Jump to content

Fixation (visual): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Journal cites:, templated 4 journal cites
m Journal cites:,
Line 1: Line 1:
{{primary sources|date=December 2018}}
{{primary sources|date=December 2018}}
[[File:Two Types of Fixational Eye Movement.png|thumb|Microsaccades and Ocular Drifts]]
[[File:Two Types of Fixational Eye Movement.png|thumb|Microsaccades and Ocular Drifts]]
'''Fixation''' or '''visual fixation''' is the maintaining of the [[Visual perception|visual]] gaze on a single location. An animal can exhibit visual fixation if they possess a [[Fovea centralis|fovea]] in the anatomy of their eye. The fovea is typically located at the center of the retina and is the point of clearest vision. The species in which fixational eye movement has been found thus far include humans, primates, cats, rabbits, turtles, salamanders, and owls. Regular eye movement alternates between [[saccade]]s and visual fixations, the notable exception being in [[smooth pursuit]], controlled by a different neural substrate that appears to have developed for hunting prey. The term "fixation" can either be used to refer to the point in time and space of focus or the act of fixating. Fixation, in the act of fixating, is the point between any two saccades, during which the eyes are relatively stationary and virtually all visual input occurs. In the absence of retinal jitter, a laboratory condition known as [[Stabilized images|retinal stabilization]], perceptions tend to rapidly fade away.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Pritchard R.M., Heron W., Hebb D.O. | year = 1960 | title = Visual Perception Approached by the Method of Stabilized Images | url = | journal = Canadian J. Psych | volume = 14 | issue = | pages = 67–77 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Coppola |first1=D |last2=Purves |first2=D |title=The Extraordinarily Rapid Disappearance of Entoptic Images |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA |date=1996 |volume=93 |issue=15 |pages=8001–8004 |doi=10.1073/pnas.93.15.8001 |pmid=8755592 |pmc=38864 }}</ref> To maintain visibility, the [[nervous system]] carries out a mechanism called '''fixational eye movement,''' which continuously stimulates [[neurons]] in the early visual areas of the [[brain]] responding to [[:wikt:transience|transient]] [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]]. There are three categories of fixational eye movements: microsaccades, ocular drifts, and ocular microtremor. Although the existence of these movements has been known since the 1950s, only recently their functions have started to become clear.<ref>M. Rucci and M. Poletti. Control and function of fixational eye movements. Annual Review of Vision Science, 1:499–518, 2015.</ref><ref name="Rucci 1–4">{{Cite journal|last=Rucci|first=Michele|author-link=Michele Rucci|last2=McGraw|first2=Paul V.|last3=Krauzlis|first3=Richard J.|date=2016-01-01|title=Fixational eye movements and perception|journal=Vision Research|volume=118|pages=1–4|doi=10.1016/j.visres.2015.12.001|issn=1878-5646|pmid=26686666}}</ref>
'''Fixation''' or '''visual fixation''' is the maintaining of the [[Visual perception|visual]] gaze on a single location. An animal can exhibit visual fixation if they possess a [[Fovea centralis|fovea]] in the anatomy of their eye. The fovea is typically located at the center of the retina and is the point of clearest vision. The species in which fixational eye movement has been found thus far include humans, primates, cats, rabbits, turtles, salamanders, and owls. Regular eye movement alternates between [[saccade]]s and visual fixations, the notable exception being in [[smooth pursuit]], controlled by a different neural substrate that appears to have developed for hunting prey. The term "fixation" can either be used to refer to the point in time and space of focus or the act of fixating. Fixation, in the act of fixating, is the point between any two saccades, during which the eyes are relatively stationary and virtually all visual input occurs. In the absence of retinal jitter, a laboratory condition known as [[Stabilized images|retinal stabilization]], perceptions tend to rapidly fade away.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Pritchard R.M., Heron W., Hebb D.O. | year = 1960 | title = Visual Perception Approached by the Method of Stabilized Images | url = | journal = Canadian J. Psych | volume = 14 | issue = | pages = 67–77 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Coppola |first1=D |last2=Purves |first2=D |title=The Extraordinarily Rapid Disappearance of Entoptic Images |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA |date=1996 |volume=93 |issue=15 |pages=8001–8004 |doi=10.1073/pnas.93.15.8001 |pmid=8755592 |pmc=38864 }}</ref> To maintain visibility, the [[nervous system]] carries out a mechanism called '''fixational eye movement,''' which continuously stimulates [[neurons]] in the early visual areas of the [[brain]] responding to [[:wikt:transience|transient]] [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]]. There are three categories of fixational eye movements: microsaccades, ocular drifts, and ocular microtremor. Although the existence of these movements has been known since the 1950s, only recently their functions have started to become clear.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Rucci M., Poletti M. | year = 2015 | title = Control and function of fixational eye movements | url = | journal = Annual Review of Vision Science | volume = 1 | issue = | pages = 499–518 }}</ref><ref name="Rucci 1–4">{{Cite journal|last=Rucci|first=Michele|author-link=Michele Rucci|last2=McGraw|first2=Paul V.|last3=Krauzlis|first3=Richard J.|date=2016-01-01|title=Fixational eye movements and perception|journal=Vision Research|volume=118|pages=1–4|doi=10.1016/j.visres.2015.12.001|issn=1878-5646|pmid=26686666}}</ref>


== Microsaccades ==
== Microsaccades ==
[[File:Saccades and Microsaccades.jpg|thumb|The lines on this image display the saccadic and microsaccadic movements of a person's eye while they looked at this face. The involuntary, micro-saccadic movement is not steady when the person's eyes are concentrated at the eyes of the woman, while the voluntary, saccadic movement goes around the periphery of the face once at any give point.]]
[[File:Saccades and Microsaccades.jpg|thumb|The lines on this image display the saccadic and microsaccadic movements of a person's eye while they looked at this face. The involuntary, micro-saccadic movement is not steady when the person's eyes are concentrated at the eyes of the woman, while the voluntary, saccadic movement goes around the periphery of the face once at any give point.]]
Microsaccades, also known as "flicks", are [[saccade]]s, involuntarily produced during fixation periods. They are the largest and fastest of the fixational eye movements. Like saccades, microsaccades are usually binocular, and conjugate movements with comparable amplitudes and directions in both eyes. In the 1960s, scientists suggested the maximum amplitude for microsaccades should be 12 [[arcminutes]] to distinguish microsaccades and saccades.<ref name="Collewijn2008">{{Cite journal|last=Collewijn|first=Han|last2=Kowler|first2=Eileen|date=2008-01-01|title=The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control|journal=Journal of Vision|volume=8|issue=14|pages=20.1–21|doi=10.1167/8.14.20|issn=1534-7362|pmc=3522523|pmid=19146321}}</ref> However, further studies have shown that microsaccades can certainly exceed this value.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Troncoso|first=Xoana G.|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Martinez-Conde|first3=Susana|date=2008-01-01|title=Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in|journal=Journal of Vision|volume=8|issue=14|pages=15.1–9|doi=10.1167/8.14.15|issn=1534-7362|pmid=19146316}}</ref> Therefore, amplitude can no longer be used to distinguish microsaccades and saccades. The only way to distinguish microsaccades from saccades is by the time in which they happen: during fixation. Regular saccades are produced during the active exploration of the eye, during non-fixation tasks such as free viewing or visual search. However, microsaccades are distinguished from regular saccades because they are only produced during fixation tasks.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} The circularity of this definition has been the subject of ample criticism.<ref>M. Poletti and M. Rucci. A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions. Vision Research, 118, 83--97, 2016.</ref>
Microsaccades, also known as "flicks", are [[saccade]]s, involuntarily produced during fixation periods. They are the largest and fastest of the fixational eye movements. Like saccades, microsaccades are usually binocular, and conjugate movements with comparable amplitudes and directions in both eyes. In the 1960s, scientists suggested the maximum amplitude for microsaccades should be 12 [[arcminutes]] to distinguish microsaccades and saccades.<ref name="Collewijn2008">{{Cite journal|last=Collewijn|first=Han|last2=Kowler|first2=Eileen|date=2008-01-01|title=The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control|journal=Journal of Vision|volume=8|issue=14|pages=20.1–21|doi=10.1167/8.14.20|issn=1534-7362|pmc=3522523|pmid=19146321}}</ref> However, further studies have shown that microsaccades can certainly exceed this value.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Troncoso|first=Xoana G.|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Martinez-Conde|first3=Susana|date=2008-01-01|title=Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in|journal=Journal of Vision|volume=8|issue=14|pages=15.1–9|doi=10.1167/8.14.15|issn=1534-7362|pmid=19146316}}</ref> Therefore, amplitude can no longer be used to distinguish microsaccades and saccades. The only way to distinguish microsaccades from saccades is by the time in which they happen: during fixation. Regular saccades are produced during the active exploration of the eye, during non-fixation tasks such as free viewing or visual search. However, microsaccades are distinguished from regular saccades because they are only produced during fixation tasks.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} The circularity of this definition has been the subject of ample criticism.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Poletti M., Rucci M. | year = 2016 | title = A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions | url = | journal = Vision Research | volume = 118 | issue = | pages = 83–97 }}</ref>


=== Mechanism ===
=== Mechanism ===
Line 11: Line 11:


=== Medical application ===
=== Medical application ===
Some neuroscientists believe that microsaccades are potentially important in neurological and ophthalmic diseases since they are strongly related to many features of visual perception, attention, and cognition.<ref name=":1"/> Research aimed at finding the purpose of microsaccades began in the 1990s.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal|last=Martinez-Conde|first=Susana|author-link=Susana Martinez-Conde|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Troncoso|first3=Xoana G.|last4=Hubel|first4=David H.|date=2009-09-01|title=Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis|journal=Trends in Neurosciences|volume=32|issue=9|pages=463–475|doi=10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006|issn=1878-108X|pmid=19716186|citeseerx=10.1.1.493.7537}}</ref> The development of non-invasive eye-movement-recording devices, the ability to record single-neuron activity in monkeys, and the use of computational processing power in the analysis of dynamic behavior led to advancements in microsaccade research.<ref name="MartinezConde2000"/>{{primary source inline|date=November 2018}} Today, there is growing interest in research on microsaccades. Research on microsaccades includes investigating the perceptual effects of microsaccades, recording the neural responses they induce, and tracking the mechanisms behind their oculomotor generation. It has been shown that when fixation is not explicitly enforced as it often occurs in vision research experiments, microsaccades precisely shift gaze to nearby locations of interest.<ref>H.-K. Ko, M. Poletti, and M. Rucci. Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task. Nat. Neurosci., 13(12):1549–1553, 2010.</ref> This behavior compensates for non-uniform vision within the foveola.<ref>M. Poletti, C. Listorti, and M. Rucci. Microscopic eye movements compensate for nonhomogeneous vision within the fovea. Curr. Biol., 23(17):1691–1695, 2013.</ref>
Some neuroscientists believe that microsaccades are potentially important in neurological and ophthalmic diseases since they are strongly related to many features of visual perception, attention, and cognition.<ref name=":1"/> Research aimed at finding the purpose of microsaccades began in the 1990s.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal|last=Martinez-Conde|first=Susana|author-link=Susana Martinez-Conde|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Troncoso|first3=Xoana G.|last4=Hubel|first4=David H.|date=2009-09-01|title=Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis|journal=Trends in Neurosciences|volume=32|issue=9|pages=463–475|doi=10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006|issn=1878-108X|pmid=19716186|citeseerx=10.1.1.493.7537}}</ref> The development of non-invasive eye-movement-recording devices, the ability to record single-neuron activity in monkeys, and the use of computational processing power in the analysis of dynamic behavior led to advancements in microsaccade research.<ref name="MartinezConde2000"/>{{primary source inline|date=November 2018}} Today, there is growing interest in research on microsaccades. Research on microsaccades includes investigating the perceptual effects of microsaccades, recording the neural responses they induce, and tracking the mechanisms behind their oculomotor generation. It has been shown that when fixation is not explicitly enforced as it often occurs in vision research experiments, microsaccades precisely shift gaze to nearby locations of interest.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Ko H.-K., Poletti M., Rucci M. | year = 2010 | title = Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task | url = | journal = Nat. Neurosci | volume = 13 | issue = 12| pages = 1549–1553 }}</ref> This behavior compensates for non-uniform vision within the foveola.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Poletti M., Listorti C., Rucci M. | year = 2013 | title = Microscopic eye movements compensate for nonhomogeneous vision within the fovea | url = | journal = Curr. Biol | volume = 23 | issue = 17| pages = 1691–1695 }}</ref>


Some studies suggest the use of microsaccades as a diagnostic method for [[ADHD]].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Panagiotidi |first1=M |last2=Overton |first2=P |last3=Stafford |first3=T |title=Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits. |journal=J Eye Mov Res |date=2017 |volume=10 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.16910/10.1.6}}</ref><ref name="Fried2014">{{cite journal |last1=Fried |last2=Tsitsiashvili |last3=Bonneh |last4=Sterkin |last5=Wygnanski-Jaffe |last6=Epstein |title=ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication. |journal=Vision Res |volume=101 |pages=62–72 |date=2014 |doi=10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.004|pmid=24863585 }}</ref> Adults diagnosed with ADHD but with no medication treatments tend to blink more and make more microsaccades.<ref name="Fried2014"/>{{primary source inline|date=October 2018}} Microsaccades are also being explored as diagnostic measures for [[Progressive supranuclear palsy]], [[Alzheimer's disease]], [[Autism Spectrum]] Disorder, acute hypoxia, and other conditions.<ref name="Alexander2018">{{cite journal |last1=Alexander |first1=Robert |last2=Macknik |first2=Stephen |last3=Martinez-Conde |first3=Susana |title=Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease |journal=Frontiers in Neurology |date=2018 |volume=9 |issue=144 |pages=144 |doi=10.3389/fneur.2018.00144|pmid=29593642 |pmc=5859063 }}</ref>
Some studies suggest the use of microsaccades as a diagnostic method for [[ADHD]].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Panagiotidi |first1=M |last2=Overton |first2=P |last3=Stafford |first3=T |title=Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits. |journal=J Eye Mov Res |date=2017 |volume=10 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.16910/10.1.6}}</ref><ref name="Fried2014">{{cite journal |last1=Fried |last2=Tsitsiashvili |last3=Bonneh |last4=Sterkin |last5=Wygnanski-Jaffe |last6=Epstein |title=ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication. |journal=Vision Res |volume=101 |pages=62–72 |date=2014 |doi=10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.004|pmid=24863585 }}</ref> Adults diagnosed with ADHD but with no medication treatments tend to blink more and make more microsaccades.<ref name="Fried2014"/>{{primary source inline|date=October 2018}} Microsaccades are also being explored as diagnostic measures for [[Progressive supranuclear palsy]], [[Alzheimer's disease]], [[Autism Spectrum]] Disorder, acute hypoxia, and other conditions.<ref name="Alexander2018">{{cite journal |last1=Alexander |first1=Robert |last2=Macknik |first2=Stephen |last3=Martinez-Conde |first3=Susana |title=Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease |journal=Frontiers in Neurology |date=2018 |volume=9 |issue=144 |pages=144 |doi=10.3389/fneur.2018.00144|pmid=29593642 |pmc=5859063 }}</ref>
Line 20: Line 20:


=== Mechanism ===
=== Mechanism ===
The motion of ocular drift is related to the processing and encoding of space and time.<ref name="ahiarieli2001"> {{cite journal | author = Ahissar E., Arieli A. | year = 2001 | title = Figuring space by time | url = | journal = Neuron | volume = 32 | issue = | pages = 185–201 | doi = [https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00466-4 }}</ref> It is also related to acquiring minute visual details of objects that are stationary, in order for these details to be further processed.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Kuang|first=Xutao|last2=Poletti|first2=Martina|last3=Victor|first3=Jonathan D.|last4=Rucci|first4=Michele|author-link4=Michele Rucci|date=2012-03-20|title=Temporal encoding of spatial information during active visual fixation|journal=Current Biology|volume=22|issue=6|pages=510–514|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.050|issn=1879-0445|pmc=3332095|pmid=22342751}}</ref><ref name="ahiarieli2012"> Ahissar, E. and A. Arieli (2012) Seeing via miniature eye movements: A dynamic hypothesis for vision. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 6:89. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2012.00089 </ref> Recent results have shown that ocular drift reformats the input signal to the retina equalizing (whitening) spatial power at non-zero temporal frequencies across a broad spatial frequency range.<ref name="D. Victor 2015">{{cite journal | author = Rucci M., Victor J. D. | year = 2015 | title = The unsteady eye: an information processing stage, not a bug | url = | journal = Trends in Neurosciences | volume = 38 | issue = | pages = 195–206 }}</ref>
The motion of ocular drift is related to the processing and encoding of space and time.<ref name="ahiarieli2001"> {{cite journal | author = Ahissar E., Arieli A. | year = 2001 | title = Figuring space by time | url = | journal = Neuron | volume = 32 | issue = | pages = 185–201 | doi = 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00466-4 }}</ref> It is also related to acquiring minute visual details of objects that are stationary, in order for these details to be further processed.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Kuang|first=Xutao|last2=Poletti|first2=Martina|last3=Victor|first3=Jonathan D.|last4=Rucci|first4=Michele|author-link4=Michele Rucci|date=2012-03-20|title=Temporal encoding of spatial information during active visual fixation|journal=Current Biology|volume=22|issue=6|pages=510–514|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.050|issn=1879-0445|pmc=3332095|pmid=22342751}}</ref><ref name="ahiarieli2012"> {{cite journal | author = Ahissar E., Arieli A. | year = 2012 | title = Seeing via miniature eye movements: A dynamic hypothesis for vision | url = | journal = Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience | volume = 6 | issue = | page = 89 | doi = [https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2012.00089 }}</ref> Recent results have shown that ocular drift reformats the input signal to the retina equalizing (whitening) spatial power at non-zero temporal frequencies across a broad spatial frequency range.<ref name="D. Victor 2015">{{cite journal | author = Rucci M., Victor J. D. | year = 2015 | title = The unsteady eye: an information processing stage, not a bug | url = | journal = Trends in Neurosciences | volume = 38 | issue = | pages = 195–206 }}</ref>


=== Medical application ===
=== Medical application ===

Revision as of 10:17, 17 February 2020

Microsaccades and Ocular Drifts

Fixation or visual fixation is the maintaining of the visual gaze on a single location. An animal can exhibit visual fixation if they possess a fovea in the anatomy of their eye. The fovea is typically located at the center of the retina and is the point of clearest vision. The species in which fixational eye movement has been found thus far include humans, primates, cats, rabbits, turtles, salamanders, and owls. Regular eye movement alternates between saccades and visual fixations, the notable exception being in smooth pursuit, controlled by a different neural substrate that appears to have developed for hunting prey. The term "fixation" can either be used to refer to the point in time and space of focus or the act of fixating. Fixation, in the act of fixating, is the point between any two saccades, during which the eyes are relatively stationary and virtually all visual input occurs. In the absence of retinal jitter, a laboratory condition known as retinal stabilization, perceptions tend to rapidly fade away.[1][2] To maintain visibility, the nervous system carries out a mechanism called fixational eye movement, which continuously stimulates neurons in the early visual areas of the brain responding to transient stimuli. There are three categories of fixational eye movements: microsaccades, ocular drifts, and ocular microtremor. Although the existence of these movements has been known since the 1950s, only recently their functions have started to become clear.[3][4]

Microsaccades

The lines on this image display the saccadic and microsaccadic movements of a person's eye while they looked at this face. The involuntary, micro-saccadic movement is not steady when the person's eyes are concentrated at the eyes of the woman, while the voluntary, saccadic movement goes around the periphery of the face once at any give point.

Microsaccades, also known as "flicks", are saccades, involuntarily produced during fixation periods. They are the largest and fastest of the fixational eye movements. Like saccades, microsaccades are usually binocular, and conjugate movements with comparable amplitudes and directions in both eyes. In the 1960s, scientists suggested the maximum amplitude for microsaccades should be 12 arcminutes to distinguish microsaccades and saccades.[5] However, further studies have shown that microsaccades can certainly exceed this value.[6] Therefore, amplitude can no longer be used to distinguish microsaccades and saccades. The only way to distinguish microsaccades from saccades is by the time in which they happen: during fixation. Regular saccades are produced during the active exploration of the eye, during non-fixation tasks such as free viewing or visual search. However, microsaccades are distinguished from regular saccades because they are only produced during fixation tasks.[citation needed] The circularity of this definition has been the subject of ample criticism.[7]

Mechanism

Moving in a straight-line-fashion, microsaccades have the ability to carry the retinal image from several dozen to several hundred photoreceptor widths. Because they shift the retinal image, microsaccades overcome adaption[5] and generate neural responses to stationary stimuli in visual neurons.[8] These movements might serve the function of maintaining visibility during fixation,[5] or might be related to attentional shifts to objects in the visual field[9] or in memory[10], might help limit binocular fixation disparity,[11] or may serve some combination of those functions.

Medical application

Some neuroscientists believe that microsaccades are potentially important in neurological and ophthalmic diseases since they are strongly related to many features of visual perception, attention, and cognition.[12] Research aimed at finding the purpose of microsaccades began in the 1990s.[12] The development of non-invasive eye-movement-recording devices, the ability to record single-neuron activity in monkeys, and the use of computational processing power in the analysis of dynamic behavior led to advancements in microsaccade research.[8][non-primary source needed] Today, there is growing interest in research on microsaccades. Research on microsaccades includes investigating the perceptual effects of microsaccades, recording the neural responses they induce, and tracking the mechanisms behind their oculomotor generation. It has been shown that when fixation is not explicitly enforced as it often occurs in vision research experiments, microsaccades precisely shift gaze to nearby locations of interest.[13] This behavior compensates for non-uniform vision within the foveola.[14]

Some studies suggest the use of microsaccades as a diagnostic method for ADHD.[15][16] Adults diagnosed with ADHD but with no medication treatments tend to blink more and make more microsaccades.[16][non-primary source needed] Microsaccades are also being explored as diagnostic measures for Progressive supranuclear palsy, Alzheimer's disease, Autism Spectrum Disorder, acute hypoxia, and other conditions.[17]

Ocular drifts

Ocular drift is the fixational eye movement characterized by a smoother, slower, roaming motion of the eye when fixed on an object. The exact movement of ocular drift is often compared to Brownian motion, which is the random motion of a particle suspended in fluid as a result of its collision with the atoms and molecules that comprise that fluid. The movement can also be compared to a random walk, characterized by random and often erratic changes in direction.[18] Ocular drifts occur incessantly during intersaccadic fixation. Although the frequency of ocular drifts is usually lower than the frequency of ocular microtremors (from 0 to 40 Hz compared to from 40 to 100 Hz), it is problematic to distinguish ocular drifts and ocular microtremors. In fact, microtremors might reflect the Brownian engine underlying the drift motion.[19] Resolution of intersaccadic eye movements is technically challenging.[4]

Brownian Motion

Mechanism

The motion of ocular drift is related to the processing and encoding of space and time.[20] It is also related to acquiring minute visual details of objects that are stationary, in order for these details to be further processed.[21][22] Recent results have shown that ocular drift reformats the input signal to the retina equalizing (whitening) spatial power at non-zero temporal frequencies across a broad spatial frequency range.[23]

Medical application

Ocular drifts were first found to be caused by an instability of the ocular motor system.[citation needed] However, more recent findings have shown that there are actually a number of suggested hypotheses as to why ocular drifts occur. First, ocular drifts can be caused by the uncontrollable random movements driven by neuronal or muscular noise.[24] Second, ocular drifts can occur to counter controlled motor variables, namely a motor control loop.[citation needed] Lastly, ocular drifts can be driven by retinal information, in a retino-motor control loop.[citation needed] When the head is not immobilized, as in daily life and as is often true in eye movement recordings in the laboratory, ocular drifts compensate for the natural fixational instability of the head.[18] Ocular drifts are altered by some neurologic conditions[17] including Tourette syndrome[25] and autism spectrum disorder[26]

Ocular microtremors

Ocular microtremors (OMTs) are small, quick, and synchronized oscillations of the eyes occurring at frequencies in a range of 40 to 100 Hz, although they typically occur at around 90 Hz in the average healthy individual.[citation needed] They are characterized by their high frequency and minuscule amplitude of just a few arcseconds. Although the function of ocular microtremors is debatable and not fully known, they seem to play a role in processing of high spatial frequencies, which allows for perception of fine detail.[23][27][28] Studies show that ocular microtremors have some promise as a tool for determining the level of consciousness in an individual[29], as well as the progression of some degenerative diseases including Parkinson's disease[30] and multiple sclerosis.[31]

Ocular microtremor tracing with burst sections underlined

Mechanism

Although originally thought to stem from spontaneous firing of motor units, the origin of ocular microtremors is now believed to be in the oculomotor nuclei in the reticular formation of the brainstem.[32] This new insight opened the possibility of using ocular tremors as a gauge for neuronal activity in that region of the central nervous system. More research must be done, but recent studies strongly suggest that decreased activity in the brainstem correlates with decreased frequency of OMTs.[33]

Medical application

Several methods of recording have been developed to observe these minuscule events, the most successful being the piezoelectric strain gauge method, which translates eye movement through a latex probe in contact with the eye leading to piezoelectric strain gauge. This method is used in research settings; more practical adaptations of this technology have been developed for use in clinical settings to monitor the depth of anesthesia.[34] Despite the availability of these methods, tremor remains more difficult to measure than other fixational eye movements, and studies addressing medical applications of tremor movements are rare as a result.[17] Some studies have, nevertheless, pointed to the possibility that tremor movements may be useful in assessing the progression of degenerative diseases including Parkinson's disease[30] and multiple sclerosis.[31]

See also

References

  1. ^ Pritchard R.M., Heron W., Hebb D.O. (1960). "Visual Perception Approached by the Method of Stabilized Images". Canadian J. Psych. 14: 67–77.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ Coppola, D; Purves, D (1996). "The Extraordinarily Rapid Disappearance of Entoptic Images". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 93 (15): 8001–8004. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.15.8001. PMC 38864. PMID 8755592.
  3. ^ Rucci M., Poletti M. (2015). "Control and function of fixational eye movements". Annual Review of Vision Science. 1: 499–518.
  4. ^ a b Rucci, Michele; McGraw, Paul V.; Krauzlis, Richard J. (2016-01-01). "Fixational eye movements and perception". Vision Research. 118: 1–4. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.12.001. ISSN 1878-5646. PMID 26686666.
  5. ^ a b c Collewijn, Han; Kowler, Eileen (2008-01-01). "The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control". Journal of Vision. 8 (14): 20.1–21. doi:10.1167/8.14.20. ISSN 1534-7362. PMC 3522523. PMID 19146321.
  6. ^ Troncoso, Xoana G.; Macknik, Stephen L.; Martinez-Conde, Susana (2008-01-01). "Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in". Journal of Vision. 8 (14): 15.1–9. doi:10.1167/8.14.15. ISSN 1534-7362. PMID 19146316.
  7. ^ Poletti M., Rucci M. (2016). "A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions". Vision Research. 118: 83–97.
  8. ^ a b Martinez-Conde, S.; Macknik, S. L.; Hubel, D. H. (2000-03-01). "Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys". Nature Neuroscience. 3 (3): 251–258. doi:10.1038/72961. ISSN 1097-6256. PMID 10700257.
  9. ^ Laubrock; Engbert; Kliegl (2005). "Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention". Vision Research. 45 (6): 721–730. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.029. PMID 15639499.
  10. ^ Martinez-Conde, S; Alexander, R (2019). "A gaze bias in the mind's eye". Nature Human Behaviour. 3 (5): 424–425. doi:10.1038/s41562-019-0546-1. PMID 31089295.
  11. ^ Valsecchi, Matteo; Gegenfurtner, Karl R. (2015). "Control of binocular gaze in a high-precision manual task". Vision Research. 110 (Pt B): 203–214. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.09.005. PMID 25250983.
  12. ^ a b Martinez-Conde, Susana; Macknik, Stephen L.; Troncoso, Xoana G.; Hubel, David H. (2009-09-01). "Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis". Trends in Neurosciences. 32 (9): 463–475. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.493.7537. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006. ISSN 1878-108X. PMID 19716186.
  13. ^ Ko H.-K., Poletti M., Rucci M. (2010). "Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task". Nat. Neurosci. 13 (12): 1549–1553.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  14. ^ Poletti M., Listorti C., Rucci M. (2013). "Microscopic eye movements compensate for nonhomogeneous vision within the fovea". Curr. Biol. 23 (17): 1691–1695.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ Panagiotidi, M; Overton, P; Stafford, T (2017). "Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits". J Eye Mov Res. 10: 1–9. doi:10.16910/10.1.6.
  16. ^ a b Fried; Tsitsiashvili; Bonneh; Sterkin; Wygnanski-Jaffe; Epstein (2014). "ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication". Vision Res. 101: 62–72. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.004. PMID 24863585.
  17. ^ a b c Alexander, Robert; Macknik, Stephen; Martinez-Conde, Susana (2018). "Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease". Frontiers in Neurology. 9 (144): 144. doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00144. PMC 5859063. PMID 29593642.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  18. ^ a b Poletti M., Aytekin M., Rucci M. (2015). "Head-eye coordination at a microscopic scale". Current Biology. 25: 3253–3259.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. ^ Ahissar, Ehud; Arieli, Amos; Fried, Moshe; Bonneh, Yoram (2016-01-01). "On the possible roles of microsaccades and drifts in visual perception". Vision Research. 118: 25–30. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.004. ISSN 1878-5646. PMID 25535005.
  20. ^ Ahissar E., Arieli A. (2001). "Figuring space by time". Neuron. 32: 185–201. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00466-4.
  21. ^ Kuang, Xutao; Poletti, Martina; Victor, Jonathan D.; Rucci, Michele (2012-03-20). "Temporal encoding of spatial information during active visual fixation". Current Biology. 22 (6): 510–514. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.050. ISSN 1879-0445. PMC 3332095. PMID 22342751.
  22. ^ Ahissar E., Arieli A. (2012). "Seeing via miniature eye movements: A dynamic hypothesis for vision". Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 6: 89. doi:[https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2012.00089. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help); External link in |doi= (help)
  23. ^ a b Rucci M., Victor J. D. (2015). "The unsteady eye: an information processing stage, not a bug". Trends in Neurosciences. 38: 195–206.
  24. ^ Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988). Movements of the Eyes (2nd Edn ed.). London: Pion. {{cite book}}: |edition= has extra text (help)
  25. ^ Shaikh (2017). "Fixational eye movements in Tourette syndrome". Neurological Sciences. 38 (11): 1977–1984. doi:10.1007/s10072-017-3069-4. PMC 6246774. PMID 28815321.
  26. ^ Frey, Hans-Peter; Molholm, Sophie; Lalor, Edmund C; Russo, Natalie N; Foxe, John J (2013). "Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder". European Journal of Neuroscience. 38 (1): 2125–2138. doi:10.1111/ejn.12243. PMC 4587666. PMID 23692590.
  27. ^ M. Rucci, R. Iovin, M. Poletti, and F. Santini (2007), Miniature Eye Movements Enhance Fine Spatial Detail, Nature. 447(7146), 851-854.
  28. ^ Otero-Millan, Jorge; Macknik, Stephen L.; Martinez-Conde, Susana (2014-01-01). "Fixational eye movements and binocular vision". Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 8: 52. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00052. ISSN 1662-5145. PMC 4083562. PMID 25071480.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  29. ^ Bolger, C; Sheahan, N; Coakley, D; Malone, J (1992). "High frequency eye tremor: reliability of measurement". Clinical Physics and Physiological Measurement. 13 (2).
  30. ^ a b Bolger, C.; Bojanic, S.; Sheahan, N. F.; Coakley, D.; Malone, J. F. (1999-04-01). "Ocular microtremor in patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 66 (4): 528–531. doi:10.1136/jnnp.66.4.528. ISSN 0022-3050. PMC 1736284. PMID 10201430.
  31. ^ a b Bolger, C.; Bojanic, S.; Sheahan, N.; Malone, J.; Hutchinson, M.; Coakley, D. (2000-05-01). "Ocular microtremor (OMT): a new neurophysiological approach to multiple sclerosis". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 68 (5): 639–642. doi:10.1136/jnnp.68.5.639. ISSN 0022-3050. PMC 1736931. PMID 10766897.
  32. ^ Spauschus, A.; Marsden, J.; Halliday, D. M.; Rosenberg, J. R.; Brown, P. (1999-06-01). "The origin of ocular microtremor in man". Experimental Brain Research. 126 (4): 556–562. doi:10.1007/s002210050764. ISSN 0014-4819. PMID 10422719.
  33. ^ Bojanic, S.; Simpson, T.; Bolger, C. (2001-04-01). "Ocular microtremor: a tool for measuring depth of anaesthesia?". British Journal of Anaesthesia. 86 (4): 519–522. doi:10.1093/bja/86.4.519. ISSN 0007-0912. PMID 11573625.
  34. ^ Bengi, H.; Thomas, J. G. (1968-03-01). "Three electronic methods for recording ocular tremor". Medical & Biological Engineering. 6 (2): 171–179. doi:10.1007/bf02474271. ISSN 0025-696X. PMID 5651798.