Health halo: Difference between revisions
←Created page with '{{subst:AFC submission/draftnew}} <!--- Important, do not remove this line before article has been created. ---> == Health Halo == The phrase health halo (or...' |
No edit summary |
||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{AFC submission|t||ts= |
{{AFC submission|t||ts=20150624142015|u=Navikagangrade|ns=118}} <!--- Important, do not remove this line before article has been created. ---> |
||
== Health Halo == |
== Health Halo == |
||
The phrase health halo (or health halo effect) denotes a phenomenon in which consumers |
The phrase health halo (or health halo effect) denotes a phenomenon in which consumers are more likely to overeat a food when it is either explicitly marketed or perceived as “healthy.” |
||
are more likely to overeat a food when it is either explicitly marketed or perceived as |
|||
“healthy.” |
|||
=== Research === |
=== Research === |
||
Through a series of experiments, Pierre Chandon, Professor of Marketing at INSEAD, and Brian Wansink, Director of Cornell University’s Food and Brand Lab, have explored this effect in detail. In a study by Chandon & Wansink, participants were asked to estimate the caloric content of a dish from Subway (a “healthy” fast-food option) and McDonalds (an “unhealthy” fast food option)<ref>Chandon, Pierre and Brian Wansink (2007), "The Biasing Health Halos of Fast Food Restaurant Health Claims: Lower Calorie Estimates and Higher Side-Dish Consumption Intentions," Journal of Consumer Research, 34:3 (October) 301-314.</ref>. Both dishes had the same caloric content. Results showed that diners perceived the Subway meal as having lower calories than the McDonald’s meal. |
|||
Through a series of experiments, Pierre Chandon, Professor of Marketing at INSEAD, |
|||
Furthermore, in an extension of this study, Chandon & Wansink found that participants underestimated the calories of an identical dish when it was labeled with a “healthy” restaurant name (Good Karma Healthy Foods) than when it was labeled with an “unhealthy” name (Jim’s Hearty Sandwiches). Another example of the health halo is that higher calorie side dishes are more likely to be ordered when the main dish is from a “healthy” restaurant than when the main dish is from an “unhealthy” restaurant<ref>Chandon, Pierre and Brian Wansink (2007), "The Biasing Health Halos of Fast Food Restaurant Health Claims: Lower Calorie Estimates and Higher Side-Dish Consumption Intentions," Journal of Consumer Research, 34:3 (October) 301-314. </ref>. Furthermore, the health halo is evident in health claims on packaging. When “low-fat” labels appeared on packaging, participants were more likely to eat more of the food<ref>Wansink, Brian and Pierre Chandon (2006), "Can "Low Fat" Nutrition Labels Lead to Obesity?," Journal of Marketing Research, 43:4 (November), 605-17.</ref>. |
|||
and Brian Wansink, Director of Cornell University’s Food and Brand Lab, have explored |
|||
this effect in detail. |
|||
In a study by Chandon & Wansink, participants were asked to estimate the caloric |
|||
content of a dish from Subway (a “healthy” fast-food option) and McDonalds (an |
|||
“unhealthy” fast food option)<ref>1</ref>. Both dishes had the same caloric content. Results showed |
|||
that diners perceived the Subway meal as having lower calories than the McDonald’s |
|||
meal. Furthermore, in an extension of this study, Chandon & Wansink found that |
|||
participants underestimated the calories of an identical dish when it was labeled with a |
|||
“healthy” restaurant name (Good Karma Healthy Foods) than when it was labeled with |
|||
an “unhealthy” name (Jim’s Hearty Sandwiches). |
|||
Another example of the health halo is that higher calorie side dishes are more likely to be |
|||
ordered when the main dish is from a “healthy” restaurant than when the main dish is |
|||
from an “unhealthy” restaurant<ref>1</ref>. Furthermore, the health halo is evident in health claims |
|||
on packaging. When “low-fat” labels appeared on packaging, participants were more |
|||
likely to eat more of the food<ref>2</ref>. |
|||
=== Explanation and Implications === |
=== Explanation and Implications === |
||
Although there are several moderators that still need to be studied, explanations of the health halo effect can be attributed to reduced amounts of guilt, resulting in increased consumption, and increased perceptions of portion size for foods with “healthy” labels and from “healthy” restaurants. Implications of the health halo effect on food manufacturers include increased need for explicit marketing. This could, for example, include explaining what exactly “low-fat” means. In this way, consumers may be less deluded and susceptible to the health halo effect. |
|||
Although there are several moderators that still need to be studied, explanations of the |
|||
health halo effect can be attributed to reduced amounts of guilt, resulting in increased |
|||
consumption, and increased perceptions of portion size for foods with “healthy” labels |
|||
and from “healthy” restaurants. |
|||
Implications of the health halo effect on food manufacturers include increased need for |
|||
explicit marketing. This could, for example, include explaining what exactly “low-fat” |
|||
means. In this way, consumers may be less deluded and susceptible to the health halo |
|||
effect. |
|||
==References== |
==References== |
||
{{reflist}} |
{{reflist}} |
||
<!--- After listing your sources please cite them using inline citations and place them after the information they cite. Please see http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:REFB for instructions on how to add citations. ---> |
<!--- After listing your sources please cite them using inline citations and place them after the information they cite. Please see http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:REFB for instructions on how to add citations. ---> |
||
1. Chandon, Pierre and Brian Wansink (2007), "The Biasing Health Halos of Fast Food |
|||
Restaurant Health Claims: Lower Calorie Estimates and Higher Side-Dish Consumption |
|||
Intentions," Journal of Consumer Research, 34:3 (October) 301-314. |
|||
2. Wansink, Brian and Pierre Chandon (2006), "Can "Low Fat" Nutrition Labels Lead to |
|||
Obesity?," Journal of Marketing Research, 43:4 (November), 605-17. |
Revision as of 14:20, 24 June 2015
This article, Health halo, has recently been created via the Articles for creation process. Please check to see if the reviewer has accidentally left this template after accepting the draft and take appropriate action as necessary.
Reviewer tools: Inform author |
Health Halo
The phrase health halo (or health halo effect) denotes a phenomenon in which consumers are more likely to overeat a food when it is either explicitly marketed or perceived as “healthy.”
Research
Through a series of experiments, Pierre Chandon, Professor of Marketing at INSEAD, and Brian Wansink, Director of Cornell University’s Food and Brand Lab, have explored this effect in detail. In a study by Chandon & Wansink, participants were asked to estimate the caloric content of a dish from Subway (a “healthy” fast-food option) and McDonalds (an “unhealthy” fast food option)[1]. Both dishes had the same caloric content. Results showed that diners perceived the Subway meal as having lower calories than the McDonald’s meal.
Furthermore, in an extension of this study, Chandon & Wansink found that participants underestimated the calories of an identical dish when it was labeled with a “healthy” restaurant name (Good Karma Healthy Foods) than when it was labeled with an “unhealthy” name (Jim’s Hearty Sandwiches). Another example of the health halo is that higher calorie side dishes are more likely to be ordered when the main dish is from a “healthy” restaurant than when the main dish is from an “unhealthy” restaurant[2]. Furthermore, the health halo is evident in health claims on packaging. When “low-fat” labels appeared on packaging, participants were more likely to eat more of the food[3].
Explanation and Implications
Although there are several moderators that still need to be studied, explanations of the health halo effect can be attributed to reduced amounts of guilt, resulting in increased consumption, and increased perceptions of portion size for foods with “healthy” labels and from “healthy” restaurants. Implications of the health halo effect on food manufacturers include increased need for explicit marketing. This could, for example, include explaining what exactly “low-fat” means. In this way, consumers may be less deluded and susceptible to the health halo effect.
References
- ^ Chandon, Pierre and Brian Wansink (2007), "The Biasing Health Halos of Fast Food Restaurant Health Claims: Lower Calorie Estimates and Higher Side-Dish Consumption Intentions," Journal of Consumer Research, 34:3 (October) 301-314.
- ^ Chandon, Pierre and Brian Wansink (2007), "The Biasing Health Halos of Fast Food Restaurant Health Claims: Lower Calorie Estimates and Higher Side-Dish Consumption Intentions," Journal of Consumer Research, 34:3 (October) 301-314.
- ^ Wansink, Brian and Pierre Chandon (2006), "Can "Low Fat" Nutrition Labels Lead to Obesity?," Journal of Marketing Research, 43:4 (November), 605-17.