Jump to content

Human multitasking

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 217.155.206.150 (talk) at 16:18, 11 June 2010 (→‎Social Consequences: let's keep this encyclopaedic please.). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Human multitasking is the performance by an individual of appearing to handle more than one task at the same time. The term is derived from computer multitasking. An example of multitasking is listening to a radio interview while typing an email. Some believe that multitasking can result in time wasted due to human context switching and apparently causing more errors due to insufficient attention. Other research illustrates our brains are capable dealing with certain 'dual multiple tasks' at the same time.[citation needed]

Etymology

The term "multitasking" was originated in the computer engineering industry[citation needed]. It was used to reference the ability of a microprocessor to apparently process several tasks simultaneously.[1] Computer multitasking in single core microprocessors actually involves time-sharing the processor; only one task can actually be active at a time, but tasks are rotated through many times a second. With multi-core computers, each core can perform a separate task simultaneously.

Research on Human Multitasking

Since the 1990s, experimental psychologists have started experiments on the nature and limits of human multitasking. It has been shown multitasking is not as workable as concentrated times. In general, these studies have disclosed that people show severe interference when even very simple tasks are performed at the same time, if both tasks require selecting and producing action (e.g., (Gladstones, Regan & Lee 1989) (Pashler 1994)). Many researchers believe that action planning represents a "bottleneck", which the human brain can only perform one task at a time. Psychiatrist Richard Hallowell[2] has gone so far as to describe multitasking as a “mythical activity in which people believe they can perform two or more tasks simultaneously.”

The Brain's Role in Multitasking

Because the brain cannot fully focus when multitasking, people take longer to complete tasks and are predisposed to error. When people attempt to complete many tasks at one time, “or [alternate] rapidly between them, errors go way up and it takes far longer—often double the time or more—to get the jobs done than if they were done sequentially,” states Meyer  [3]. This is largely because “the brain is compelled to restart and refocus”  [4]. A study by Meyer and David Kieras found that in the interim between each exchange, the brain makes no progress whatsoever. Therefore, multitasking people not only perform each task less suitably, but lose time in the process.

When presented with much information, the brain is forced to pause and refocus continuously as one switches between tasks  [4]. Realistically, this is “a rapid toggling among tasks rather than simultaneous processing.” According to a study done by Jordan Grafman, chief of the cognitive neuroscience section at the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, “the most anterior part [of the brain] allows [a person] to leave something when it’s incomplete and return to the same place and continue from there,” while Broadman’s Area 10, a part of the brain’s frontal lobes, is important for establishing and attaining long term goals  [3]. Focusing on multiple dissimilar tasks at once forces the brain to process all activity in its anterior. Though the brain is complex and can perform a myriad of tasks, it cannot multitask well.

Another study by René Marois, a psychologist of Vanderbilt University, discovered that the brain exhibits a “response selection bottleneck” when asked to perform several tasks at once. The brain must then decide which activity is most important, thereby taking more time. Psychologist David Meyer of the University of Michigan claims that, instead of a “bottleneck,” the brain experiences “adaptive executive control” which places priorities on each activity. These viewpoints differ in that, while bottlenecking attempts to force many thoughts through the brain at once, adaptive executive control prioritizes tasks to maintain resemblance of order. The brain better understands this order and, as psychologists such as Dr. Meyer believe, can therefore be trained to multitask  [5]. Because the brain is an expanse of yet uncharted territory, psychologists do not understand how the brain truly processes input and reacts to overstimulation.

Some research suggests that the human brain can be trained to multitask. A study published in Child Development by Monica Luciana, associate professor of psychology at the University of Minnesota, discovered that the brain’s capability of categorizing competing information continues to develop until ages sixteen and seventeen. Perhaps if people are trained to multitask at an early age, they will become efficient at multitasking. A study by Vanderbilt University found that multitasking is largely limited by “the speed with which our prefrontal cortex processes information.” Paul E. Dux, co-author of the study, believes that this process can become faster through proper training. The research team found that with training, the brain can think and perform certain tasks more quickly, effectively allowing time for another task. The study trained seven people to perform two simple tasks, either separately or together, and conducted brain scans of the participants. The individuals multitasked poorly at first but, with training, were able to adeptly perform the tasks simultaneously. Brain scans of the participants indicate that the prefrontal cortex quickened its ability to process the information, enabling the individuals to multitask more efficiently. However, the study also suggests that the brain is incapable of performing multiple tasks at one time, even after extensive training  [6]. This study further indicates that, while the brain can become adept at processing and responding to certain information, it cannot truly multitask.

People have a limited ability to retain information, which worsens when the amount of information increases. For this reason people alter information to make it more memorable, such as separating a ten-digit phone number into three smaller groups or dividing the alphabet into sets of three to five letters. George Miller, former psychologist at Harvard University, believes the limits to the human brain’s capacity centers around “the number seven, plus or minus two.” An illustrative example of this is a test in which a person must repeat numbers read aloud. While two or three numbers are easily repeated, shown in the beginning straight line, fifteen numbers becomes more difficult, as the line curves. The person would, on average, repeat seven correctly  [7]. Brains are only capable of storing a limited amount of information in their short term memories.

This ineffectiveness of the human brain for multitasking has been demonstrated in different studies [8].

Laboratory based studies of multi-tasking indicate that one motivation for switching between tasks is to increase the time spent on the task that produces the most reward (Payne, Duggan & Neth, 2007). This reward could be progress towards an overall task goal or it could simply be the opportunity to pursue a more interesting or fun activity. Payne, Duggan and Neth (2007) found that decisions to switch task reflected either the reward provided by the current task or the availability of a suitable opportunity to switch (i.e. the completion of a subgoal). A French FMRI study published in 2010 indicated preliminary support for the hypothesis that the brain can pursue at most two goals simultaneously, one for each frontal lobe (which has a goal-oriented area).[9]

Continuous Partial Attention

Author Steven Berlin Johnson describes one kind of multitasking: “It usually involves skimming the surface of the incoming data, picking out the relevant details, and moving on to the next stream. You’re paying attention, but only partially. That lets you cast a wider net, but it also runs the risk of keeping you from really studying the fish."[10] Multimedia pioneer Linda Stone coined the phrase "continuous partial attention" for this kind of processing.[11] Continuous partial attention is multitasking where things do not get studied in depth.

In the ever converging computer and human world of technology we are all multitasking at rates that are significantly higher than previous generations. Some of us are able to talk on a cellphone, text, respond to Social networking all while driving a car. In the workplace some of us have 15-20 different websites open simultaneously and toggle comfortably between the various windows while maintaining very high level of productivity. I refer to these select few individuals as being:

Rapidly increasing technology fosters multitasking because it promotes multiple sources of input at a given time. Instead of exchanging old equipment like TV, print, and music, for new equipment such as computers, the Internet, and video games children and teens combine forms of media and continually increase sources of input  [12]. According to studies by the Kaiser Family Foundation, in 1999 only 16 percent of time spent using media such as internet, television, video games, telephones, text-messaging, or e-mail was combined. In 2005, 26 percent of the time this media was used together  [5]. This increase in media usage decreases the amount of attention paid to each device. Today 82 percent of youth use the Internet by the seventh grade, according to the Pew Internet and American Life Project. A 2005 survey by the Kaiser Family Foundation found that, while their usage of media continued at a constant 6.5 hours per day, Americans ages 8 to 18 were crowding roughly 8.5 hours’ worth of media into their days due to multitasking. The survey showed that one quarter to one third of the participants have more than one input “most of the time” while watching television, listening to music, or reading  [3]. The 2007 Harvard Business Review featured Linda Stone’s idea of “continuous partial attention,” or, “constantly scanning for opportunities and staying on top of contacts, events, and activities in an effort to miss nothing”  [5] As technology provides more distractions, attention is spread among tasks more thinly.

A prevalent example of this inattention to detail due to multitasking is apparent when people talk on cell phones while driving. Talking and driving are not mutually exclusive because focusing on both the conversation and the road uses the same part of the brain. As a result, people generally become more concerned with their phone conversations and do not concentrate on their immediate surroundings. A 2006 study published in the Human Factors journal showed that drivers talking on cell phones were more involved in rear-end collisions and sped up slower than drivers intoxicated over the .08% legal limit. When talking, people must withdraw their attention from the road in order to formulate responses. Because the brain cannot focus on two sources of input at one time, driving and listening or talking, constantly changing input provided by cell phones distracts the brain and increases the likelihood of accidents.

Social Consequences

Because society endorses constant multitasking, such as listening to an iPod while exercising, physical social interaction is negatively affected. Already, youth commonly text and listen to iPods while having a “conversation”. Though they may be talented at switching their attention rapidly between their favorite song, phone, and a friend’s response, they are physically incapable of focusing on both in the same moment; thus, the friend is neglected. Because an important part of a message is communicated through body language and tone of voice, a person who texts while conversing will miss a great part of what the other says.[13] The texting person will also convey the message that they are disinterested in the conversation. A person can feel excluded when talking to someone who whips out a cell phone to text in the midst of conversation. This portrays that the texting person does not care for what the other thinks because they do not maintain eye contact or pay complete attention to the conversation. Through electronic communication, body language and expression are lost altogether. [citation needed] “Thousands of years of evolution created human physical communication… that puts broadband to shame in its ability to convey meaning and create bonds”  [3]. This long-term accomplishment is easily discarded when people turn to the quick, easy methods of communicating through technology. [citation needed]

Multitasking has been criticized as a hindrance to completing tasks or feeling happiness. Barry Schwartz has noted that, given the media-rich landscape of the Internet era, it is tempting to get into a habit of dwelling in a constant sea of information with too many choices, which has been noted to have a negative effect on human happiness [14].

The idea that women are better multitaskers than men has been popular in the media, but there has yet to be a scientific study to confirm this. Limited empirical evidence seems to support the idea that both genders are equal in their ability to multitask.[15]

Observers of youth in modern society often comment upon the apparently advanced multitasking capabilities of the youngest generations of humans (Generation Y and Generation Z). While it is true that contemporary researchers find that youths in today's world exhibit high levels of multitasking, most experts believe that members of the Net Generation are not any better at multitasking than members of older generations.[16]

See also

References

  1. ^ "You say Multitasking like it's a good thing" by Charles J. Abate, March/April 2009 issue of NEAtoday
  2. ^ Hallowell, Richard. Crazy Busy: Overstretched, Overbooked, and About to Snap! Strategies for Handling Your Fast-Paced Life. 2007. Ballantine Books. ISBN 0345482441
  3. ^ a b c d Wallis, Claudia (19 Mar.), The Multitasking Generation (PDF), retrieved 4/26/10 {{citation}}: Check date values in: |accessdate=, |date=, and |year= / |date= mismatch (help)
  4. ^ a b Lin, Lin (11 Sept.), Multitasking in Today’s Learning Environment:Does Technology Make a Difference? University of North Texas., retrieved 4/26/10 {{citation}}: Check date values in: |accessdate=, |date=, and |year= / |date= mismatch (help)
  5. ^ a b c Rosen, Christine (2008), The Myth of Multitasking (PDF), retrieved 4/26/10 {{citation}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)
  6. ^ Moran, Melanie (2009), Training Can Improve Multitasking Ability, retrieved 4/26/10 {{citation}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)
  7. ^ Klingberg, Torkel (2009), The Overflowing Brain: Information Overload and the Limits of Working Memory, Oxford: Oxford UP, pp. 7, 8, ISBN 0195372883
  8. ^ Adam Gorlick (2009). Media multitaskers pay mental price, Stanford study shows, Stanford Report, August 24, 2009.
  9. ^ "Multitasking Brain Divides And Conquers, To A Point". Retrieved 2010-04-23.
  10. ^ Everything Bad is Good for You: How Today's Popular Culture Is Actually Making Us Smarter by Steven Berlin Johnson, p.61
  11. ^ http://www.continuouspartialattention.com Continuous Partial Attention
  12. ^ Foehr, Ulla (12 2006). "MEDIA MULTITASKING AMONG AMERICAN YOUTH" (PDF). The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. Kaiser Family Foundation. Retrieved 3/8/10. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= and |date= (help)
  13. ^ Albert Mehrabian and his colleagues found that for isolated words concerning personal feelings 55% of a person’s message is conveyed through body language, 38% through tone of voice, and 7% through words. This cannot be generalized for longer messages.Mehrabian's Communication Study, 2010, retrieved 4/26/10 {{citation}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)
  14. ^ Schwartz, Barry (2004). The Paradox of Choice: Why More Is Less. Ecco. ISBN 0060005696.
  15. ^ Havel Marilyn A. (2009). Gender differences in multitasking abilities, Missouri Western State University
  16. ^ Carrier, L Mark, Cheever, Nancy A, Rosen, Larry D, Benitez, Sandra, & Chang, Jennifer (2009). "Multitasking across generations: Multitasking choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of Americans", Computers in Human Behavior, Volume 25, p483-489.
  • Ferriss, Timothy. The 4-hour Workweek: Escape 9-5, Live Anywhere, and Join the New Rich. New York: Crown, 2007. Print.
  • Miller, George A. "The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information." Psychological Review 101.2 (1994): 1-17. Print.
  • Strayer, David L., Frank A. Drews, and Dennis J. Crouch. "A Comparison of the Cell Phone Driver and the Drunk Driver." Human Factors 48.2 (2006): 381-91. Print.

Further reading

[1] - The Multitasking Virus and the End of Learning?