Cryptophasia
Cryptophasia is a phenomenon of a language developed by twins (identical or fraternal) that only the two children could understand.[1] The word has its roots from crypto meaning secret and phasia meaning speech. Most linguists associate cryptophasia with idioglossia, which is any language used by only one, or very few, people. Cryptophasia also differs from idioglossia on including mirrored actions like twin-walk and identical mannerisms.
While sources[2] claim that twins and children from multiple births develop this ability perhaps because of more interpersonal communication between themselves than with the parents, there is inadequate scientific proof to verify these claims.
Classification
It has been reported that up to 50% of young twins will have their own twin language which they use to communicate only with each other and one that cannot be understood by others.[3] "In all cases known, the language consists of onomatopoeic expressions, some neologisms, but for the greatest part of words from the adult language adapted to the constrained phonological possibilities of young children. These words being hardly recognizable, the language may turn out to be completely unintelligible to speakers of the parents' languages, but they resemble each other in that they lack morphology and that word order is based on pragmatic principles such as saliency and the semantic scope of words. Neither the structure of the languages nor its emergence can be explained by other than situational factors."[4]
Causes
A delay in the phonological development of one or both twins (or two siblings at similar age of language development) is said to be a main cause of cryptophasia. Twins can develop the ability to communicate with one another without working within the grammar of their parents' language, thus possibly leading to a short-term delay in linguistic development of one or both twins.[3] The causes seem largely not to be in the greater pre- and perinatal biological problems twins may experience, but rather in postnatal factors. These factors occur within the family environment and include both a reduced quality of verbal stimulation and interaction with adult language models which result in greater demands of care-giving made on parents of twins, and the intensive interaction between twins which may increase their chances of developing cryptophasia and idioglossia at the expense of conventional language.[5]
Social effects
An article in Slate dealt with what is observed about the social effects of cryptophasia between twins and how it relates to societies. "Societies need unambiguous ways to distinguish between subject and object," Bakker says. "In the twin situation these can be dispensed with, but not in languages in which it is necessary to refer to events outside the direct situation." [6]
See also
June and Jennifer Gibbons, Barbadian-born Welsh twin sisters whose language was an example of cryptophasia and was exemplified by the twins' simultaneous actions, which often mirrored each other.
References
- ^ Thomas, JG (April 1996). "The early parenting of twins". Military Medicine. 161 (4): 233–235. PMID 8935514.
- ^ How stuff works article
- ^ a b Bishop. "'Twin language': A risk factor for language impairment?". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
- ^ Bakker, P. "Autonomous Language of Twins". PubMED.
- ^ Zani, Bruna. "Communicative Skills in Childhood: The Case of Twins". Discourse Processes. Retrieved 20 February 2013.
- ^ Jon Lackman (Aug 24, 2011) "Dugon, Haus You Dinikin, Du-Ah", Slate