Karl Duncker: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Degraeve (talk | contribs)
KHirsch (talk | contribs)
Line 19: Line 19:


== Publications ==
== Publications ==
* {{cite journal
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1926
|ref=Duncker1926
|title=A qualitative (experimental and theoretical) study of productive thinking (solving of comprehensible problems)
|journal=Pedagogical Seminary and journal of genetic psychology
|volume=33
|pages=642-708
|publisher=Clark University
|location=Worcester, Mass.
|issn=0885-6559
|oclc=1590585
}}


* {{cite journal
*''Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens'', Springer, Berlin 1935
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1929
|ref=Duncker1929
|month=December
|title=Über induzierte Bewegung (Ein Beitrag zur Theorie optisch wahrgenommener Bewegung)
|trans_title=Concening induced movement (A contribution to the theory of visually perceived movement)
|journal=Psychologische Forschung
|volume=12
|pages=180-259
|doi=10.1007/BF02409210
|url=http://www.springerlink.com/content/a125j84uv7298180/
|publisher=Springer-Verlag
|issn=0033-3026
|oclc=1069713
}}


* {{cite journal
(Psychology of productive thinking)
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1932
|ref=Duncker1932
|month=December
|title=Behaviorismus und Gestaltpsychologie
|trans_title=Behaviorism and Gestalt psychology
|journal=Erkenntnis
|url=http://www.springerlink.com/content/m7g448747670n80p/
|volume=3
|pages=162-176
|doi=10.1007/BF01886416
|publisher=Springer Netherlands
|issn=0165-0106
}}


* {{cite book
*''Behaviorismus und Gestaltpsychologie''. In: ''Erkenntnis 3'', S. 162-176
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|title=Zur psychologie des produktiven denkens
|trans_title=Psychology of productive thinking
|year=1935
|ref=Duncker1935
|publisher=Springer
|oclc=6677283
|nopp=135
}}


* {{cite journal
*''Lernen und Einsicht im Dienst der Zielerreichung''. In: ''Acta Psychologica'', Hague, 1, S. 77-82
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1936
|ref=Duncker1936
|month=
|title=Lernen und Einsicht im Dienst der Zielerreichung
|trans_title=Learning and insight for reaching goals
|journal=Acta Psychologica
|volume=1
|doi=10.1016/S0001-6918(36)90037-9
|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6V5T-472C7TD-9/2/e6564ae746503a3d34648eeab35084fa
|pages=77-82
|publisher=Martinus Nijhoff, Elsevier Science B.V.
|location=The Hague
|issn=0001-6918
|oclc=1447968
}}


* {{cite journal
(Learning and insight for reaching goals)
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1937
|ref=Duncker1937
|month=December
|title=Some preliminary experiments on the mutual influence of pains
|journal=Psychologische Forschung
|volume=21
|issue=1
|pages=311-326
|doi=10.1007/BF02441209
|url=http://www.springerlink.com/content/q04g105147825514/
|publisher=Springer-Verlag
|issn=0033-3026
|oclc=1069713
}}


* {{cite journal
*''Ethical Relativity? An enquiry into the psychology of ethics''. In: ''Mind 48'', S. 39-57
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|title=Experimental modification of children's food preferences through social suggestion
|doi=10.1037/h0056660
|journal=Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
|year=1938
|ref=Duncker1938
|month=October
|issue=4
|volume=33
|pages=489-507
|publisher=American Psychological Association
}}


* {{cite journal
*''On pleasure, emotion, and striving''. In: ''Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 1'', S. 391-430.
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1939
|ref=Duncker1939a
|month=April
|title=The influence of past experience upon perceptual properties
|journal=American journal of Psychology
|volume=52
|page=255-265
|url=http://www.jstor.org/stable/1416111
|publisher=University of Illinois Press
|issn=0002-9556
|oclc=1408768
}}

* {{cite journal
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1939
|ref=Duncker1939b
|title=Ethical Relativity? An enquiry into the psychology of ethics
|journal=Mind
|url=http://mind.oxfordjournals.org/content/volXLVIII/issue189/index.dtl
|volume=48
|issue=189
|pages=39-57
|doi=10.1093/mind/XLVIII.189.39
|publisher=Oxford University Press
|issn=0026-4423
|oclc=1594357
}}

* {{cite journal
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|last2=Krechevsky
|first2=I.
|year=1939
|ref=DunckerKrechevsky1939
|month=March
|title=On solution-achievement
|journal=Psychological Review
|volume=3
|pages=162-176
|doi=10.1037/h0060101
|publisher=American Psychological Association
|issn=0033-295X
}}

* {{cite journal
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|year=1941
|ref=Duncker1941
|month=June
|title=On pleasure, emotion, and striving
|journal=Philosophy and Phenomenological Research
|url=http://www.jstor.org/stable/2103143
|volume=1
|pages=391-430
|doi=
|publisher=International Phenomenological Society
}} (also online at [http://gestalttheory.net/archive/Dunemot.pdf The International Society for Gestalt Theory and its Applications])

* {{cite book
|last=Duncker
|first=Karl
|title=On problem solving
|series=Psychological monographs
|volume=58
|year=1945
|ref=Duncker1945
|publisher=American Psychological Association
|oclc=968793
|nopp=112
}}


== Notes ==
== Notes ==

Revision as of 20:03, 14 September 2009

Karl Duncker (February 2, 1903 in LeipzigFebruary 23, 1940) was a representative of the Gestalt Theory of psychology. He attended Friedrich-Wilhelms-University from 1923 to 1923, spent 1925-1926 at Clark University in Worcester, MA as a visiting professor, where he received a masters in arts degree.[1] Until 1935 he was a student and assistant of the founders of Gestalt psychology in Berlin: Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Köhler and Kurt Koffka. In 1935, exiled by the Nazis, he got an assistantship in Cambridge with Frederic Charles Bartlett and later emigrated to the USA where he was again an assistant of Wolfgang Köhler’s at Swarthmore College. He committed suicide at 37 years of age.


Achievements

Duncker coined the term functional fixedness for describing the difficulties in visual perception and in problem solving that arise from the fact that one element of a whole situation already has a (fixed)function which has to be changed for making the correct perception or for finding the solution to the problem. [2]

The candle problem (Karl Duncker, 1945).

In his “candle problem” the situation was defined by the objects: a box of candles, a box of thumb-tacks and a book of matches. The task was to fix the candles on the wall without any additional elements. The difficulty of this problem arises from the functional fixedness of the candle box. It is a container in the problem situation but must be used as a shelf in the solution situation.

Other examples for this type of mental restructuring are:

  • an electromagnet must be used as part of a pendulum
  • a branch of a tree must be used as a tool
  • a brick must be used a paper weight
  • another meaning of a word must be found that is different from the meaning within the context of the sentence

Publications

  • Duncker, Karl (1926). "A qualitative (experimental and theoretical) study of productive thinking (solving of comprehensible problems)". Pedagogical Seminary and journal of genetic psychology. 33. Worcester, Mass.: Clark University: 642–708. ISSN 0885-6559. OCLC 1590585.
  • Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur psychologie des produktiven denkens. Springer. OCLC 6677283. {{cite book}}: Invalid |nopp=135 (help); Unknown parameter |nopp= ignored (|no-pp= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |trans_title= ignored (|trans-title= suggested) (help)
  • Duncker, Karl (1938). "Experimental modification of children's food preferences through social suggestion". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,. 33 (4). American Psychological Association: 489–507. doi:10.1037/h0056660. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link)
  • Duncker, Karl (1945). On problem solving. Psychological monographs. Vol. 58. American Psychological Association. OCLC 968793. {{cite book}}: Invalid |nopp=112 (help); Unknown parameter |nopp= ignored (|no-pp= suggested) (help)

Notes

  1. ^ Schnall, Simone, "Thinking in psychological science: ideas and their makers" (editor: Jaan Valsiner), Transaction Publishers, 2007
  2. ^ Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens, Springer, Berlin 1935

See also