goal. It is thought that Collective Intentionality only implies aware intentions–the causal antecedents of action. Therefore, only three or four years old, after years of continuous interaction with other persons, children can develop an ability for Collective Intentionality, which acts as the comprehension of cultural institutions based on collective beliefs and practices.<ref>Tomasello, M.; Rakoczy, H. "What Makes Human Cognition Unique? From Individual to Shared to Collective Intentionality". ''Mind & Language'', Vol. 18, No. 2, April 2003, pp. 121–147.</ref> In contrast, the psychological construct of Shared Intentionality describes unaware processes during social learning at the onset of life, when organisms in the simple reflexes substage of the sensorimotor stage of cognitive development do not possess abstract thinking.<ref>Tomasello, M. ''The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition''. Cambridge, Massachusetts: [[Harvard University Press]]. 1999.</ref> This difference between the two concepts implies the possibility of two different neurophysiological processes underlying their appearance.
The concept is slightly close to [[Collective intentionality|Collective Intentionality]]. The philosophical notion of Collective intentionality defines the capability of collectives to form co-intentions when individuals become jointly directed at objects, matters of fact, states of affairs, goals, or values. This co-intention occurs when two or more individuals undertake an aware task together. The attribute of the Collective Intentionality is defined in the object’s awareness of a common intention, in the conscious power of minds to be jointly directed at a goal. It is thought that Collective Intentionality only implies aware intentions–the causal antecedents of action. Therefore, only three or four years old, after years of continuous interaction with other persons, children can develop an ability for Collective Intentionality, which acts as the comprehension of cultural institutions based on collective beliefs and practices.<ref>Tomasello, M.; Rakoczy, H. "What Makes Human Cognition Unique? From Individual to Shared to Collective Intentionality". ''Mind & Language'', Vol. 18, No. 2, April 2003, pp. 121–147.</ref> In contrast, the psychological construct of Shared Intentionality describes unaware processes during social learning at the onset of life, when organisms in the simple reflexes substage of the sensorimotor stage of cognitive development do not possess abstract thinking.<ref>Tomasello, M. ''The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition''. Cambridge, Massachusetts: [[Harvard University Press]]. 1999.</ref> This difference between the two concepts implies the possibility of two different neurophysiological processes underlying their appearance.


In recent years, the [[Psychological construct]] of Shared Intentionality is being explored from different perspectives by studying: e.g., the cognitive processes involved in creating and sustaining cooperative group activity,<ref>Skau, S. "Analysis of the cognitive processes involved in creating and sustaining cooperative group activity". ''Frontiers in Psychology''. 2022 Dec 7; 13:1038309. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1038309</ref> collaborative neuronal activity in inter-brain neuroscience studies,<ref>Fishburn, F. A.; Murty, V. P.; Hlutkowsky, C. O.; MacGillivray, C. E.; Bemis, L. M.; Murphy, M. E.; Huppert, T. J.; Perlman, S. B. "Putting our heads together: Interpersonal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism for shared intentionality". ''Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience''. 2018 Aug; 13(8):841-9.</ref><ref>Astolfi; L.; et al.: "Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans". Brain Topography, 2010; 23:243–256.</ref><ref>Szymanski, C.; et al. "Teams on the same wavelength perform better: Inter-brain phase synchronization constitutes a neural substrate for social facilitation". ''Neuroimage'', 2017; 15:425–436.</ref><ref>Hu, Y.; et al. "Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making". ''Biological Psychology'', 2018; 133: 54–62, 2018.</ref><ref>Painter, D. R.; Kim, J. J.; Renton, A. I.; Mattingley, J. B. "Joint control of visually guided actions involves concordant increases in behavioural and neural coupling". ''Communications Biology''. 2021; 4(1):1-5. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-02319-3.</ref><ref>Liu, J.; Zhang, R.; Xie, E.; et al. "Shared intentionality modulates interpersonal neural synchronization at the establishment of communication system". ''Communications Biology'', 6, 832 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-023-05197-z</ref> and group performance in psychophysiological studies.<ref>Atmaca, S.; Sebanz, N.; Prinz, W.; Knoblich, G. "Action co-representation: The joint SNARC effect". ''Social Neuroscience'' 2008; 3:3-4, 410-420.</ref><ref>Shteynberg, G.; Galinsky, A. D. "Implicit coordination: Sharing goals with similar others intensifies goal pursuit". ''Journal of Experimental Social Psychology'', 2011; 47: 1291–1294, ISSN 0022-1031.</ref><ref>McClung, J. S.; Placì, S.; Bangerter, A.; Clément, F.; Bshary, R. "The language of cooperation: Shared intentionality drives variation in helping as a function of group membership". ''Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences''; 2017; 284:20171682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1682.</ref><ref>Val Danilov, I; Mihailova, S.; Perepjolkina, V. "Unconscious social interaction, coherent intelligence in learning". ''Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of Education, Research and Innovation''; ICERI; Seville (Spain), 2019, pp. 2217–2222. DOI: 10.21125/iceri.2019.0606, https://library.iated.org/view/DANILOV2019UNC</ref><ref>Tang, N.; Gong, S.; Zhao, M.; Gu, C.; Zhou, J.; Shen, M.; Gao, T. "Exploring an imagined 'we' in human collective hunting: Joint commitment within shared intentionality". ''Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society'', 2022; 44(44). https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3wj722pb.</ref> However, the nature of the interaction in Shared Intentionality is unclear, since it occurs even in infants, organisms at the simple reflexes stage of development.<ref>Val Danilov, I; Mihailova, S. "A new perspective on assessing cognition in children through estimating shared intentionality". ''Journal of Intelligence'', 2022: 10: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020021</ref>
In recent years, the [[Psychological construct]] of Shared Intentionality is being explored from different perspectives by studying: e.g., the cognitive processes involved in creating and sustaining cooperative group activity,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Skau |first=Simon |date=2022 |title=Analysis of the cognitive processes involved in creating and sustaining cooperative group activity |url=https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1038309 |journal=Frontiers in Psychology |volume=13 |doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1038309 |issn=1664-1078 |pmc=PMC9768542 |pmid=36571038}}</ref> collaborative neuronal activity in inter-brain neuroscience studies,<ref>Fishburn, F. A.; Murty, V. P.; Hlutkowsky, C. O.; MacGillivray, C. E.; Bemis, L. M.; Murphy, M. E.; Huppert, T. J.; Perlman, S. B. "Putting our heads together: Interpersonal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism for shared intentionality". ''Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience''. 2018 Aug; 13(8):841-9.</ref><ref>Astolfi; L.; et al.: "Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans". Brain Topography, 2010; 23:243–256.</ref><ref>Szymanski, C.; et al. "Teams on the same wavelength perform better: Inter-brain phase synchronization constitutes a neural substrate for social facilitation". ''Neuroimage'', 2017; 15:425–436.</ref><ref>Hu, Y.; et al. "Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making". ''Biological Psychology'', 2018; 133: 54–62, 2018.</ref><ref>Painter, D. R.; Kim, J. J.; Renton, A. I.; Mattingley, J. B. "Joint control of visually guided actions involves concordant increases in behavioural and neural coupling". ''Communications Biology''. 2021; 4(1):1-5. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-02319-3.</ref><ref>Liu, J.; Zhang, R.; Xie, E.; et al. "Shared intentionality modulates interpersonal neural synchronization at the establishment of communication system". ''Communications Biology'', 6, 832 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-023-05197-z</ref> and group performance in psychophysiological studies.<ref>Atmaca, S.; Sebanz, N.; Prinz, W.; Knoblich, G. "Action co-representation: The joint SNARC effect". ''Social Neuroscience'' 2008; 3:3-4, 410-420.</ref><ref>Shteynberg, G.; Galinsky, A. D. "Implicit coordination: Sharing goals with similar others intensifies goal pursuit". ''Journal of Experimental Social Psychology'', 2011; 47: 1291–1294, ISSN 0022-1031.</ref><ref>McClung, J. S.; Placì, S.; Bangerter, A.; Clément, F.; Bshary, R. "The language of cooperation: Shared intentionality drives variation in helping as a function of group membership". ''Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences''; 2017; 284:20171682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1682.</ref><ref>Val Danilov, I; Mihailova, S.; Perepjolkina, V. "Unconscious social interaction, coherent intelligence in learning". ''Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of Education, Research and Innovation''; ICERI; Seville (Spain), 2019, pp. 2217–2222. DOI: 10.21125/iceri.2019.0606, https://library.iated.org/view/DANILOV2019UNC</ref><ref>Tang, N.; Gong, S.; Zhao, M.; Gu, C.; Zhou, J.; Shen, M.; Gao, T. "Exploring an imagined 'we' in human collective hunting: Joint commitment within shared intentionality". ''Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society'', 2022; 44(44). https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3wj722pb.</ref> However, the nature of the interaction in Shared Intentionality is unclear, since it occurs even in infants, organisms at the simple reflexes stage of development.<ref>Val Danilov, I; Mihailova, S. "A new perspective on assessing cognition in children through estimating shared intentionality". ''Journal of Intelligence'', 2022: 10: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020021</ref>


== Definitions ==
== Definitions ==

Revision as of 08:56, 16 November 2023

Shared intentionality is a concept in psychology that describes the human capacity to engage with the psychological states of others. According to conventional wisdom in cognitive sciences, shared intentionality supports the development of everything from cooperative interactions and knowledge assimilation to moral identity and cultural evolution that provides building societies, being a pre-requisite of social reality formation.[1] Knowledge about shared intentionality has been developing since the last century's end. This psychological construct was introduced in the 1980s with a straightforward definition of sharing psychological states among participants without attributing to age when it begins.[2][3][4] The development of knowledge on mother-child interactions has made additional attributes about appearing shared intentionality; it enables one-year-olds to cultural learning.[5] Later, Tomasello et al. specified that, even at birth, infants grasp shared intentionality with caregivers – this ability to share psychological states with others emerges immediately after birth.[6] Tomasello hypothesized gradually increasing social bonds between children and caregivers through the essential motive force of shared intentionality beginning from emotion sharing from birth.[7] In 2022, Michael Tomasello received the prestigious David Rumelhart Prize 2022 in the Cognitive Science Society as an award for his insights into cognition evolution and, specifically, the knowledge development about a contribution of shared intentionality to cognition and social reality formation.[1]

The concept is slightly close to Collective Intentionality. The philosophical notion of Collective intentionality defines the capability of collectives to form co-intentions when individuals become jointly directed at objects, matters of fact, states of affairs, goals, or values. This co-intention occurs when two or more individuals undertake an aware task together. The attribute of the Collective Intentionality is defined in the object’s awareness of a common intention, in the conscious power of minds to be jointly directed at a goal. It is thought that Collective Intentionality only implies aware intentions–the causal antecedents of action. Therefore, only three or four years old, after years of continuous interaction with other persons, children can develop an ability for Collective Intentionality, which acts as the comprehension of cultural institutions based on collective beliefs and practices.[8] In contrast, the psychological construct of Shared Intentionality describes unaware processes during social learning at the onset of life, when organisms in the simple reflexes substage of the sensorimotor stage of cognitive development do not possess abstract thinking.[9] This difference between the two concepts implies the possibility of two different neurophysiological processes underlying their appearance.

In recent years, the Psychological construct of Shared Intentionality is being explored from different perspectives by studying: e.g., the cognitive processes involved in creating and sustaining cooperative group activity,[10] collaborative neuronal activity in inter-brain neuroscience studies,[11][12][13][14][15][16] and group performance in psychophysiological studies.[17][18][19][20][21] However, the nature of the interaction in Shared Intentionality is unclear, since it occurs even in infants, organisms at the simple reflexes stage of development.[22]

Definitions

In classical thought, a definition (see more in the wiki article) is a statement of a thing's essence. Shared Intentionality is a latent variable that can only be inferred indirectly. The definition of this Psychological construct should be carefully considered because the translational impact of research relies on how we define this implicit phenomenon. The definition of Shared Intentionality should constitute further research directions, e.g., for understanding the neurophysiological processes underlying cooperative targeting in individuals. The study on Shared Intentionality began by understanding the phenomenon as the ability to enable coordinated, collaborative interactions made possible by a motivation to share mental states. However, because this mutual comprehension of a target succeeds in even newborns, the essence of the phenomenon is unclear. The definition should explain meanings of such a fundamental for this construct constituent as the mode of how this sharing of mental states may occur, considering the age of the organism exhibiting this ability. The initial definition left unclear how infants are able to perceive Shared Intentionality while they cannot provide communication through sensory cues at the beginning of social learning. Even the essence of a mental state at this developmental stage needs clarification.

Based on recent insights in neuroscience research, a hypothesis of neurophysiological grounds of Shared Intentionality specified that this collaborative interaction emerges in the mother-child pairs at birth for sharing the essential sensory stimulus of the actual cognitive problem.[23][24][25] This social bond enables ecological training of immature organisms, starting at the simple reflexes substage of the sensorimotor stage of cognitive development.[26]

Contributions

Shared Intentionality is a driver of environmental learning of organisms at the onset of cognition. The significance of this notion is that it defines pre-perceptual communication, which contributes to the assimilation of initial environmental knowledge in the period when organisms cannot communicate through sensory cues. In this stage, organisms are not even able to target; they can show goal-directed behavior in primitive reflexes only. Even a perception of objects cannot appear on its own independently in this developmental stage. The organisms should gain a holistic representation of the environment before beginning perception for then successfully processing the organization, identification, and interpretation of sensory information.[27] This explanation of Shared Intentionality significance also yields an analytical reasoning of why it is defined as an unaware interaction in which the immature recipient organism experiences the ability to select the only stimulus that the mature contributor organism is targeting.[28]

An explanation of neurophysiological processes during this initial interaction is intriguing as much as essential since it reveals perspectives for understanding perception and consciousness and even promotes advances in many fields of knowledge, from biomedicine to artificial intelligence. In medicine, evaluation of shared intentionality magnitude in mother-child dyad can contribute to assessing children's cognitive development.[29][30] Progress in knowledge of underlying processes of shared intentionality can provide management of artificial neural networks of intelligent prosthetic limbs via a bond with the human sensorimotor network.[31] The feasibility of integrating the human brain with a computer shows the way to a new stage in artificial intelligence design.[32]

The neurophysiological hypothesis

Nowadays, only one hypothesis tends to explain neurophysiological processes during Shared Intentionality in integrative complexity from cellular to interpersonal dynamics levels. According to Val Danilov [33], Shared Intentionality emerges in the mother (contributor) and child (recipient) dyad under specific conditions that match the mother-fetus communication model. This model of interaction between closely related organisms is described by the following attributes: (i) social learning in a lack of meaningful sensory interaction between them; (ii) unintelligible stimuli (for the recipient) in a shared ecological context; (iii) a single low-frequency harmonic oscillator.[34]

In the beginning, interpersonal dynamics in these relative organisms maintain the inherited mechanism of social entrainment of the recipient to the contributor's socio-biological rhythm, synchronizing physiological processes in these organisms. Therefore at the cellular level, a coordination of gamma neuronal activity in each organism occurs in similar separate networks of different subsystems that are relevant to the interpersonal dynamics of these organisms in the specific ecological context.[35]

The increased heartbeats of the contributor (low-frequency oscillator) coordinate central and peripheral gamma temporal coordination in each organism by nesting gamma oscillations of local networks (interference of delta and gamma waves). The similarly modulated local gamma-temporal coordination in different brain zones of the different nervous systems provides a coordinated neuronal activity that can provide integrated neuronal processing.[36] So, because of the interpersonal dynamics, shared ecological context, and the low-frequency oscillator, cells and even their networks in different nervous systems behave coordinately (nonlocal neuronal coupling), and the integrated neuronal processing in all organisms is similar.[37] In these conditions, each intentional act of the contributor becomes a template for the recipient's nervous system–the "instructions" about synaptic structural organisation corresponding to a specific sensory stimulus.[38]

In short, the mother's heartbeats can synchronize brain gamma waves of already excited central and peripheral neuronal ensembles, similar in both organisms due to physiological entrainment being in the shared ecosystem, and, due to this physiological harmony, specific sensorimotor networks activation in the mother entrain those in the fetus; and because of the shared ecosystem, this engagement trains the young nervous system to respond correctly to certain sensory stimuli through statistical mechanisms based on numerous successful trials and errors.[39][40] In such a manner, an intentional act of the contributor simultaneously becomes an appearance of subliminal perception in the recipient. Therefore, nonlocal neuronal coupling provides subliminal perception in the recipient, similar to the intentional act of the contributor. Shared Intentionality gives the recipient a direct clue for the relevant stimulus, providing pre-perceptual communication.[41]

References

  1. ^ a b "David E. Rumelhart Prize". CognitiveScienceSociety.org. Cognitive Science Society. 2022. "2022 Recipient - Michael Tomasello" section. Retrieved 10 November 2023.
  2. ^ Gilbert, M. On Social Facts. London: Routledge; 1989
  3. ^ Searle, J. R. The Rediscovery of the Mind. London: MIT Press; 1992.
  4. ^ Tuomela, Raimo. The Importance of Us: A Philosophical Study of Basic Social Notions. Stanford University Press; 1995.
  5. ^ Tomasello, M.; Rakoczy, H. "What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality". Mind & Language, 2003; 18: 121-147.
  6. ^ Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Behne, T.; Moll, H. "Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005; 28: 675-691.
  7. ^ Tomasello, M. Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press; 2019.
  8. ^ Tomasello, M.; Rakoczy, H. "What Makes Human Cognition Unique? From Individual to Shared to Collective Intentionality". Mind & Language, Vol. 18, No. 2, April 2003, pp. 121–147.
  9. ^ Tomasello, M. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. 1999.
  10. ^ Skau, Simon (2022). "Analysis of the cognitive processes involved in creating and sustaining cooperative group activity". Frontiers in Psychology. 13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1038309. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 9768542. PMID 36571038.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  11. ^ Fishburn, F. A.; Murty, V. P.; Hlutkowsky, C. O.; MacGillivray, C. E.; Bemis, L. M.; Murphy, M. E.; Huppert, T. J.; Perlman, S. B. "Putting our heads together: Interpersonal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism for shared intentionality". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2018 Aug; 13(8):841-9.
  12. ^ Astolfi; L.; et al.: "Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans". Brain Topography, 2010; 23:243–256.
  13. ^ Szymanski, C.; et al. "Teams on the same wavelength perform better: Inter-brain phase synchronization constitutes a neural substrate for social facilitation". Neuroimage, 2017; 15:425–436.
  14. ^ Hu, Y.; et al. "Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making". Biological Psychology, 2018; 133: 54–62, 2018.
  15. ^ Painter, D. R.; Kim, J. J.; Renton, A. I.; Mattingley, J. B. "Joint control of visually guided actions involves concordant increases in behavioural and neural coupling". Communications Biology. 2021; 4(1):1-5. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-02319-3.
  16. ^ Liu, J.; Zhang, R.; Xie, E.; et al. "Shared intentionality modulates interpersonal neural synchronization at the establishment of communication system". Communications Biology, 6, 832 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-023-05197-z
  17. ^ Atmaca, S.; Sebanz, N.; Prinz, W.; Knoblich, G. "Action co-representation: The joint SNARC effect". Social Neuroscience 2008; 3:3-4, 410-420.
  18. ^ Shteynberg, G.; Galinsky, A. D. "Implicit coordination: Sharing goals with similar others intensifies goal pursuit". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2011; 47: 1291–1294, ISSN 0022-1031.
  19. ^ McClung, J. S.; Placì, S.; Bangerter, A.; Clément, F.; Bshary, R. "The language of cooperation: Shared intentionality drives variation in helping as a function of group membership". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences; 2017; 284:20171682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1682.
  20. ^ Val Danilov, I; Mihailova, S.; Perepjolkina, V. "Unconscious social interaction, coherent intelligence in learning". Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of Education, Research and Innovation; ICERI; Seville (Spain), 2019, pp. 2217–2222. DOI: 10.21125/iceri.2019.0606, https://library.iated.org/view/DANILOV2019UNC
  21. ^ Tang, N.; Gong, S.; Zhao, M.; Gu, C.; Zhou, J.; Shen, M.; Gao, T. "Exploring an imagined 'we' in human collective hunting: Joint commitment within shared intentionality". Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2022; 44(44). https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3wj722pb.
  22. ^ Val Danilov, I; Mihailova, S. "A new perspective on assessing cognition in children through estimating shared intentionality". Journal of Intelligence, 2022: 10: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020021
  23. ^ Val Danilov, I; Mihailova S. "Neuronal coherence agent for shared intentionality: A hypothesis of neurobiological processes occurring during social interaction". OBM Neurobiology 2021; 5(4): 26; DOI: 10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2104113
  24. ^ Val Danilov, Igor (2023). "Theoretical Grounds of Shared Intentionality for Neuroscience in Developing Bioengineering Systems". OBM Neurobiology. 7 (1): 156. doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2301156.
  25. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Advances in computer-aided diagnosis of developmental delay in children using bioengineering systems: A new math model and algorithm". In: Rajakumar, G.; Du, K. L.; Rocha, Á. (eds) Intelligent Communication Technologies and Virtual Mobile Networks. ICICV 2023; "Lecture Notes on Data Engineering and Communications Technologies" series, vol. 171. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-1767-9_33
  26. ^ Val Danilov, I. "A bioengineering system for assessing children's cognitive development by computerized evaluation of shared intentionality". Proceedings of the 2022 International Conference on Computational Science and Computational Intelligence (CSCI'22: December 14-16, 2022, Las Vegas, Nevada); Publisher: IEEE Computer Society. Arabnia, Hamid R.; Deligiannidis, Leonidas; Tinetti, Fernando G.; Tran, Quoc-Nam (eds.). ISBN: 979-8-3503-2028-2; IEEE Catalog Number: CFP2271X-USB, pp. 1583–1590. DOI: 10.1109/CSCI58124.2022.00284, https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/10216436?fbclid=IwAR22GYUVlz-Ym4_oFbAVuhqi5Erf28JUR3abSitMv4cVNYIuBqfo3MSHa7c
  27. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Imitation or Early Imitation: Towards the Problem of Primary Data Entry." Journal of Higher Education Theory and Practice, 21(4). 2021. https://doi.org/10.33423/jhetp.v21i4.4222
  28. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Smartphone in Detecting Developmental Disability in Infancy: A Theoretical Approach to Shared Intentionality for Assessment Tool of Cognitive Decline and e-Learning." K. Arai (Ed.): Proceedings of the SAI 2022, LNNS 508, pp. 1–11, 2022. Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-10467-1_19 . https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-031-10467-1_19
  29. ^ Val Danilov I, Mihailova S, Svajyan A. "Computerized Assessment of Cognitive Development in Neurotypical and Neurodivergent Children." OBM Neurobiology 2022;6(3):18; doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2203137. https://www.lidsen.com/journals/neurobiology/neurobiology-06-03-137
  30. ^ Val Danilov I, Svajyan A, Mihailova S. "A New Computer-Aided Method for Assessing Children's Cognition in Bioengineering Systems for Diagnosing Developmental Delay." OBM Neurobiology 2023; 7(4): 189; doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2304189. https://www.lidsen.com/journals/neurobiology/neurobiology-07-04-189
  31. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Contactless Human-Computer Systems via Shared Intentionality: A Concept Design for the Next Generation of Smart Prosthetic Limbs." in: Arai K. (eds) Proceedings of the Future Technologies Conference (FTC) 2021, Volume 3. FTC 2021. Lecture Notes in Networks and Systems, vol 360. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-89912-7_59
  32. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Advanced Artificial Intelligence in Contactless Human-Computer Systems Through Shared Intentionality." In: Arai K. (eds) Advances in Information and Communication. FICC 2022. Lecture Notes in Networks and Systems, vol 438. Springer, Cham. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-98012-2_54
  33. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Shared Intentionality Modulation at the Cell Level: Low-Frequency Oscillations for Temporal Coordination in Bioengineering Systems." OBM Neurobiology 2023; 7(4): 185; doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2304185. https://www.lidsen.com/journals/neurobiology/neurobiology-07-04-185
  34. ^ 33
  35. ^ 33
  36. ^ 33
  37. ^ 33
  38. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Low-Frequency Oscillations for Nonlocal Neuronal Coupling in Shared Intentionality Before and After Birth: Toward the Origin of Perception." OBM Neurobiology 2023; 7(4): 192; doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2304192. https://www.lidsen.com/journals/neurobiology/neurobiology-07-04-192
  39. ^ 33
  40. ^ 38
  41. ^ Val Danilov, I. "Advances in Computer-Aided Diagnosis of Developmental Delay in Children Using Bioengineering Systems: A New Math Model and Algorithm." In Intelligent Communication Technologies and Virtual Mobile Networks (pp. 443-458). Singapore: Springer Nature Singapore. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-1767-9_33

 This article incorporates text from a free content work. Licensed under CC BY 4.0. Text taken from Theoretical Grounds of Shared Intentionality for Neuroscience in Developing Bioengineering Systems​, Val Danilov, Igor. OBM Neurobiology.