Variability hypothesis: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
m Removed parameters. | You can use this bot yourself. Report bugs here. | User-activated.
m Added some more studies and recent findings supporting the GMVH hypothesis
Line 1: Line 1:
[[File:NormalDist.png|alt=Two normal distributions with equal means but different standard deviations.|thumb|Two distribution curves with identical means but different variabilities. Note that the curve with the greater variability yields higher values in both the lowest and highest ends of the range.]]
[[File:NormalDist.png|alt=Two normal distributions with equal means but different standard deviations.|thumb|Two distribution curves with identical means but different variabilities. Note that the curve with the greater variability yields higher values in both the lowest and highest ends of the range.]]
The '''variability hypothesis''', also known as the '''greater male variability hypothesis''', states that males display greater variability in traits than females do. It has often been discussed in relation to [[cognitive ability]], where it has been observed that human males are more likely than females to have very high or very low intelligence. The sex-difference in the variability of intelligence has been discussed since at least [[Charles Darwin]].<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Sex Differences in Variability in General Intelligence: A New Look at the Old Question|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/3/6/518|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2008-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26158978|pages = 518–531|volume = 3|issue = 6|doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00096.x|first = Wendy|last = Johnson|first2 = Andrew|last2 = Carothers|first3 = Ian J.|last3 = Deary|citeseerx = 10.1.1.605.5483}}</ref> Sex-differences in variability are present in many abilities and traits - including physical, psychological and genetic ones. It is not only found in humans but in other sexually-selected species as well.
The '''variability hypothesis''', also known as the '''greater male variability hypothesis''', states that males display greater variability in traits than females do. It has often been discussed in relation to [[cognitive ability]], where it has been observed that human males are more likely than females to have very high or very low intelligence. The sex-difference in the variability of intelligence has been discussed since at least [[Charles Darwin]].<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Sex Differences in Variability in General Intelligence: A New Look at the Old Question|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/3/6/518|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2008-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26158978|pages = 518–531|volume = 3|issue = 6|doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00096.x|first = Wendy|last = Johnson|first2 = Andrew|last2 = Carothers|first3 = Ian J.|last3 = Deary|citeseerx = 10.1.1.605.5483}}</ref> Sex-differences in variability are present in many abilities and traits - including physical, psychological and genetic ones. It is not only found in humans but in other sexually dimorphic species as well.


==Early studies==
==Early studies==
Line 20: Line 20:


==Modern studies==
==Modern studies==
The results seem to vary based on the type of problem, but some recent studies have found that the variability hypothesis is true for parts of IQ tests, with more men falling at the extremes of the spectrum.<ref>{{cite journal|author1=Machin, S. |author2=Pekkarinen, T. |title=Global Sex Differences in Test Score Variability|journal=Science|year=2008|volume=322|pmid=19039123|pages=1331–2|doi=10.1126/science.1162573|issue=5906}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|first =Larry V. | last = Hedges |author2=Nowell, Amy | title=Sex Differences in Mental Test Scores, Variability, and Numbers of High-Scoring Individuals | journal=Science| year=1995 | volume=269 | pages=41–45 | doi=10.1126/science.7604277|pmid =7604277|issue =5220 | bibcode=1995Sci...269...41H}}</ref> In general different studies show different results; for example, males are more variable in mathematical and spatial abilities, but in some others the women are more variable.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A cross-cultural perspective|journal = Sex Roles|date = 1994-01-01|issn = 0360-0025|pages = 81–92|volume = 30|issue = 1–2|doi = 10.1007/BF01420741|first = Alan|last = Feingold}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender Differences in Extreme Mathematical Achievement: An International Perspective on Biological and Social Factors|jstor = 10.1086/589252|journal = American Journal of Sociology|date = 2008-11-01|pages = S138–S170|volume = 114|issue = S1|doi = 10.1086/589252|first = Andrew M.|last = Penner}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title = Global Sex Differences in Test Score Variability|journal = Science|date = 2008-11-28|issn = 0036-8075|pmid = 19039123|pages = 1331–1332|volume = 322|issue = 5906|doi = 10.1126/science.1162573|first = Stephen|last = Machin|first2 = Tuomas|last2 = Pekkarinen}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender, culture, and mathematics performance|journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|date = 2009-06-02|issn = 0027-8424|pmc = 2689999|pmid = 19487665|pages = 8801–8807|volume = 106|issue = 22|doi = 10.1073/pnas.0901265106|first = Janet S.|last = Hyde|first2 = Janet E.|last2 = Mertz|bibcode = 2009PNAS..106.8801H}}</ref>
The 21st Century has witnessed a resurgence of research on gender differences in variability, with most of the emphasis on humans. The results vary based on the type of problem, but some recent studies have found that the variability hypothesis is true for parts of IQ tests, with more men falling at the extremes of the distribution.<ref>{{cite journal|author1=Machin, S. |author2=Pekkarinen, T. |title=Global Sex Differences in Test Score Variability|journal=Science|year=2008|volume=322|pmid=19039123|pages=1331–2|doi=10.1126/science.1162573|issue=5906}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|first =Larry V. | last = Hedges |author2=Nowell, Amy | title=Sex Differences in Mental Test Scores, Variability, and Numbers of High-Scoring Individuals | journal=Science| year=1995 | volume=269 | pages=41–45 | doi=10.1126/science.7604277|pmid =7604277|issue =5220 | bibcode=1995Sci...269...41H}}</ref> In general different studies show different results; for example, males are more variable in mathematical and spatial abilities, but in some others the women are more variable.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A cross-cultural perspective|journal = Sex Roles|date = 1994-01-01|issn = 0360-0025|pages = 81–92|volume = 30|issue = 1–2|doi = 10.1007/BF01420741|first = Alan|last = Feingold}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender Differences in Extreme Mathematical Achievement: An International Perspective on Biological and Social Factors|jstor = 10.1086/589252|journal = American Journal of Sociology|date = 2008-11-01|pages = S138–S170|volume = 114|issue = S1|doi = 10.1086/589252|first = Andrew M.|last = Penner}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title = Global Sex Differences in Test Score Variability|journal = Science|date = 2008-11-28|issn = 0036-8075|pmid = 19039123|pages = 1331–1332|volume = 322|issue = 5906|doi = 10.1126/science.1162573|first = Stephen|last = Machin|first2 = Tuomas|last2 = Pekkarinen}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender, culture, and mathematics performance|journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|date = 2009-06-02|issn = 0027-8424|pmc = 2689999|pmid = 19487665|pages = 8801–8807|volume = 106|issue = 22|doi = 10.1073/pnas.0901265106|first = Janet S.|last = Hyde|first2 = Janet E.|last2 = Mertz|bibcode = 2009PNAS..106.8801H}}</ref>


Recent studies shows that greater male variability in mathematics persists in the U.S., although the ratio of boys to girls at the top end of the distribution is reversed in some specific immigrant groups.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender Similarities Characterize Math Performance|journal = Science|date = 2008-07-25|issn = 0036-8075|pmid = 18653867|pages = 494–495|volume = 321|issue = 5888|doi = 10.1126/science.1160364|first = Janet S.|last = Hyde|first2 = Sara M.|last2 = Lindberg|first3 = Marcia C.|last3 = Linn|first4 = Amy B.|last4 = Ellis|first5 = Caroline C.|last5 = Williams}} <blockquote>''"Greater male variance is indicated by VR > 1.0. All VRs, by state and grade, are >1.0 [range 1.11 to 1.21 (see top table on p. 494)]. Thus, our analyses show greater male variability, although the discrepancy in variances is not large."''</blockquote></ref>
Recent studies shows that greater male variability in mathematics persists in the U.S., although the ratio of boys to girls at the top end of the distribution is reversed in some specific immigrant groups.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Gender Similarities Characterize Math Performance|journal = Science|date = 2008-07-25|issn = 0036-8075|pmid = 18653867|pages = 494–495|volume = 321|issue = 5888|doi = 10.1126/science.1160364|first = Janet S.|last = Hyde|first2 = Sara M.|last2 = Lindberg|first3 = Marcia C.|last3 = Linn|first4 = Amy B.|last4 = Ellis|first5 = Caroline C.|last5 = Williams}} <blockquote>''"Greater male variance is indicated by VR > 1.0. All VRs, by state and grade, are >1.0 [range 1.11 to 1.21 (see top table on p. 494)]. Thus, our analyses show greater male variability, although the discrepancy in variances is not large"''</blockquote></ref>

A 2007 meta-analysis found that males are more variable on most measures of quantitative and visuospatial ability.<ref> Halpern, Diane F. et all. "The Science of Sex Differences in Science and Mathematics."
Psychological Science in the Public Interest. Volume: 8 issue: 1, page(s): 1-51, Issue published: August 1, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x </ref> A 2014 review found that males tend to have higher variance on mathematical and verbal abilities but females tend to have higher variance on fear and emotionality; however, the differences in variance are small and the causes remain unknown.<ref> Hyde, Janet Shibley. "Gender Similarities and Differences." The Annual Review of Psychology. 2014. 65:3.1–3.26 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057. https://www.gwern.net/docs/iq/2014-hyde.pdf. </ref> A 2005 meta-analyses found greater female variability on the standard [[progressive matrices]], and no difference in variability on the advanced progressive matrices.<ref> Irwing, Paul. Lynn, Richard. "Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta‐analysis." British Journal of Psychology. Volume96, Issue 4 November 2005 Pages 505-524. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X53542 . </ref> A 2010 meta-analysis of 242 studies found that males have an 8% greater variance in mathematical abilities than females.<ref> Lindberg, Sara M.,Hyde, Janet Shibley,Petersen, Jennifer L.,Linn, Marcia C. "New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis." Psychological Bulletin, Vol 136(6), Nov 2010, 1123-1135. http://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fa0021276. </ref> A 2012 study of standardized scores across 86 countries showed that variance was consistent within a given country, but differed between different countries, indicating that variance could be driven by sociocultural factors.<ref>{{cite journal|author1=Kane, Jonathan M. |author2=Mertz, Janet E. |title=Debunking Myths about Gender and Mathematics Performance|journal=Notices of the American Mathematical Society|year=2012|volume=59|pages=10–21|url=https://www.ams.org/staff/jackson/fea-mertz.pdf|doi=10.1090/noti790}}</ref> A 2017 literature review by Sean Stevens for the [[Heterodox Academy]] found that males do tend to be more variable than females on a variety of measures of intelligence and personality and that gender-egalitarian countries demonstrate the greatest differences.<ref> Stevens, Sean. Haidt. Jonathan. "The Greater Male Variability Hypothesis. An addendum to the Google memo." 2017. September 4th. https://heterodoxacademy.org/the-greater-male-variability-hypothesis/ </ref>
A 2014 review found that males tend to have higher variance on mathematical and verbal abilities but females tend to have higher variance on fear and emotionality; however, the differences in variance are small and the causes remain unknown.<ref> Hyde, Janet Shibley. "Gender Similarities and Differences." The Annual Review of Psychology. 2014. 65:3.1–3.26 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057. https://www.gwern.net/docs/iq/2014-hyde.pdf. </ref> A 2005 meta-analyses found greater female variability on the standard [[progressive matrices]], and no difference in variability on the advanced progressive matrices, <ref> Irwing, Paul. Lynn, Richard. "Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta‐analysis." British Journal of Psychology. Volume96, Issue 4 November 2005 Pages 505-524. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X53542 . </ref> and a 2007 meta-analysis found that males are more variable on most measures of quantitative and visuospatial ability.<ref> Halpern, Diane F. et all. "The Science of Sex Differences in Science and Mathematics." Psychological Science in the Public Interest. Volume: 8 issue: 1, page(s): 1-51, Issue published: August 1, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x </ref> Moreover, a 2009 study in developmental psychology examined non-cognitive traits including blood parameters and birth weight as well as certain cognitive traits, and concluded that “greater intrasex phenotype variability in males than in females is a fundamental aspect of the gender differences in humans”<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Lehre |first1=Anne‐Catherine |last2=Lehre |first2=Knut |last3=Laake |first3=Petter |last4=Danbolt |first4=Niels |title="Greater intrasex phenotype variability in males than in females is a fundamental aspect of the gender differences in humans." |journal=Developmental Psychobiology |date=2009 |issue=51 |pages=198-206 |doi=doi:10.1002/dev.20358 |url=https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Greater-intrasex-phenotype-variability-in-males-in-Lehre-Lehre/fe2cbb66af0658481e4f2bd5ea737a8494e7d834}}</ref>

2010 meta-analysis of 242 studies found that males have an 8% greater variance in mathematical abilities than females.<ref> Lindberg, Sara M.,Hyde, Janet Shibley,Petersen, Jennifer L.,Linn, Marcia C. "New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis." Psychological Bulletin, Vol 136(6), Nov 2010, 1123-1135. http://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fa0021276. </ref> A 2012 study of standardized scores across 86 countries showed that variance was consistent within a given country, but differed between different countries, indicating that variance could be driven by sociocultural factors.<ref>{{cite journal|author1=Kane, Jonathan M. |author2=Mertz, Janet E. |title=Debunking Myths about Gender and Mathematics Performance|journal=Notices of the American Mathematical Society|year=2012|volume=59|pages=10–21|url=https://www.ams.org/staff/jackson/fea-mertz.pdf|doi=10.1090/noti790}}</ref> A 2013 study of gender differences in creative thinking in mainland China generally supported the hypothesis that boys have greater variability than girls in creativity test performance.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=He |first1=Wu-jing |last2=Wong |first2=Wan-chi |last3=Li |first3=Yin |last4=Xu |first4=Huana |title=A study of the greater male variability hypothesis in creative thinking in mainland china: Male superiority exists. |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |date=2013 |issue=55 |pages=882-886 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.017 |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0191886913003036}}</ref> A 2016 study of twelve databases from the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement and the Program for International Student Assessment were used to analyze gender differences within an international perspective from 1995 to 2015, and concluded, “The ‘greater male variability hypothesis’ is confirmed.”<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Baye |first1=Ariane |last2=Monseur |first2=Christian |title=Gender differences in variability and extreme scores in an international context |journal=Large Scale Assessments in Education |date=2016 |issue=4 |pages=1-16 |url=https://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s40536-015-0015-x}}</ref>

A 2017 literature review by Sean Stevens for the [[Heterodox Academy]] found that males do tend to be more variable than females on a variety of measures of intelligence and personality and that gender-egalitarian countries demonstrate the greatest differences.<ref> Stevens, Sean. Haidt. Jonathan. "The Greater Male Variability Hypothesis. An addendum to the Google memo." 2017. September 4th. https://heterodoxacademy.org/the-greater-male-variability-hypothesis/ </ref>. A 2019 meta-analysis of sex differences in variability across nations in reading, mathematics and science confirmed that variability is greater for males internationally.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gray |first1=Helen |last2=Lyth |first2=Andrew |last3=McKenna |first3=Catherine |last4=Slothard |first4=Susan |last5=Tymms |first5=Peter |last6=Copping |first6=Lee |title=Sex differences in variability across nations in reading, mathematics and science: a meta‐analytic extension of Baye and Monseur (2016) |journal=Large Scale Assessments in Education |issue=4 |pages=1-29 |doi=https://doi.org/10.1186/s40536‐019‐0070‐9 |url=https://largescaleassessmentsineducation.springeropen.com/articles/10.1186/s40536-019-0070-9}}</ref>


==See also==
==See also==

Revision as of 23:10, 26 February 2019

Two normal distributions with equal means but different standard deviations.
Two distribution curves with identical means but different variabilities. Note that the curve with the greater variability yields higher values in both the lowest and highest ends of the range.

The variability hypothesis, also known as the greater male variability hypothesis, states that males display greater variability in traits than females do. It has often been discussed in relation to cognitive ability, where it has been observed that human males are more likely than females to have very high or very low intelligence. The sex-difference in the variability of intelligence has been discussed since at least Charles Darwin.[1] Sex-differences in variability are present in many abilities and traits - including physical, psychological and genetic ones. It is not only found in humans but in other sexually dimorphic species as well.

Early studies

The variability hypothesis originated in the early nineteenth century with Johann Meckel, who argued that males have a greater range of ability than females, especially in intelligence. In other words, he believed that most geniuses and most mentally impaired people are men. Because he considered males to be the "superior animal," Meckel concluded that females' lack of variation was a sign of inferiority.[2][3]

Later that century the character of the variability hypothesis was altered to coincide better with Charles Darwin's emphasis on the importance of variation from the average for the evolutionary process. Following Darwin, the variability hypothesis was instead put forward as man's greater variability compared to women, and its application was soon extended to sex differences in mental abilities.[2][3][4] Thus, men were thought to vary greatly in their abilities, while women were assumed to be basically the same in their abilities. The greater number of men at both ends of the intellectual spectrum (as patients in institutions and as great intellectual achievers) was taken as evidence of man's greater innate variability.[2][3][4][5][6][7]

In the early twentieth century, the variability hypothesis flourished in sociological, psychological, medical and educational literature. The hypothesis received support not from empirical evidence, but rather from "armchair dogma" about innate female inferiority.[citation needed] Among the proponents of the variability hypothesis at this time were psychologists G. Stanley Hall, Edward Lee Thorndike, and James McKeen Cattell.[2][3][7] One logical conclusion drawn from the variability hypothesis was that since women were not expected to exhibit above-average intelligence, it was unreasonable to expect eminence from them. This led Thorndike and Hall to suggest the adoption of curricula aimed at preparing women for their future roles as mothers and wives.[4] The only significant critic of the variability hypothesis was Karl Pearson, a British psychologist who had studied variability in 1897 and found no sex differences.[citation needed] Pearson's research was the only published scientific investigation of the variability hypothesis prior to the work of American psychologist Leta Hollingworth.[2][3][4]

Leta Hollingworth's studies

Hollingworth's position at the Clearing House for Mental Defectives allowed her the opportunity to refute the variability hypothesis. By examining the case records of 1,000 patients, Hollingworth determined that, although men outnumbered women in the clearing house, the ratio of men to women decreased with age. Hollingworth explained this to be the result of men facing greater societal expectations than women. Consequently, deficiencies in men were often detected at an earlier age, while similar deficiencies in women might not be detected because less was expected of them. Therefore, deficiencies in women would be required to be more pronounced than those in men in order to be detected at similar ages.[2][3][4][5][7]

Hollingworth also attacked the variability hypothesis theoretically, criticizing the underlying logic of the hypothesis.[2][3][4][8] Hollingworth argued that the variability hypothesis was flawed because: (1) it had not been empirically established that men were more anatomically variable than women, (2) even if greater anatomical variability in men were established this would not necessarily mean that men were also more variable in mental traits, (3) even if it were established that men were more variable in mental traits this would not automatically mean that men were innately more variable, (4) variability is not significant in and of itself, but rather depends on what the variability consists of, and (5) that any possible differences in variability between men and women must also be understood with reference to the fact that women lack the opportunity to achieve eminence because of their prescribed societal and cultural roles.[2][3][4] Additionally, the argument that great variability automatically meant greater range was criticized by Hollingworth.[4][6][how?]

In an attempt to examine the validity of the variability hypothesis, while avoiding intervening social and cultural factors, Hollingworth gathered data on birth weight and length of 1,000 male and 1,000 female neonates. This research found virtually no difference in the variability of male and female infants, and it was concluded that if variability "favoured" any sex it was the female sex.[2][3][4][5] Additionally, along with the anthropologist Robert Lowie, Hollingworth published a review of literature from anatomical, physiological, and cross-cultural studies, in which no objective evidence was found to support the idea of innate female inferiority.[2][3][4][6][8]

Hollingworth's doctoral dissertation also dealt with the psychology of women. Entitled "Functional periodicity: An experimental study of the mental and motor abilities of women during menstruation", it found no evidence of changes in performance associated with phases of the menstrual cycle, refuting a common belief of the time.[2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10] Hollingworth's graduate supervisor was E. L. Thorndike, himself a supporter of the variability hypothesis.[2][3][7]

Modern studies

The 21st Century has witnessed a resurgence of research on gender differences in variability, with most of the emphasis on humans. The results vary based on the type of problem, but some recent studies have found that the variability hypothesis is true for parts of IQ tests, with more men falling at the extremes of the distribution.[11][12] In general different studies show different results; for example, males are more variable in mathematical and spatial abilities, but in some others the women are more variable.[13][14][15][16]

Recent studies shows that greater male variability in mathematics persists in the U.S., although the ratio of boys to girls at the top end of the distribution is reversed in some specific immigrant groups.[17]

A 2014 review found that males tend to have higher variance on mathematical and verbal abilities but females tend to have higher variance on fear and emotionality; however, the differences in variance are small and the causes remain unknown.[18] A 2005 meta-analyses found greater female variability on the standard progressive matrices, and no difference in variability on the advanced progressive matrices, [19] and a 2007 meta-analysis found that males are more variable on most measures of quantitative and visuospatial ability.[20] Moreover, a 2009 study in developmental psychology examined non-cognitive traits including blood parameters and birth weight as well as certain cognitive traits, and concluded that “greater intrasex phenotype variability in males than in females is a fundamental aspect of the gender differences in humans”[21]

2010 meta-analysis of 242 studies found that males have an 8% greater variance in mathematical abilities than females.[22] A 2012 study of standardized scores across 86 countries showed that variance was consistent within a given country, but differed between different countries, indicating that variance could be driven by sociocultural factors.[23] A 2013 study of gender differences in creative thinking in mainland China generally supported the hypothesis that boys have greater variability than girls in creativity test performance.[24] A 2016 study of twelve databases from the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement and the Program for International Student Assessment were used to analyze gender differences within an international perspective from 1995 to 2015, and concluded, “The ‘greater male variability hypothesis’ is confirmed.”[25]

A 2017 literature review by Sean Stevens for the Heterodox Academy found that males do tend to be more variable than females on a variety of measures of intelligence and personality and that gender-egalitarian countries demonstrate the greatest differences.[26]. A 2019 meta-analysis of sex differences in variability across nations in reading, mathematics and science confirmed that variability is greater for males internationally.[27]

See also

References

  1. ^ Johnson, Wendy; Carothers, Andrew; Deary, Ian J. (2008-11-01). "Sex Differences in Variability in General Intelligence: A New Look at the Old Question". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 3 (6): 518–531. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.605.5483. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00096.x. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 26158978.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Benjamin, L. T. (1975). The pioneering work of Leta Hollingworth in the psychology of women. Nebraska History, 56, 493-505.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Benjamin, L. T. (1990). Leta Stetter Hollingworth: Psychologist, educator, feminist. Roeper Review, 12, 145-151.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Shields, S. A. (1975). Ms. Pilgrim’s Progress: The contributions of Leta Stetter Hollingworth to the psychology of women. American Psychologist, 30, 852-857.
  5. ^ a b c d Benjamin, L. T., & Shields, S. A. (1990). Leta Stetter Hollingworth (1886-1939). In A. N. O’Connell & N. F. Russo (Eds.), Women in psychology: A bio-bibliographic sourcebook (pp.173-183). Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood.
  6. ^ a b c d Denmark, F. L., & Fernandez, L. C. (1993). Historical development of the psychology of women. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 1-22). Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood.
  7. ^ a b c d e Shields, S. A. (1991). Leta Stetter Hollingworth: "Literature of Opinion" and the study of individual differences. In G. A. Kimble, M. Wertheimer, and C. White (Eds.), Portraits of pioneers in psychology (pp.243-255). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  8. ^ a b c Poffenberger, A. T. (1940). Leta Stetter Hollingworth: 1886-1939. The American Journal of Psychology, 53, 299-301.
  9. ^ Gates, A. I. (1940). Leta S. Hollingworth. Science, 91, 9-11.
  10. ^ Shields, S. A., & Mallory, M. E. (1987). Leta Stetter Hollingworth speaks on "Columbia’s legacy". Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 285-300.
  11. ^ Machin, S.; Pekkarinen, T. (2008). "Global Sex Differences in Test Score Variability". Science. 322 (5906): 1331–2. doi:10.1126/science.1162573. PMID 19039123.
  12. ^ Hedges, Larry V.; Nowell, Amy (1995). "Sex Differences in Mental Test Scores, Variability, and Numbers of High-Scoring Individuals". Science. 269 (5220): 41–45. Bibcode:1995Sci...269...41H. doi:10.1126/science.7604277. PMID 7604277.
  13. ^ Feingold, Alan (1994-01-01). "Gender differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A cross-cultural perspective". Sex Roles. 30 (1–2): 81–92. doi:10.1007/BF01420741. ISSN 0360-0025.
  14. ^ Penner, Andrew M. (2008-11-01). "Gender Differences in Extreme Mathematical Achievement: An International Perspective on Biological and Social Factors". American Journal of Sociology. 114 (S1): S138–S170. doi:10.1086/589252. JSTOR 10.1086/589252.
  15. ^ Machin, Stephen; Pekkarinen, Tuomas (2008-11-28). "Global Sex Differences in Test Score Variability". Science. 322 (5906): 1331–1332. doi:10.1126/science.1162573. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 19039123.
  16. ^ Hyde, Janet S.; Mertz, Janet E. (2009-06-02). "Gender, culture, and mathematics performance". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (22): 8801–8807. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.8801H. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901265106. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2689999. PMID 19487665.
  17. ^ Hyde, Janet S.; Lindberg, Sara M.; Linn, Marcia C.; Ellis, Amy B.; Williams, Caroline C. (2008-07-25). "Gender Similarities Characterize Math Performance". Science. 321 (5888): 494–495. doi:10.1126/science.1160364. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 18653867.

    "Greater male variance is indicated by VR > 1.0. All VRs, by state and grade, are >1.0 [range 1.11 to 1.21 (see top table on p. 494)]. Thus, our analyses show greater male variability, although the discrepancy in variances is not large"

  18. ^ Hyde, Janet Shibley. "Gender Similarities and Differences." The Annual Review of Psychology. 2014. 65:3.1–3.26 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057. https://www.gwern.net/docs/iq/2014-hyde.pdf.
  19. ^ Irwing, Paul. Lynn, Richard. "Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta‐analysis." British Journal of Psychology. Volume96, Issue 4 November 2005 Pages 505-524. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X53542 .
  20. ^ Halpern, Diane F. et all. "The Science of Sex Differences in Science and Mathematics." Psychological Science in the Public Interest. Volume: 8 issue: 1, page(s): 1-51, Issue published: August 1, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x
  21. ^ Lehre, Anne‐Catherine; Lehre, Knut; Laake, Petter; Danbolt, Niels (2009). ""Greater intrasex phenotype variability in males than in females is a fundamental aspect of the gender differences in humans."". Developmental Psychobiology (51): 198–206. doi:doi:10.1002/dev.20358. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  22. ^ Lindberg, Sara M.,Hyde, Janet Shibley,Petersen, Jennifer L.,Linn, Marcia C. "New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis." Psychological Bulletin, Vol 136(6), Nov 2010, 1123-1135. http://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fa0021276.
  23. ^ Kane, Jonathan M.; Mertz, Janet E. (2012). "Debunking Myths about Gender and Mathematics Performance" (PDF). Notices of the American Mathematical Society. 59: 10–21. doi:10.1090/noti790.
  24. ^ He, Wu-jing; Wong, Wan-chi; Li, Yin; Xu, Huana (2013). "A study of the greater male variability hypothesis in creative thinking in mainland china: Male superiority exists". Personality and Individual Differences (55): 882–886. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.017.
  25. ^ Baye, Ariane; Monseur, Christian (2016). "Gender differences in variability and extreme scores in an international context". Large Scale Assessments in Education (4): 1–16.
  26. ^ Stevens, Sean. Haidt. Jonathan. "The Greater Male Variability Hypothesis. An addendum to the Google memo." 2017. September 4th. https://heterodoxacademy.org/the-greater-male-variability-hypothesis/
  27. ^ Gray, Helen; Lyth, Andrew; McKenna, Catherine; Slothard, Susan; Tymms, Peter; Copping, Lee. "Sex differences in variability across nations in reading, mathematics and science: a meta‐analytic extension of Baye and Monseur (2016)". Large Scale Assessments in Education (4): 1–29. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/s40536‐019‐0070‐9. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help); External link in |doi= (help)