Jump to content

Social buffering: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Effblandl (talk | contribs)
No edit summary
Line 12: Line 12:


=== Early History ===
=== Early History ===
Psychological research in the mid-twentieth century began to increasingly reveal the role of stressful life events on psychological well-being.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Thoits |first=Peggy A. |date=1982 |title=Conceptual, Methodological, and Theoretical Problems in Studying Social Support as a Buffer Against Life Stress |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2136511 |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |volume=23 |issue=2 |pages=145–159 |doi=10.2307/2136511 |issn=0022-1465}}</ref> This was also around the time that there was a focus on creating standardized approach to diagnosing mental illnesses, with the first Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (commonly referred to as the [[DSM-5|DSM]]) being published in 1952.<ref>{{Cite web |title=DSM History |url=https://www.psychiatry.org/psychiatrists/practice/dsm/about-dsm/history-of-the-dsm |website=American Psychiatric Association}}</ref> With a honed focus on effective, universal ways to measure mental well-being, and the application of experimental psychology on social issues, a large literature on the effects of social support began to form. This occurred in an effort to fill in the gaps on the specific factors that mediate the relationship between life events and psychological consequences.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Wilcox |first=Brian L. |date=1981-08 |title=Social support, life stress, and psychological adjustment: A test of the buffering hypothesis |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00918169 |journal=American Journal of Community Psychology |volume=9 |issue=4 |pages=371–386 |doi=10.1007/bf00918169 |issn=0091-0562}}</ref>
Psychological research in the mid-twentieth century began to increasingly reveal the role of stressful life events on psychological well-being.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Thoits |first=Peggy A. |date=1982 |title=Conceptual, Methodological, and Theoretical Problems in Studying Social Support as a Buffer Against Life Stress |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2136511 |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |volume=23 |issue=2 |pages=145–159 |doi=10.2307/2136511 |issn=0022-1465}}</ref> This was also around the time that there was a focus on creating standardized approach to diagnosing mental illnesses, with the first Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (commonly referred to as the [[DSM-5|DSM]]) being published in 1952.<ref>{{Cite web |title=DSM History |url=https://www.psychiatry.org/psychiatrists/practice/dsm/about-dsm/history-of-the-dsm |website=American Psychiatric Association}}</ref> With a honed focus on effective, universal ways to measure mental well-being, and the application of experimental psychology on social issues, a large literature on the effects of social support began to form. This occurred in an effort to fill in the gaps on the specific factors that mediate the relationship between life events and psychological consequences.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Wilcox |first=Brian L. |date=August 1981 |title=Social support, life stress, and psychological adjustment: A test of the buffering hypothesis |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00918169 |journal=American Journal of Community Psychology |volume=9 |issue=4 |pages=371–386 |doi=10.1007/bf00918169 |issn=0091-0562}}</ref>


Specific focus on the attenuation of social support on the negative impacts of stressful events on physical and mental health began in the mid-1970’s.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lin |first=Nan |last2=Woelfel |first2=Mary W. |last3=Light |first3=Stephen C. |date=1985 |title=The Buffering Effect of Social Support Subsequent to an Important Life Event |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2136756 |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |volume=26 |issue=3 |pages=247–263 |doi=10.2307/2136756 |issn=0022-1465}}</ref> This is around the time when the idea of social buffering began to take shape.
Specific focus on the attenuation of social support on the negative impacts of stressful events on physical and mental health began in the mid-1970’s.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lin |first=Nan |last2=Woelfel |first2=Mary W. |last3=Light |first3=Stephen C. |date=1985 |title=The Buffering Effect of Social Support Subsequent to an Important Life Event |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2136756 |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |volume=26 |issue=3 |pages=247–263 |doi=10.2307/2136756 |issn=0022-1465}}</ref> This is around the time when the idea of social buffering began to take shape.
Line 21: Line 21:


=== Social Support ===
=== Social Support ===
Social buffering is a subset of [[social support]], and not all occurrences of social support are social buffering. Social support encompasses both the expectation and actual act of being assisted, nurtured, attended to, or participation in a social network.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |date=2017-05 |title=Social Buffering of Stress in Development: A Career Perspective |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691616680612 |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=12 |issue=3 |pages=355–373 |doi=10.1177/1745691616680612 |issn=1745-6916}}</ref>
Social buffering is a subset of [[social support]], and not all occurrences of social support are social buffering. Social support encompasses both the expectation and actual act of being assisted, nurtured, attended to, or participation in a social network.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |date=May 2017 |title=Social Buffering of Stress in Development: A Career Perspective |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691616680612 |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=12 |issue=3 |pages=355–373 |doi=10.1177/1745691616680612 |issn=1745-6916}}</ref>


Research on social support makes the distinction between perceived support and received social support. Perceived social support refers to the amount of support people believe that they could potentially receive from their available support system, while received social support is the level of support that people have received.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Helgeson |first=Vicki S. |date=1993-05 |title=Two Important Distinctions in Social Support: Kind of Support and Perceived Versus Received<sup>1</sup> |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01008.x |journal=Journal of Applied Social Psychology |volume=23 |issue=10 |pages=825–845 |doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01008.x |issn=0021-9029}}</ref><ref>{{Citation |last=Taylor |first=Shelley E. |title=Social Support: A Review |date=2012-09-18 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342819.013.0009 |work=The Oxford Handbook of Health Psychology |pages=190–214 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=0-19-534281-X}}</ref> Some studies have shown that perceived social support may be better for mitigating the negative effects of stress on health and psychological well-being,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Uchino |first=Bert N. |date=2009-05 |title=Understanding the Links Between Social Support and Physical Health: A Life-Span Perspective With Emphasis on the Separability of Perceived and Received Support |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=4 |issue=3 |pages=236–255 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x |issn=1745-6916}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Taylor |first=Shelley E. |last2=Sherman |first2=David K. |last3=Kim |first3=Heejung S. |last4=Jarcho |first4=Johanna |last5=Takagi |first5=Kaori |last6=Dunagan |first6=Melissa S. |date=2004 |title=Culture and Social Support: Who Seeks It and Why? |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.354 |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=87 |issue=3 |pages=354–362 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.354 |issn=1939-1315}}</ref> meaning there is a measurable difference between the types of social support and proper definition of variables is important to research in the area.
Research on social support makes the distinction between perceived support and received social support. Perceived social support refers to the amount of support people believe that they could potentially receive from their available support system, while received social support is the level of support that people have received.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Helgeson |first=Vicki S. |date=May 1993 |title=Two Important Distinctions in Social Support: Kind of Support and Perceived Versus Received<sup>1</sup> |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01008.x |journal=Journal of Applied Social Psychology |volume=23 |issue=10 |pages=825–845 |doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01008.x |issn=0021-9029}}</ref><ref name=set>{{Citation |last=Taylor |first=Shelley E. |title=Social Support: A Review |date=2012-09-18 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342819.013.0009 |work=The Oxford Handbook of Health Psychology |pages=190–214 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=0-19-534281-X}}</ref> Some studies have shown that perceived social support may be better for mitigating the negative effects of stress on health and psychological well-being,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Uchino |first=Bert N. |date=May 2009 |title=Understanding the Links Between Social Support and Physical Health: A Life-Span Perspective With Emphasis on the Separability of Perceived and Received Support |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=4 |issue=3 |pages=236–255 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x |issn=1745-6916}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Taylor |first=Shelley E. |last2=Sherman |first2=David K. |last3=Kim |first3=Heejung S. |last4=Jarcho |first4=Johanna |last5=Takagi |first5=Kaori |last6=Dunagan |first6=Melissa S. |date=2004 |title=Culture and Social Support: Who Seeks It and Why? |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.354 |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=87 |issue=3 |pages=354–362 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.354 |issn=1939-1315}}</ref> meaning there is a measurable difference between the types of social support and proper definition of variables is important to research in the area.


Social support is robustly associated with positive health outcomes. Research has shown that people with larger [[Social network|social networks]], higher quality [[Relationships and health|relationships]], and greater access to social support resources have better [[Mental health|mental]] and [[Health|physical health]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hornstein |first=Erica A. |last2=Eisenberger |first2=Naomi I. |date=2017-05-02 |editor-last=Brown |editor-first=Kirk Warren |title=Unpacking the buffering effect of social support figures: Social support attenuates fear acquisition |url=https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175891 |journal=PLOS ONE |language=en |volume=12 |issue=5 |pages=e0175891 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0175891 |issn=1932-6203 |pmc=PMC5413011 |pmid=28463999}}</ref> The effects of social support have been studied in many different domains of psychology, such as [[Social psychology|social]], [[Developmental psychology|developmental]], [[Clinical psychology|clinical]], [[Health psychology|health]], and [[neuroscience]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schwarzer |first=Ralf |last2=Leppin |first2=Anja |date=1989-01 |title=Social support and health: A meta-analysis |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08870448908400361 |journal=Psychology & Health |language=en |volume=3 |issue=1 |pages=1–15 |doi=10.1080/08870448908400361 |issn=0887-0446}}</ref><ref>{{Citation |last=Taylor |first=Shelley E. |title=Social Support: A Review |date=2012-09-18 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342819.013.0009 |work=The Oxford Handbook of Health Psychology |pages=190–214 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=0-19-534281-X}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Winemiller |first=David R. |last2=Mitchell |first2=M. Ellen |last3=Sutliff |first3=Jane |last4=Cline |first4=Daniel I. |date=1993-09 |title=Measurement strategies in social support: A descriptive review of the literature |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199309)49:5<638::aid-jclp2270490505>3.0.co;2-7 |journal=Journal of Clinical Psychology |volume=49 |issue=5 |pages=638–648 |doi=10.1002/1097-4679(199309)49:5<638::aid-jclp2270490505>3.0.co;2-7 |issn=0021-9762}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |last2=Taylor |first2=Shelley E. |last3=Gable |first3=Shelly L. |last4=Hilmert |first4=Clayton J. |last5=Lieberman |first5=Matthew D. |date=2007-05-01 |title=Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811907000857 |journal=NeuroImage |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=1601–1612 |doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038 |issn=1053-8119}}</ref>
Social support is robustly associated with positive health outcomes. Research has shown that people with larger [[Social network|social networks]], higher quality [[Relationships and health|relationships]], and greater access to social support resources have better [[Mental health|mental]] and [[Health|physical health]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hornstein |first=Erica A. |last2=Eisenberger |first2=Naomi I. |date=2017-05-02 |editor-last=Brown |editor-first=Kirk Warren |title=Unpacking the buffering effect of social support figures: Social support attenuates fear acquisition |url=https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175891 |journal=PLOS ONE |language=en |volume=12 |issue=5 |pages=e0175891 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0175891 |issn=1932-6203 |pmc=5413011 |pmid=28463999}}</ref> The effects of social support have been studied in many different domains of psychology, such as [[Social psychology|social]], [[Developmental psychology|developmental]], [[Clinical psychology|clinical]], [[Health psychology|health]], and [[neuroscience]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schwarzer |first=Ralf |last2=Leppin |first2=Anja |date=January 1989 |title=Social support and health: A meta-analysis |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08870448908400361 |journal=Psychology & Health |language=en |volume=3 |issue=1 |pages=1–15 |doi=10.1080/08870448908400361 |issn=0887-0446}}</ref><ref name=set/><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Winemiller |first=David R. |last2=Mitchell |first2=M. Ellen |last3=Sutliff |first3=Jane |last4=Cline |first4=Daniel I. |date=September 1993 |title=Measurement strategies in social support: A descriptive review of the literature |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199309)49:5<638::aid-jclp2270490505>3.0.co;2-7 |journal=Journal of Clinical Psychology |volume=49 |issue=5 |pages=638–648 |doi=10.1002/1097-4679(199309)49:5<638::aid-jclp2270490505>3.0.co;2-7 |issn=0021-9762}}</ref><ref name=nie>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |last2=Taylor |first2=Shelley E. |last3=Gable |first3=Shelly L. |last4=Hilmert |first4=Clayton J. |last5=Lieberman |first5=Matthew D. |date=2007-05-01 |title=Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811907000857 |journal=NeuroImage |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=1601–1612 |doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038 |issn=1053-8119}}</ref>


=== Competing Model: Direct Effects Hypothesis ===
=== Competing Model: Direct Effects Hypothesis ===
The social buffering hypothesis is often compared to or evaluated with the direct effect hypothesis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schaefer |first=Catherine |date=1985-03 |title=Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818501200110 |journal=Health Education Quarterly |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=109–112 |doi=10.1177/109019818501200110 |issn=0195-8402}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |date=2014-03-18 |editor-last=Hobfoll |editor-first=Stevan E. |title=Stress, Social Support, And Women |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315803128 |doi=10.4324/9781315803128}}</ref> This hypothesis differs from social buffering in that it suggests that social support enhances physical and psychological well-being regardless of the presence of stressors. This model says that social support is beneficial all the time, and that people with high social support have overall better health than those without it.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Cohen |first=Sheldon |last2=Wills |first2=Thomas A. |date=1985-09 |title=Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |language=en |volume=98 |issue=2 |pages=310–357 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 |issn=1939-1455}}</ref>
The social buffering hypothesis is often compared to or evaluated with the direct effect hypothesis.<ref name=cs>{{Cite journal |last=Schaefer |first=Catherine |date=March 1985 |title=Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818501200110 |journal=Health Education Quarterly |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=109–112 |doi=10.1177/109019818501200110 |issn=0195-8402}}</ref><ref name=seh>{{Cite web |date=2014-03-18 |editor-last=Hobfoll |editor-first=Stevan E. |title=Stress, Social Support, And Women |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315803128 |doi=10.4324/9781315803128}}</ref> This hypothesis differs from social buffering in that it suggests that social support enhances physical and psychological well-being regardless of the presence of stressors. This model says that social support is beneficial all the time, and that people with high social support have overall better health than those without it.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Cohen |first=Sheldon |last2=Wills |first2=Thomas A. |date=September 1985 |title=Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |language=en |volume=98 |issue=2 |pages=310–357 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 |issn=1939-1455}}</ref>


The two models tend to deal with different measures of social support. The direct effects hypothesis measures the level at which a person is integrated into a social network, while the social buffering hypothesis assesses how available the social resources are that help people respond to stressful events.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schaefer |first=Catherine |date=1985-03 |title=Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818501200110 |journal=Health Education Quarterly |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=109–112 |doi=10.1177/109019818501200110 |issn=0195-8402}}</ref> The language around both hypotheses also tends to be different, with direct effects hypothesis often looking at the enhancement of health and well-being as a result of the perception of support and integration in a network, whereas the buffering hypothesis is more concerned with protection (or prevention), especially in times of need.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schaefer |first=Catherine |date=1985-03 |title=Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818501200110 |journal=Health Education Quarterly |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=109–112 |doi=10.1177/109019818501200110 |issn=0195-8402}}</ref>
The two models tend to deal with different measures of social support. The direct effects hypothesis measures the level at which a person is integrated into a social network, while the social buffering hypothesis assesses how available the social resources are that help people respond to stressful events.<ref name=cs/> The language around both hypotheses also tends to be different, with direct effects hypothesis often looking at the enhancement of health and well-being as a result of the perception of support and integration in a network, whereas the buffering hypothesis is more concerned with protection (or prevention), especially in times of need.<ref name=cs/>


Statistically, the direct effect hypothesis holds that there is no interaction between stress and social support,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Whelan |first=Christopher T. |date=1993-01 |title=The role of social support in mediating the psychological consequences of economic stress |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797 |journal=Sociology of Health &amp; Illness |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=86–101 |doi=10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797 |issn=0141-9889}}</ref> meaning the same beneficial effects will be observed notwithstanding of the level of stress. Conversely, according to the social buffering hypothesis, the magnitude of the beneficial effect from social support is larger when stress is present, which is reflected in a statistically significant observable [[Interaction (statistics)|interaction]] when the two effects are studied experimentally.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schaefer |first=Catherine |date=1985-03 |title=Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818501200110 |journal=Health Education Quarterly |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=109–112 |doi=10.1177/109019818501200110 |issn=0195-8402}}</ref> This also means that knowledge of the degree of stress is required for the social buffering hypothesis, where this level not as relevant for the direct effects hypothesis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Whelan |first=Christopher T. |date=1993-01 |title=The role of social support in mediating the psychological consequences of economic stress |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797 |journal=Sociology of Health &amp; Illness |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=86–101 |doi=10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797 |issn=0141-9889}}</ref>
Statistically, the direct effect hypothesis holds that there is no interaction between stress and social support,<ref name=ctw>{{Cite journal |last=Whelan |first=Christopher T. |date=January 1993 |title=The role of social support in mediating the psychological consequences of economic stress |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797 |journal=Sociology of Health &amp; Illness |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=86–101 |doi=10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797 |issn=0141-9889}}</ref> meaning the same beneficial effects will be observed notwithstanding of the level of stress. Conversely, according to the social buffering hypothesis, the magnitude of the beneficial effect from social support is larger when stress is present, which is reflected in a statistically significant observable [[Interaction (statistics)|interaction]] when the two effects are studied experimentally.<ref name=cs/> This also means that knowledge of the degree of stress is required for the social buffering hypothesis, where this level not as relevant for the direct effects hypothesis.<ref name=ctw/>


Despite these models providing somewhat separate explanations, research has found support for both hypotheses,<ref>{{Citation |last=Taylor |first=Shelley E. |title=Social Support: A Review |date=2012-09-18 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342819.013.0009 |work=The Oxford Handbook of Health Psychology |pages=190–214 |access-date=2023-11-26 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=0-19-534281-X}}</ref> and some work even suggests that both processes happen simultaneously.<ref>{{Cite journal |date=2014-03-18 |editor-last=Hobfoll |editor-first=Stevan E. |title=Stress, Social Support, And Women |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315803128 |doi=10.4324/9781315803128}}</ref> Researchers have suggested that work directed at critically comparing the two hypotheses may not actually benefit the field studying social support.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schaefer |first=Catherine |date=1985-03 |title=Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818501200110 |journal=Health Education Quarterly |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=109–112 |doi=10.1177/109019818501200110 |issn=0195-8402}}</ref> Instead it may be more beneficial to use either as a model that aims to explain specific questions about how social support relates to mediators of health, that can be behavioral, psychological, emotional, or biological.
Despite these models providing somewhat separate explanations, research has found support for both hypotheses,<ref name=set/> and some work even suggests that both processes happen simultaneously.<ref name=seh/> Researchers have suggested that work directed at critically comparing the two hypotheses may not actually benefit the field studying social support.<ref name=cs/> Instead it may be more beneficial to use either as a model that aims to explain specific questions about how social support relates to mediators of health, that can be behavioral, psychological, emotional, or biological.


== Biology ==
== Biology ==
A large proportion of research done on social buffering and health consequences shows that the HPA axis plays a central role in the link between the two.
A large proportion of research done on social buffering and health consequences shows that the HPA axis plays a central role in the link between the two.


The [[Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis|hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal]] (HPA) axis is a crucial regulator of [[Neuroendocrinology|neuroendocrinal]] responses in the body. The HPA axis is made up of a series of pathways and feedback loops that involve the [[hypothalamus]], [[Anterior pituitary|anterior pituitary gland]], and [[adrenal gland]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sheng |first=Julietta A. |last2=Bales |first2=Natalie J. |last3=Myers |first3=Sage A. |last4=Bautista |first4=Anna I. |last5=Roueinfar |first5=Mina |last6=Hale |first6=Taben M. |last7=Handa |first7=Robert J. |date=2021-01-13 |title=The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis: Development, Programming Actions of Hormones, and Maternal-Fetal Interactions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=14 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref> It modulates several physiological processes, including the [[autonomic nervous system]], [[immune system]] reactions, [[metabolism]], and several other processes that are active during short-term physiological responses to stress.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sheng |first=Julietta A. |last2=Bales |first2=Natalie J. |last3=Myers |first3=Sage A. |last4=Bautista |first4=Anna I. |last5=Roueinfar |first5=Mina |last6=Hale |first6=Taben M. |last7=Handa |first7=Robert J. |date=2021-01-13 |title=The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis: Development, Programming Actions of Hormones, and Maternal-Fetal Interactions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=14 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref> The HPA axis also plays a major role in bodily [[homeostasis]], which includes regulating the cardiovascular system, [[reproductive system]], and [[central nervous system]] in addition to those previously mentioned.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sheng |first=Julietta A. |last2=Bales |first2=Natalie J. |last3=Myers |first3=Sage A. |last4=Bautista |first4=Anna I. |last5=Roueinfar |first5=Mina |last6=Hale |first6=Taben M. |last7=Handa |first7=Robert J. |date=2021-01-13 |title=The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis: Development, Programming Actions of Hormones, and Maternal-Fetal Interactions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=14 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref>
The [[Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis|hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal]] (HPA) axis is a crucial regulator of [[Neuroendocrinology|neuroendocrinal]] responses in the body. The HPA axis is made up of a series of pathways and feedback loops that involve the [[hypothalamus]], [[Anterior pituitary|anterior pituitary gland]], and [[adrenal gland]].<ref name=jas>{{Cite journal |last=Sheng |first=Julietta A. |last2=Bales |first2=Natalie J. |last3=Myers |first3=Sage A. |last4=Bautista |first4=Anna I. |last5=Roueinfar |first5=Mina |last6=Hale |first6=Taben M. |last7=Handa |first7=Robert J. |date=2021-01-13 |title=The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis: Development, Programming Actions of Hormones, and Maternal-Fetal Interactions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=14 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref> It modulates several physiological processes, including the [[autonomic nervous system]], [[immune system]] reactions, [[metabolism]], and several other processes that are active during short-term physiological responses to stress.<ref name=jas/> The HPA axis also plays a major role in bodily [[homeostasis]], which includes regulating the cardiovascular system, [[reproductive system]], and [[central nervous system]] in addition to those previously mentioned.<ref name=jas/>


Proper functioning of the HPA axis is very important for [[adaptation]] and development, and both over and under reactivity can lead to a series of consequences.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sheng |first=Julietta A. |last2=Bales |first2=Natalie J. |last3=Myers |first3=Sage A. |last4=Bautista |first4=Anna I. |last5=Roueinfar |first5=Mina |last6=Hale |first6=Taben M. |last7=Handa |first7=Robert J. |date=2021-01-13 |title=The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis: Development, Programming Actions of Hormones, and Maternal-Fetal Interactions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=14 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref> It is important for humans to experience high levels of circulating stress hormones early in life so that they can learn to effectively respond to threat and adapt to their environments.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sapolsky |first=R. M. |date=2000-02-01 |title=How Do Glucocorticoids Influence Stress Responses? Integrating Permissive, Suppressive, Stimulatory, and Preparative Actions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/er.21.1.55 |journal=Endocrine Reviews |volume=21 |issue=1 |pages=55–89 |doi=10.1210/er.21.1.55 |issn=0163-769X}}</ref> However, too much stress in childhood can lead to long term and often irreversible dysfunction of the HPA axis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=Michael B. |last2=Kaiser |first2=Sylvia |last3=Sachser |first3=Norbert |date=2009-10-01 |title=Social buffering of the stress response: Diversity, mechanisms, and functions |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0091302209000405 |journal=Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology |series=Hormones & Social Behavior |volume=30 |issue=4 |pages=470–482 |doi=10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001 |issn=0091-3022}}</ref>
Proper functioning of the HPA axis is very important for [[adaptation]] and development, and both over and under reactivity can lead to a series of consequences.<ref name=jas/> It is important for humans to experience high levels of circulating stress hormones early in life so that they can learn to effectively respond to threat and adapt to their environments.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sapolsky |first=R. M. |date=2000-02-01 |title=How Do Glucocorticoids Influence Stress Responses? Integrating Permissive, Suppressive, Stimulatory, and Preparative Actions |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/er.21.1.55 |journal=Endocrine Reviews |volume=21 |issue=1 |pages=55–89 |doi=10.1210/er.21.1.55 |issn=0163-769X}}</ref> However, too much stress in childhood can lead to long term and often irreversible dysfunction of the HPA axis.<ref name=mbh>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=Michael B. |last2=Kaiser |first2=Sylvia |last3=Sachser |first3=Norbert |date=2009-10-01 |title=Social buffering of the stress response: Diversity, mechanisms, and functions |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0091302209000405 |journal=Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology |series=Hormones & Social Behavior |volume=30 |issue=4 |pages=470–482 |doi=10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001 |issn=0091-3022}}</ref>


HPA axis activity goes up during aversive or arousing situations, which can be induced by physical or psychosocial events. The HPA axis is particularly sensitive to psychological stressors,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Mason |first=John W. |date=1975-06 |title=A Historical View of the Stress Field |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940405 |journal=Journal of Human Stress |language=en |volume=1 |issue=2 |pages=22–36 |doi=10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940405 |issn=0097-840X}}</ref> including uncertainty, novelty, and the feeling of being out of control.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ketz |first=H.‐A. |date=1979-01 |title=J. P. Henry und P. M. Stephens: Stress, Health and the Social Environment, A Sociobiologic Approach to Medicine. 282 Seiten, Springer Verlag, New York, Heidelberg, Berlin 1977. Preis: 66,‐ DM, 30.40 $ |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/food.19790230239 |journal=Food / Nahrung |volume=23 |issue=2 |pages=196–196 |doi=10.1002/food.19790230239 |issn=0027-769X}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=John W. |last2=Smotherman |first2=William P. |last3=Levine |first3=Seymour |date=1976-04 |title=Conditioned taste aversion and the pituitary-adrenal system |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0091-6773(76)91571-6 |journal=Behavioral Biology |volume=16 |issue=4 |pages=413–424 |doi=10.1016/s0091-6773(76)91571-6 |issn=0091-6773}}</ref> In addition to being influenced by psychological stressors, one of the most powerful and widely studied moderators of HPA axis activity induced by stressful events is social support.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=Michael B. |last2=Kaiser |first2=Sylvia |last3=Sachser |first3=Norbert |date=2009-10-01 |title=Social buffering of the stress response: Diversity, mechanisms, and functions |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0091302209000405 |journal=Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology |series=Hormones & Social Behavior |volume=30 |issue=4 |pages=470–482 |doi=10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001 |issn=0091-3022}}</ref> This is why the HPA axis is often a focal point in physiological research examining social buffering effects.
HPA axis activity goes up during aversive or arousing situations, which can be induced by physical or psychosocial events. The HPA axis is particularly sensitive to psychological stressors,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Mason |first=John W. |date=June 1975 |title=A Historical View of the Stress Field |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940405 |journal=Journal of Human Stress |language=en |volume=1 |issue=2 |pages=22–36 |doi=10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940405 |issn=0097-840X}}</ref> including uncertainty, novelty, and the feeling of being out of control.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ketz |first=H.‐A. |date=January 1979 |title=J. P. Henry und P. M. Stephens: Stress, Health and the Social Environment, A Sociobiologic Approach to Medicine. 282 Seiten, Springer Verlag, New York, Heidelberg, Berlin 1977. Preis: 66,‐ DM, 30.40 $ |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/food.19790230239 |journal=Food / Nahrung |volume=23 |issue=2 |pages=196–196 |doi=10.1002/food.19790230239 |issn=0027-769X}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=John W. |last2=Smotherman |first2=William P. |last3=Levine |first3=Seymour |date=April 1976 |title=Conditioned taste aversion and the pituitary-adrenal system |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0091-6773(76)91571-6 |journal=Behavioral Biology |volume=16 |issue=4 |pages=413–424 |doi=10.1016/s0091-6773(76)91571-6 |issn=0091-6773}}</ref> In addition to being influenced by psychological stressors, one of the most powerful and widely studied moderators of HPA axis activity induced by stressful events is social support.<ref name=mbh/> This is why the HPA axis is often a focal point in physiological research examining social buffering effects.


The biological mechanisms of social buffering have been explored, and several components have been identified as relevant:
The biological mechanisms of social buffering have been explored, and several components have been identified as relevant:
Line 49: Line 49:
'''Endocrine'''
'''Endocrine'''


There are several hormones involved in HPA axis endocrine regulation. One is [[Corticotropin-releasing hormone|corticotrophin-releasing factor]] (CRF). CRF stimulates the release of [[adrenocorticotropic hormone]] (ACTH), which is the crucial physiological regulator of increased activity of the HPA axis.<ref>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Another is [[vasopressin]] (AVP), and [[Cortisol|glucocorticoids]] are the final step in the process.<ref>{{Citation |last=Aguilera |first=Greti |title=The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis and Neuroendocrine Responses to Stress |date=2012 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-375097-6.10008-3 |work=Handbook of Neuroendocrinology |pages=175–196 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Social buffering is observed when high levels of social support are correlated with lower levels of stress hormone and HPA axis activation.
There are several hormones involved in HPA axis endocrine regulation. One is [[Corticotropin-releasing hormone|corticotrophin-releasing factor]] (CRF). CRF stimulates the release of [[adrenocorticotropic hormone]] (ACTH), which is the crucial physiological regulator of increased activity of the HPA axis.<ref name=yk>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Another is [[vasopressin]] (AVP), and [[Cortisol|glucocorticoids]] are the final step in the process.<ref>{{Citation |last=Aguilera |first=Greti |title=The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis and Neuroendocrine Responses to Stress |date=2012 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-375097-6.10008-3 |work=Handbook of Neuroendocrinology |pages=175–196 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Social buffering is observed when high levels of social support are correlated with lower levels of stress hormone and HPA axis activation.


'''Oxytocin'''
'''Oxytocin'''


[[Oxytocin]] has been recognized as an important hormone involved in the mechanism of social buffering. Oxytocin is a molecule that is often called the “love hormone”.<ref>{{Cite web |date=2021-07-20 |title=Oxytocin: The love hormone |url=https://www.health.harvard.edu/mind-and-mood/oxytocin-the-love-hormone |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=Harvard Health |language=en}}</ref> It is released into the blood in response to physiological and psychological stress, and increased oxytocin release from the hypothalamus inhibits activation of the HPA axis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=DeVries |first=A.Courtney |last2=Glasper |first2=Erica R |last3=Detillion |first3=Courtney E |date=2003-08 |title=Social modulation of stress responses |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00152-5 |journal=Physiology &amp; Behavior |volume=79 |issue=3 |pages=399–407 |doi=10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00152-5 |issn=0031-9384}}</ref><ref>{{Citation |last=Neumann |first=Inga D. |title=Chapter 12 Involvement of the brain oxytocin system in stress coping: interactions with the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis |date=2002 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0079-6123(02)39014-9 |work=Vasopressin and Oxytocin: From Genes to Clinical Applications |pages=147–162 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Oxytocin is involved in stress processes by inhibiting the release of corticosteroids, ACTH release from the pituitary gland, and release of CRF from the hypothalamus.<ref>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> The central nervous system also meditates the inhibitory effect of oxytocin on the HPA axis.<ref>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Oxytocin is also involved in the behavioral side of social buffering. Oxytocin's role in bonding means that it is related to the process of social support, in addition to being released in response to a stressor.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Crockford |first=Catherine |last2=Deschner |first2=Tobias |last3=Wittig |first3=Roman M. |date=2018 |title=The Role of Oxytocin in Social Buffering: What Do Primate Studies Add? |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28864973/#:~:text=The%20neuropeptide,%20oxytocin,%20may%20mediate,thus%20reducing%20the%20stress%20response. |journal=Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences |volume=35 |pages=155–173 |doi=10.1007/7854_2017_12 |issn=1866-3370 |pmid=28864973}}</ref>
[[Oxytocin]] has been recognized as an important hormone involved in the mechanism of social buffering. Oxytocin is a molecule that is often called the “love hormone”.<ref>{{Cite web |date=2021-07-20 |title=Oxytocin: The love hormone |url=https://www.health.harvard.edu/mind-and-mood/oxytocin-the-love-hormone |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=Harvard Health |language=en}}</ref> It is released into the blood in response to physiological and psychological stress, and increased oxytocin release from the hypothalamus inhibits activation of the HPA axis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=DeVries |first=A.Courtney |last2=Glasper |first2=Erica R |last3=Detillion |first3=Courtney E |date=August 2003 |title=Social modulation of stress responses |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00152-5 |journal=Physiology &amp; Behavior |volume=79 |issue=3 |pages=399–407 |doi=10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00152-5 |issn=0031-9384}}</ref><ref>{{Citation |last=Neumann |first=Inga D. |title=Chapter 12 Involvement of the brain oxytocin system in stress coping: interactions with the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis |date=2002 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0079-6123(02)39014-9 |work=Vasopressin and Oxytocin: From Genes to Clinical Applications |pages=147–162 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Oxytocin is involved in stress processes by inhibiting the release of corticosteroids, ACTH release from the pituitary gland, and release of CRF from the hypothalamus.<ref name=yk/> The central nervous system also meditates the inhibitory effect of oxytocin on the HPA axis.<ref name=yk/> Oxytocin is also involved in the behavioral side of social buffering. Oxytocin's role in bonding means that it is related to the process of social support, in addition to being released in response to a stressor.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Crockford |first=Catherine |last2=Deschner |first2=Tobias |last3=Wittig |first3=Roman M. |date=2018 |title=The Role of Oxytocin in Social Buffering: What Do Primate Studies Add? |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28864973/#:~:text=The%20neuropeptide,%20oxytocin,%20may%20mediate,thus%20reducing%20the%20stress%20response. |journal=Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences |volume=35 |pages=155–173 |doi=10.1007/7854_2017_12 |issn=1866-3370 |pmid=28864973}}</ref>


'''Opioids'''
'''Opioids'''


Endogenous opioids also appear to play a major role in social buffering, via a reinforcing effect that is active in social attachment.<ref>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Opioid release is also observed when animals recognize each other, which supports the idea that animals find social support in others of the same species. This is necessary to see social buffering effects in animals, and opioid release also helps explain the seeking of affiliation in animals as well. Opioid release as a result of social stimulation has also been found to reduce CRF activity in the brain and body.<ref>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref>
Endogenous opioids also appear to play a major role in social buffering, via a reinforcing effect that is active in social attachment.<ref name=yk/> Opioid release is also observed when animals recognize each other, which supports the idea that animals find social support in others of the same species. This is necessary to see social buffering effects in animals, and opioid release also helps explain the seeking of affiliation in animals as well. Opioid release as a result of social stimulation has also been found to reduce CRF activity in the brain and body.<ref name=yk/>


'''Neural Circuitry'''
'''Neural Circuitry'''
Line 63: Line 63:
Research on the brain regions involved in social buffering is less extensive as the HPA axis and cortisol.
Research on the brain regions involved in social buffering is less extensive as the HPA axis and cortisol.


However, the [[prefrontal cortex]] has been identified as a region involved in the social buffering and stress responses. Higher activity in the prefrontal cortex has been found to be correlated with higher degrees of cortisol responses to stress. A similar relationship has been observed in the [[anterior cingulate cortex]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |last2=Taylor |first2=Shelley E. |last3=Gable |first3=Shelly L. |last4=Hilmert |first4=Clayton J. |last5=Lieberman |first5=Matthew D. |date=2007-05-01 |title=Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811907000857 |journal=NeuroImage |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=1601–1612 |doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038 |issn=1053-8119}}</ref> Both the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex have been shown to be involved in emotional responses and reactivity to stressful situations,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |last2=Lieberman |first2=Matthew D. |last3=Williams |first3=Kipling D. |date=2003-10-10 |title=Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion |url=https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1089134 |journal=Science |language=en |volume=302 |issue=5643 |pages=290–292 |doi=10.1126/science.1089134 |issn=0036-8075}}</ref> and activity in these areas is closely related with HPA activation. The presence of social support causes cortisol levels to go down as well as decreased activity in these regions.
However, the [[prefrontal cortex]] has been identified as a region involved in the social buffering and stress responses. Higher activity in the prefrontal cortex has been found to be correlated with higher degrees of cortisol responses to stress. A similar relationship has been observed in the [[anterior cingulate cortex]].<ref name=nie/> Both the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex have been shown to be involved in emotional responses and reactivity to stressful situations,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |last2=Lieberman |first2=Matthew D. |last3=Williams |first3=Kipling D. |date=2003-10-10 |title=Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion |url=https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1089134 |journal=Science |language=en |volume=302 |issue=5643 |pages=290–292 |doi=10.1126/science.1089134 |issn=0036-8075}}</ref> and activity in these areas is closely related with HPA activation. The presence of social support causes cortisol levels to go down as well as decreased activity in these regions.


Social buffering has also been observed in regions associated with responding to safety, which include the [[ventromedial prefrontal cortex]], [[anterior cingulate cortex]], posterior insula, and [[posterior cingulate cortex]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |date=2013-07 |title=An Empirical Review of the Neural Underpinnings of Receiving and Giving Social Support: Implications for Health |url=https://journals.lww.com/00006842-201307000-00006 |journal=Psychosomatic Medicine |language=en |volume=75 |issue=6 |pages=545–556 |doi=10.1097/PSY.0b013e31829de2e7 |issn=0033-3174 |pmc=PMC3763941 |pmid=23804014}}</ref>
Social buffering has also been observed in regions associated with responding to safety, which include the [[ventromedial prefrontal cortex]], [[anterior cingulate cortex]], posterior insula, and [[posterior cingulate cortex]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Eisenberger |first=Naomi I. |date=July 2013 |title=An Empirical Review of the Neural Underpinnings of Receiving and Giving Social Support: Implications for Health |url=https://journals.lww.com/00006842-201307000-00006 |journal=Psychosomatic Medicine |language=en |volume=75 |issue=6 |pages=545–556 |doi=10.1097/PSY.0b013e31829de2e7 |issn=0033-3174 |pmc=3763941 |pmid=23804014}}</ref>


== Clinical Applications ==
== Clinical Applications ==
Social support has been historically identified as very important for people’s well-being, and it can be even more important for populations that are vulnerable to high stress and [[loneliness]]. Work on the social buffering hypothesis has been done on these populations, which include racial and ethnic [[Minority group|minorities]], sexual minorities, middle-aged and elderly, impoverished individuals, and other adversely affective demographic groups. This type of work aims to find specific applications of social buffering, often to provide frameworks for developing and/or assessing effectiveness of treatments or build an understanding for prevention of the negative consequences of stressful life events.
Social support has been historically identified as very important for people’s well-being, and it can be even more important for populations that are vulnerable to high stress and [[loneliness]]. Work on the social buffering hypothesis has been done on these populations, which include racial and ethnic [[Minority group|minorities]], sexual minorities, middle-aged and elderly, impoverished individuals, and other adversely affective demographic groups. This type of work aims to find specific applications of social buffering, often to provide frameworks for developing and/or assessing effectiveness of treatments or build an understanding for prevention of the negative consequences of stressful life events.


A social buffering effect was observed in work done on [[Suicide|suicidality]], and findings suggest that focusing on buffering is a possible key focus in developing interventions.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Trujillo |first=Michael A. |last2=Perrin |first2=Paul B. |last3=Sutter |first3=Megan |last4=Tabaac |first4=Ariella |last5=Benotsch |first5=Eric G. |date=2017-01-02 |title=The buffering role of social support on the associations among discrimination, mental health, and suicidality in a transgender sample |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/15532739.2016.1247405 |journal=International Journal of Transgenderism |language=en |volume=18 |issue=1 |pages=39–52 |doi=10.1080/15532739.2016.1247405 |issn=1553-2739 |pmc=PMC5996383 |pmid=29904324}}</ref> The buffering effect has also been found to be strong in individuals with depression,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Dalgard |first=Odd Steffen |last2=Bj⊘rk |first2=Sven |last3=Tambs |first3=Kristian |date=1995-01 |title=Social Support, Negative Life Events and Mental Health |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/the-british-journal-of-psychiatry/article/abs/social-support-negative-life-events-and-mental-health/9025BB1DC3B9451EA962A14309BFE2F6 |journal=The British Journal of Psychiatry |language=en |volume=166 |issue=1 |pages=29–34 |doi=10.1192/bjp.166.1.29 |issn=0007-1250}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Takizawa |first=Tohru |last2=Kondo |first2=Tsuyoshi |last3=Sakihara |first3=Seizou |last4=Ariizumi |first4=Makoto |last5=Watanabe |first5=Naoki |last6=Oyama |first6=Hirofumi |date=2006-12 |title=Stress buffering effects of social support on depressive symptoms in middle age: Reciprocity and community mental health |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01579.x |journal=Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences |language=en |volume=60 |issue=6 |pages=652–661 |doi=10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01579.x |issn=1323-1316}}</ref> meaning that social support can reduce symptoms of depression under times of stress. A relationship has also been drawn between social buffering and drug and alcohol use disorders, which lower likelihood of developing substance abuse disorders with higher social support.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Stockdale |first=Susan E. |last2=Wells |first2=Kenneth B. |last3=Tang |first3=Lingqi |last4=Belin |first4=Thomas R. |last5=Zhang |first5=Lily |last6=Sherbourne |first6=Cathy D. |date=2007-11-01 |title=The importance of social context: Neighborhood stressors, stress-buffering mechanisms, and alcohol, drug, and mental health disorders |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0277953607003024 |journal=Social Science & Medicine |series=Placing Health in Context |volume=65 |issue=9 |pages=1867–1881 |doi=10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.045 |issn=0277-9536}}</ref>
A social buffering effect was observed in work done on [[Suicide|suicidality]], and findings suggest that focusing on buffering is a possible key focus in developing interventions.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Trujillo |first=Michael A. |last2=Perrin |first2=Paul B. |last3=Sutter |first3=Megan |last4=Tabaac |first4=Ariella |last5=Benotsch |first5=Eric G. |date=2017-01-02 |title=The buffering role of social support on the associations among discrimination, mental health, and suicidality in a transgender sample |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/15532739.2016.1247405 |journal=International Journal of Transgenderism |language=en |volume=18 |issue=1 |pages=39–52 |doi=10.1080/15532739.2016.1247405 |issn=1553-2739 |pmc=5996383 |pmid=29904324}}</ref> The buffering effect has also been found to be strong in individuals with depression,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Dalgard |first=Odd Steffen |last2=Bj⊘rk |first2=Sven |last3=Tambs |first3=Kristian |date=January 1995 |title=Social Support, Negative Life Events and Mental Health |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/the-british-journal-of-psychiatry/article/abs/social-support-negative-life-events-and-mental-health/9025BB1DC3B9451EA962A14309BFE2F6 |journal=The British Journal of Psychiatry |language=en |volume=166 |issue=1 |pages=29–34 |doi=10.1192/bjp.166.1.29 |issn=0007-1250}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Takizawa |first=Tohru |last2=Kondo |first2=Tsuyoshi |last3=Sakihara |first3=Seizou |last4=Ariizumi |first4=Makoto |last5=Watanabe |first5=Naoki |last6=Oyama |first6=Hirofumi |date=December 2006 |title=Stress buffering effects of social support on depressive symptoms in middle age: Reciprocity and community mental health |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01579.x |journal=Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences |language=en |volume=60 |issue=6 |pages=652–661 |doi=10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01579.x |issn=1323-1316}}</ref> meaning that social support can reduce symptoms of depression under times of stress. A relationship has also been drawn between social buffering and drug and alcohol use disorders, which lower likelihood of developing substance abuse disorders with higher social support.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Stockdale |first=Susan E. |last2=Wells |first2=Kenneth B. |last3=Tang |first3=Lingqi |last4=Belin |first4=Thomas R. |last5=Zhang |first5=Lily |last6=Sherbourne |first6=Cathy D. |date=2007-11-01 |title=The importance of social context: Neighborhood stressors, stress-buffering mechanisms, and alcohol, drug, and mental health disorders |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0277953607003024 |journal=Social Science & Medicine |series=Placing Health in Context |volume=65 |issue=9 |pages=1867–1881 |doi=10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.045 |issn=0277-9536}}</ref>


Another issue that social buffering relates to is loneliness. The United States has a growing rate of loneliness, some consider it a “loneliness epidemic”,<ref>{{Cite web |last=Health (OASH) |first=Office of the Assistant Secretary for |date=2023-05-03 |title=New Surgeon General Advisory Raises Alarm about the Devastating Impact of the Epidemic of Loneliness and Isolation in the United States |url=https://www.hhs.gov/about/news/2023/05/03/new-surgeon-general-advisory-raises-alarm-about-devastating-impact-epidemic-loneliness-isolation-united-states.html |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=www.hhs.gov |language=en}}</ref> and there is a documented rise in the number of people living alone in many cultures globally.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schafer |first=Markus H |last2=Sun |first2=Haosen |last3=Lee |first3=Jin A |date=2021-11-30 |title=Compensatory Connections? Living Alone, Loneliness, and the Buffering Role of Social Connection Among Older American and European Adults |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |journal=The Journals of Gerontology: Series B |volume=77 |issue=8 |pages=1550–1560 |doi=10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |issn=1079-5014}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |date=February 10, 2022 |title=Increased loneliness has become a global public health issue |url=https://www.openaccessgovernment.org/loneliness-health-countries |website=Open Access Government}}</ref> Loneliness is strongly linked with many psychiatric disorders, including [[Depression (mood)|depression]] and [[Anxiety disorder|anxiety]], as well as several physical disorders, such as [[Cardiovascular disease|cardiovascular diseases]] and [[hypertension]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Mushtaq |first=Raheel |date=2014 |title=Relationship Between Loneliness, Psychiatric Disorders and Physical Health ? A Review on the Psychological Aspects of Loneliness |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.7860/jcdr/2014/10077.4828 |journal=JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND DIAGNOSTIC RESEARCH |doi=10.7860/jcdr/2014/10077.4828 |issn=2249-782X}}</ref> Additionally, loneliness increases with age and has serious health consequences particularly in older populations.<ref>{{Cite web |date=2022-12-08 |title=Loneliness and Social Isolation Linked to Serious Health Conditions |url=https://www.cdc.gov/aging/publications/features/lonely-older-adults.html |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=www.cdc.gov |language=en-us}}</ref> Research shows that high levels of social connectedness can help alleviate negative effects of loneliness that frequently accompany getting older.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schafer |first=Markus H |last2=Sun |first2=Haosen |last3=Lee |first3=Jin A |date=2021-11-30 |title=Compensatory Connections? Living Alone, Loneliness, and the Buffering Role of Social Connection Among Older American and European Adults |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |journal=The Journals of Gerontology: Series B |volume=77 |issue=8 |pages=1550–1560 |doi=10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |issn=1079-5014}}</ref> The robust connection between loneliness and poor mental and physical health is difficult to debate, and social buffering research can highlight the specific aspects of loneliness that are most damaging.
Another issue that social buffering relates to is loneliness. The United States has a growing rate of loneliness, some consider it a “loneliness epidemic”,<ref>{{Cite web |publisher=Office of the Assistant Secretary for Health |date=2023-05-03 |title=New Surgeon General Advisory Raises Alarm about the Devastating Impact of the Epidemic of Loneliness and Isolation in the United States |url=https://www.hhs.gov/about/news/2023/05/03/new-surgeon-general-advisory-raises-alarm-about-devastating-impact-epidemic-loneliness-isolation-united-states.html |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=www.hhs.gov |language=en}}</ref> and there is a documented rise in the number of people living alone in many cultures globally.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schafer |first=Markus H |last2=Sun |first2=Haosen |last3=Lee |first3=Jin A |date=2021-11-30 |title=Compensatory Connections? Living Alone, Loneliness, and the Buffering Role of Social Connection Among Older American and European Adults |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |journal=The Journals of Gerontology: Series B |volume=77 |issue=8 |pages=1550–1560 |doi=10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |issn=1079-5014}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |date=February 10, 2022 |title=Increased loneliness has become a global public health issue |url=https://www.openaccessgovernment.org/loneliness-health-countries |website=Open Access Government}}</ref> Loneliness is strongly linked with many psychiatric disorders, including [[Depression (mood)|depression]] and [[Anxiety disorder|anxiety]], as well as several physical disorders, such as [[Cardiovascular disease|cardiovascular diseases]] and [[hypertension]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Mushtaq |first=Raheel |date=2014 |title=Relationship Between Loneliness, Psychiatric Disorders and Physical Health ? A Review on the Psychological Aspects of Loneliness |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.7860/jcdr/2014/10077.4828 |journal=Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research |doi=10.7860/jcdr/2014/10077.4828 |issn=2249-782X}}</ref> Additionally, loneliness increases with age and has serious health consequences particularly in older populations.<ref>{{Cite web |date=2022-12-08 |title=Loneliness and Social Isolation Linked to Serious Health Conditions |url=https://www.cdc.gov/aging/publications/features/lonely-older-adults.html |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=www.cdc.gov |language=en-us}}</ref> Research shows that high levels of social connectedness can help alleviate negative effects of loneliness that frequently accompany getting older.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Schafer |first=Markus H |last2=Sun |first2=Haosen |last3=Lee |first3=Jin A |date=2021-11-30 |title=Compensatory Connections? Living Alone, Loneliness, and the Buffering Role of Social Connection Among Older American and European Adults |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |journal=The Journals of Gerontology: Series B |volume=77 |issue=8 |pages=1550–1560 |doi=10.1093/geronb/gbab217 |issn=1079-5014}}</ref> The robust connection between loneliness and poor mental and physical health is difficult to debate, and social buffering research can highlight the specific aspects of loneliness that are most damaging.


Social buffering is also relevant to the process of [[acculturation]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lee |first=Jee-Sook |last2=Koeske |first2=Gary F. |last3=Sales |first3=Esther |date=2004-09-01 |title=Social support buffering of acculturative stress: a study of mental health symptoms among Korean international students |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0147176704000446 |journal=International Journal of Intercultural Relations |volume=28 |issue=5 |pages=399–414 |doi=10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.08.005 |issn=0147-1767}}</ref> Immigrants, guest workers, and international students my experience increased likelihood of isolation and experiencing psychological difficulty and consequences, such as depressive symptoms.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Jang |first=Heejung |last2=Tang |first2=Fengyan |date=2021-12-15 |title=Loneliness, age at immigration, family relationships, and depression among older immigrants: A moderated relationship |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02654075211061279 |journal=Journal of Social and Personal Relationships |volume=39 |issue=6 |pages=1602–1622 |doi=10.1177/02654075211061279 |issn=0265-4075}}</ref> Research shows that those who have more interpersonal connections and participate in acculturation at higher degrees, benefit from the effects of social buffering.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lee |first=Jee-Sook |last2=Koeske |first2=Gary F. |last3=Sales |first3=Esther |date=2004-09-01 |title=Social support buffering of acculturative stress: a study of mental health symptoms among Korean international students |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0147176704000446 |journal=International Journal of Intercultural Relations |volume=28 |issue=5 |pages=399–414 |doi=10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.08.005 |issn=0147-1767}}</ref> Increasing the size of one’s social network has been shown to have salient buffering effects particularly in older immigrants as well.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Jang |first=Heejung |last2=Tang |first2=Fengyan |date=2021-12-15 |title=Loneliness, age at immigration, family relationships, and depression among older immigrants: A moderated relationship |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02654075211061279 |journal=Journal of Social and Personal Relationships |volume=39 |issue=6 |pages=1602–1622 |doi=10.1177/02654075211061279 |issn=0265-4075}}</ref>
Social buffering is also relevant to the process of [[acculturation]].<ref name=jsl>{{Cite journal |last=Lee |first=Jee-Sook |last2=Koeske |first2=Gary F. |last3=Sales |first3=Esther |date=2004-09-01 |title=Social support buffering of acculturative stress: a study of mental health symptoms among Korean international students |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0147176704000446 |journal=International Journal of Intercultural Relations |volume=28 |issue=5 |pages=399–414 |doi=10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.08.005 |issn=0147-1767}}</ref> Immigrants, guest workers, and international students my experience increased likelihood of isolation and experiencing psychological difficulty and consequences, such as depressive symptoms.<ref name=hj>{{Cite journal |last=Jang |first=Heejung |last2=Tang |first2=Fengyan |date=2021-12-15 |title=Loneliness, age at immigration, family relationships, and depression among older immigrants: A moderated relationship |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02654075211061279 |journal=Journal of Social and Personal Relationships |volume=39 |issue=6 |pages=1602–1622 |doi=10.1177/02654075211061279 |issn=0265-4075}}</ref> Research shows that those who have more interpersonal connections and participate in acculturation at higher degrees, benefit from the effects of social buffering.<ref name=jsl/> Increasing the size of one’s social network has been shown to have salient buffering effects particularly in older immigrants as well.<ref name=hj/>


It Is important to note that social buffering works differently in different groups in society.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kawachi |first=Ichiro |last2=Berkman |first2=Lisa F. |date=2001-09-01 |title=Social ties and mental health |url=https://doi.org/10.1093/jurban/78.3.458 |journal=Journal of Urban Health |language=en |volume=78 |issue=3 |pages=458–467 |doi=10.1093/jurban/78.3.458 |issn=1468-2869 |pmc=PMC3455910 |pmid=11564849}}</ref> There are gender, age, and cultural differences.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=Michael B. |last2=Kaiser |first2=Sylvia |last3=Sachser |first3=Norbert |date=2009-10-01 |title=Social buffering of the stress response: Diversity, mechanisms, and functions |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0091302209000405 |journal=Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology |series=Hormones & Social Behavior |volume=30 |issue=4 |pages=470–482 |doi=10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001 |issn=0091-3022}}</ref> Additionally, it can be difficult to study the effects of social support on stress in individuals who have impaired social functioning.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ditzen |first=Beate |last2=Heinrichs |first2=Markus |date=2014 |title=Psychobiology of social support: The social dimension of stress buffering |url=https://www.medra.org/servlet/aliasResolver?alias=iospress&doi=10.3233/RNN-139008 |journal=Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience |volume=32 |issue=1 |pages=149–162 |doi=10.3233/RNN-139008}}</ref> These include individuals with [[Autism spectrum|autism spectrum disorders]], social phobias, and [[social anxiety disorder]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Reinelt |first=Eva |last2=Aldinger |first2=Maren |last3=Stopsack |first3=Malte |last4=Schwahn |first4=Christian |last5=John |first5=Ulrich |last6=Baumeister |first6=Sebastian E. |last7=Grabe |first7=Hans Jörgen |last8=Barnow |first8=Sven |date=2014-08-01 |title=High social support buffers the effects of 5-HTTLPR genotypes within social anxiety disorder |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0481-5 |journal=European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience |language=en |volume=264 |issue=5 |pages=433–439 |doi=10.1007/s00406-013-0481-5 |issn=1433-8491}}</ref>
It Is important to note that social buffering works differently in different groups in society.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kawachi |first=Ichiro |last2=Berkman |first2=Lisa F. |date=2001-09-01 |title=Social ties and mental health |url=https://doi.org/10.1093/jurban/78.3.458 |journal=Journal of Urban Health |language=en |volume=78 |issue=3 |pages=458–467 |doi=10.1093/jurban/78.3.458 |issn=1468-2869 |pmc=3455910 |pmid=11564849}}</ref> There are gender, age, and cultural differences.<ref name=mbh/> Additionally, it can be difficult to study the effects of social support on stress in individuals who have impaired social functioning.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ditzen |first=Beate |last2=Heinrichs |first2=Markus |date=2014 |title=Psychobiology of social support: The social dimension of stress buffering |url=https://www.medra.org/servlet/aliasResolver?alias=iospress&doi=10.3233/RNN-139008 |journal=Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience |volume=32 |issue=1 |pages=149–162 |doi=10.3233/RNN-139008}}</ref> These include individuals with [[Autism spectrum|autism spectrum disorders]], social phobias, and [[social anxiety disorder]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Reinelt |first=Eva |last2=Aldinger |first2=Maren |last3=Stopsack |first3=Malte |last4=Schwahn |first4=Christian |last5=John |first5=Ulrich |last6=Baumeister |first6=Sebastian E. |last7=Grabe |first7=Hans Jörgen |last8=Barnow |first8=Sven |date=2014-08-01 |title=High social support buffers the effects of 5-HTTLPR genotypes within social anxiety disorder |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0481-5 |journal=European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience |language=en |volume=264 |issue=5 |pages=433–439 |doi=10.1007/s00406-013-0481-5 |issn=1433-8491}}</ref>


== Development ==
== Development ==
Social buffering is recognized as an essential way through which childhood experiences affect [[Developmental psychology|development]] and subsequent health.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hostinar |first=Camelia E. |last2=Sullivan |first2=Regina M. |last3=Gunnar |first3=Megan R. |date=2014-01 |title=Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032671 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=140 |issue=1 |pages=256–282 |doi=10.1037/a0032671 |issn=1939-1455}}</ref>
Social buffering is recognized as an essential way through which childhood experiences affect [[Developmental psychology|development]] and subsequent health.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hostinar |first=Camelia E. |last2=Sullivan |first2=Regina M. |last3=Gunnar |first3=Megan R. |date=January 2014 |title=Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032671 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=140 |issue=1 |pages=256–282 |doi=10.1037/a0032671 |issn=1939-1455}}</ref>


During infancy, parents play a large role in regulating the negative consequences of childhood, especially regarding [[fear]] or [[pain]] responses.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Hostinar |first2=Camelia E. |date=2015-09-03 |title=The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=479–488 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |issn=1747-0919 |pmc=PMC4618716 |pmid=26230646}}</ref> Attachment is also very relevant to studies on development and stress reduction. [[Attachment theory]] posits that the type of attachment relationship children form with their parents influences the ability to [[Emotional self-regulation|regulate emotional]] states whether or not the parents are immediately present.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Hostinar |first2=Camelia E. |date=2015-09-03 |title=The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=479–488 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |issn=1747-0919 |pmc=PMC4618716 |pmid=26230646}}</ref> Stress buffering effects are often seen with securely attached children,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Brodersen |first2=Laurie |last3=Nachmias |first3=Melissa |last4=Buss |first4=Kristin |last5=Rigatuso |first5=Joseph |date=1996-04 |title=Stress reactivity and attachment security |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::aid-dev1>3.0.co;2-m |journal=Developmental Psychobiology |volume=29 |issue=3 |pages=191–204 |doi=10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::aid-dev1>3.0.co;2-m |issn=0012-1630}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Hostinar |first2=Camelia E. |date=2015-09-03 |title=The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=479–488 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |issn=1747-0919 |pmc=PMC4618716 |pmid=26230646}}</ref> indicating that the type and stability of relationships is crucial to how well a child recovers from stressful events.
During infancy, parents play a large role in regulating the negative consequences of childhood, especially regarding [[fear]] or [[pain]] responses.<ref name=mrg>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Hostinar |first2=Camelia E. |date=2015-09-03 |title=The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=479–488 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |issn=1747-0919 |pmc=4618716 |pmid=26230646}}</ref> Attachment is also very relevant to studies on development and stress reduction. [[Attachment theory]] posits that the type of attachment relationship children form with their parents influences the ability to [[Emotional self-regulation|regulate emotional]] states whether or not the parents are immediately present.<ref name=mrg/> Stress buffering effects are often seen with securely attached children,<ref name=mrg/><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Brodersen |first2=Laurie |last3=Nachmias |first3=Melissa |last4=Buss |first4=Kristin |last5=Rigatuso |first5=Joseph |date=April 1996 |title=Stress reactivity and attachment security |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::aid-dev1>3.0.co;2-m |journal=Developmental Psychobiology |volume=29 |issue=3 |pages=191–204 |doi=10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::aid-dev1>3.0.co;2-m |issn=0012-1630}}</ref> indicating that the type and stability of relationships is crucial to how well a child recovers from stressful events.


Some research shows that parents are especially important for social buffering up until around [[puberty]] or late childhood,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Hostinar |first2=Camelia E. |date=2015-09-03 |title=The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=479–488 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |issn=1747-0919 |pmc=PMC4618716 |pmid=26230646}}</ref> when primary caregivers tend to become less influential than peers in social settings.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Brown |first=B. Bradford |last2=Larson |first2=James |date=2009-10-30 |title=Peer Relationships in Adolescence |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy002004 |journal=Handbook of Adolescent Psychology |doi=10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy002004}}</ref> This does not take away from the role that parents play in social buffering as a whole, and instead reflects how they are replaced by friends who become the source of social support with buffering. Furthermore, major stressors during adolescence and puberty tend to be peers and social standing, so the social buffering provided by friends during this time is heavily interwoven with [[Social network|social networks]] and relates to an idea called the friendship protection hypothesis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Hostinar |first2=Camelia E. |date=2015-09-03 |title=The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants |url=http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=10 |issue=5 |pages=479–488 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747 |issn=1747-0919 |pmc=PMC4618716 |pmid=26230646}}</ref> The friendship protection hypothesis reflects the idea of the social buffering hypothesis and explains how children with at least one supportive friend are less negatively affected by [[bullying]] and peer rejection.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kendrick |first=Kristin |last2=Jutengren |first2=Göran |last3=Stattin |first3=Håkan |date=2012-03-29 |title=The protective role of supportive friends against bullying perpetration and victimization |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014 |journal=Journal of Adolescence |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=1069–1080 |doi=10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014 |issn=0140-1971}}</ref>
Some research shows that parents are especially important for social buffering up until around [[puberty]] or late childhood,<ref name=mrg/> when primary caregivers tend to become less influential than peers in social settings.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Brown |first=B. Bradford |last2=Larson |first2=James |date=2009-10-30 |title=Peer Relationships in Adolescence |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy002004 |journal=Handbook of Adolescent Psychology |doi=10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy002004}}</ref> This does not take away from the role that parents play in social buffering as a whole, and instead reflects how they are replaced by friends who become the source of social support with buffering. Furthermore, major stressors during adolescence and puberty tend to be peers and social standing, so the social buffering provided by friends during this time is heavily interwoven with [[Social network|social networks]] and relates to an idea called the friendship protection hypothesis.<ref name=mrg/> The friendship protection hypothesis reflects the idea of the social buffering hypothesis and explains how children with at least one supportive friend are less negatively affected by [[bullying]] and peer rejection.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kendrick |first=Kristin |last2=Jutengren |first2=Göran |last3=Stattin |first3=Håkan |date=2012-03-29 |title=The protective role of supportive friends against bullying perpetration and victimization |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014 |journal=Journal of Adolescence |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=1069–1080 |doi=10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014 |issn=0140-1971}}</ref>


When looking at social buffering and development, a common approach to measuring stress responses involves looking at the [[Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis|HPA axis]]. The HPA axis is referred to as one of the primary hormonal stress systems.<ref>{{Citation |last=Aguilera |first=Greti |title=The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis and Neuroendocrine Responses to Stress |date=2012 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-375097-6.10008-3 |work=Handbook of Neuroendocrinology |pages=175–196 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Research looking at stress and social buffering in development consistently shows that parents play a role in shaping HPA axis function, which is evidenced in part how early social deprivation can lead to long term dysfunction of stress reactivity.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hostinar |first=Camelia E. |last2=Johnson |first2=Anna E. |last3=Gunnar |first3=Megan R. |date=2015-11 |title=Early social deprivation and the social buffering of cortisol stress responses in late childhood: An experimental study. |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000029 |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=51 |issue=11 |pages=1597–1608 |doi=10.1037/dev0000029 |issn=1939-0599}}</ref> Social buffering effects have also been observed in that the presence of a parent during a threatening event can lower or completely block the activation of the HPA axis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Sullivan |first2=Regina M. |date=2017-01-02 |title=The neurodevelopment of social buffering and fear learning: integration and crosstalk |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2016.1151824 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=1–7 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2016.1151824 |issn=1747-0919}}</ref>
When looking at social buffering and development, a common approach to measuring stress responses involves looking at the [[Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis|HPA axis]]. The HPA axis is referred to as one of the primary hormonal stress systems.<ref>{{Citation |last=Aguilera |first=Greti |title=The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis and Neuroendocrine Responses to Stress |date=2012 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-375097-6.10008-3 |work=Handbook of Neuroendocrinology |pages=175–196 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> Research looking at stress and social buffering in development consistently shows that parents play a role in shaping HPA axis function, which is evidenced in part how early social deprivation can lead to long term dysfunction of stress reactivity.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hostinar |first=Camelia E. |last2=Johnson |first2=Anna E. |last3=Gunnar |first3=Megan R. |date=November 2015 |title=Early social deprivation and the social buffering of cortisol stress responses in late childhood: An experimental study. |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000029 |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=51 |issue=11 |pages=1597–1608 |doi=10.1037/dev0000029 |issn=1939-0599}}</ref> Social buffering effects have also been observed in that the presence of a parent during a threatening event can lower or completely block the activation of the HPA axis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gunnar |first=Megan R. |last2=Sullivan |first2=Regina M. |date=2017-01-02 |title=The neurodevelopment of social buffering and fear learning: integration and crosstalk |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470919.2016.1151824 |journal=Social Neuroscience |language=en |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=1–7 |doi=10.1080/17470919.2016.1151824 |issn=1747-0919}}</ref>


Additional support for the social buffering hypothesis and [[social neuroscience]] involves [[fear conditioning]]. The [[amygdala]] has been recognized as an important part in the process of fear learning, and research has shown that children have reduced amygdala activity when around a parent.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gee |first=Dylan G. |last2=Humphreys |first2=Kathryn L. |last3=Flannery |first3=Jessica |last4=Goff |first4=Bonnie |last5=Telzer |first5=Eva H. |last6=Shapiro |first6=Mor |last7=Hare |first7=Todd A. |last8=Bookheimer |first8=Susan Y. |last9=Tottenham |first9=Nim |date=2013-03-06 |title=A Developmental Shift from Positive to Negative Connectivity in Human Amygdala–Prefrontal Circuitry |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3446-12.2013 |journal=The Journal of Neuroscience |volume=33 |issue=10 |pages=4584–4593 |doi=10.1523/jneurosci.3446-12.2013 |issn=0270-6474}}</ref> In addition to this, greater connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala was observed when a child viewed their mother compared to a stranger.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Tottenham |first=Nim |last2=Shapiro |first2=Mor |last3=Telzer |first3=Eva H. |last4=Humphreys |first4=Kathryn L. |date=2012-05 |title=Amygdala response to mother |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01128.x |journal=Developmental Science |language=en |volume=15 |issue=3 |pages=307–319 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01128.x |issn=1363-755X |pmc=PMC3522470 |pmid=22490172}}</ref> The prefrontal cortex is involved in emotional processes so communication between the two brain areas suggests that parents play a large role in emotional regulation and provide neurological support for social buffering.
Additional support for the social buffering hypothesis and [[social neuroscience]] involves [[fear conditioning]]. The [[amygdala]] has been recognized as an important part in the process of fear learning, and research has shown that children have reduced amygdala activity when around a parent.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gee |first=Dylan G. |last2=Humphreys |first2=Kathryn L. |last3=Flannery |first3=Jessica |last4=Goff |first4=Bonnie |last5=Telzer |first5=Eva H. |last6=Shapiro |first6=Mor |last7=Hare |first7=Todd A. |last8=Bookheimer |first8=Susan Y. |last9=Tottenham |first9=Nim |date=2013-03-06 |title=A Developmental Shift from Positive to Negative Connectivity in Human Amygdala–Prefrontal Circuitry |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3446-12.2013 |journal=The Journal of Neuroscience |volume=33 |issue=10 |pages=4584–4593 |doi=10.1523/jneurosci.3446-12.2013 |issn=0270-6474}}</ref> In addition to this, greater connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala was observed when a child viewed their mother compared to a stranger.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Tottenham |first=Nim |last2=Shapiro |first2=Mor |last3=Telzer |first3=Eva H. |last4=Humphreys |first4=Kathryn L. |date=May 2012 |title=Amygdala response to mother |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01128.x |journal=Developmental Science |language=en |volume=15 |issue=3 |pages=307–319 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01128.x |issn=1363-755X |pmc=3522470 |pmid=22490172}}</ref> The prefrontal cortex is involved in emotional processes so communication between the two brain areas suggests that parents play a large role in emotional regulation and provide neurological support for social buffering.


Although stress processes and fear learning are evolutionarily crucial to learn, research on social buffering shows that social support can reduce the negative developmental consequences of too high stress and potentially aid with proper biological functioning.
Although stress processes and fear learning are evolutionarily crucial to learn, research on social buffering shows that social support can reduce the negative developmental consequences of too high stress and potentially aid with proper biological functioning.
Line 94: Line 94:
Phenomena consistent with the social buffering hypothesis have been widely observed in non-human animals. The literature on animals uses the word conspecifics to refer to animals of the same species, and in each case it is clear that the effects are seen between like-animals and not due to the presence of other beings in general.
Phenomena consistent with the social buffering hypothesis have been widely observed in non-human animals. The literature on animals uses the word conspecifics to refer to animals of the same species, and in each case it is clear that the effects are seen between like-animals and not due to the presence of other beings in general.


Social interaction and communication are very important for survival in many species of animals, aiding with cooperation and essential for protection from threats.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kikusui |first=Takefumi |last2=Winslow |first2=James T |last3=Mori |first3=Yuji |date=2006-12-29 |title=Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety |url=https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |language=en |volume=361 |issue=1476 |pages=2215–2228 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |issn=0962-8436 |pmc=PMC1764848 |pmid=17118934}}</ref> Highly social animals, such as [[Primate|non-human primates]], [[Rodent|rodents]], and [[Bird|birds]], show positive physiological and psychological effects when they are together with animals of the same species.<ref>{{Citation |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |title=Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering |date=2018 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-805397-3.00011-5 |work=Neuronal Correlates of Empathy |pages=137–149 |access-date=2023-11-27 |publisher=Elsevier}}</ref> This was an idea that gained more attention in the early 2000’s, while many of the experiments conducted in earlier research focused on the stress-inducing factors of social interaction.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kikusui |first=Takefumi |last2=Winslow |first2=James T |last3=Mori |first3=Yuji |date=2006-12-29 |title=Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety |url=https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |language=en |volume=361 |issue=1476 |pages=2215–2228 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |issn=0962-8436 |pmc=PMC1764848 |pmid=17118934}}</ref>
Social interaction and communication are very important for survival in many species of animals, aiding with cooperation and essential for protection from threats.<ref name=tk>{{Cite journal |last=Kikusui |first=Takefumi |last2=Winslow |first2=James T |last3=Mori |first3=Yuji |date=2006-12-29 |title=Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety |url=https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |language=en |volume=361 |issue=1476 |pages=2215–2228 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |issn=0962-8436 |pmc=1764848 |pmid=17118934}}</ref> Highly social animals, such as [[Primate|non-human primates]], [[Rodent|rodents]], and [[Bird|birds]], show positive physiological and psychological effects when they are together with animals of the same species.<ref name=yk/> This was an idea that gained more attention in the early 2000’s, while many of the experiments conducted in earlier research focused on the stress-inducing factors of social interaction.<ref name=tk/>


Social buffering has been done on a wide range of animals, including guinea pigs,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=Michael B. |last2=Zate |first2=Ryan |last3=Maken |first3=Deborah S. |date=2008-03 |title=Social buffering of the cortisol response of adult female guinea pigs |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.005 |journal=Physiology &amp; Behavior |volume=93 |issue=4-5 |pages=883–888 |doi=10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.005 |issn=0031-9384}}</ref> horses,<ref>{{Cite web |last=Ricci-Bonot |first=Claire |last2=Romero |first2=Teresa |last3=Nicol |first3=Christine |last4=Mills |first4=Daniel |date=2021-02-26 |title=Social buffering in horses (Equus caballus): Influence of context, companion familiarity and companion habituation |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-266932/v1 |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=dx.doi.org}}</ref> rhesus monkeys,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gust |first=Deborah A. |last2=Gordon |first2=Thomas P. |last3=Hambright |first3=M.Karen |last4=Wilson |first4=Mark E. |date=1993-09 |title=Relationship between Social Factors and Pituitary-Adrenocortical Activity in Female Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1993.1024 |journal=Hormones and Behavior |volume=27 |issue=3 |pages=318–331 |doi=10.1006/hbeh.1993.1024 |issn=0018-506X}}</ref> and pigs.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=McGlone |first=John J. |last2=Stansbury |first2=Wayne F. |last3=Tribble |first3=Leland F. |date=1986-09-01 |title=Aerosolized 5α-androst-16-en-3-one Reduced Agonistic Behavior and Temporarily Improved Performance of Growing Pigs1 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas1986.633679x |journal=Journal of Animal Science |volume=63 |issue=3 |pages=679–684 |doi=10.2527/jas1986.633679x |issn=0021-8812}}</ref> Studies have found support for social buffering both from a physiological, in the form of reduced HPA activity or lower cortisol levels, and behavioral perspective. Behavioral observations in rats, for example, include increased locomotor activity, indicating less fear, in a stress inducing open space in the presence of other rats, and an increase in the seeking of the proximity of other rats when they were exposed to stress.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kikusui |first=Takefumi |last2=Winslow |first2=James T |last3=Mori |first3=Yuji |date=2006-12-29 |title=Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety |url=https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |language=en |volume=361 |issue=1476 |pages=2215–2228 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2006.1941 |issn=0962-8436 |pmc=PMC1764848 |pmid=17118934}}</ref>
Social buffering has been done on a wide range of animals, including guinea pigs,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hennessy |first=Michael B. |last2=Zate |first2=Ryan |last3=Maken |first3=Deborah S. |date=March 2008 |title=Social buffering of the cortisol response of adult female guinea pigs |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.005 |journal=Physiology &amp; Behavior |volume=93 |issue=4-5 |pages=883–888 |doi=10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.005 |issn=0031-9384}}</ref> horses,<ref>{{Cite web |last=Ricci-Bonot |first=Claire |last2=Romero |first2=Teresa |last3=Nicol |first3=Christine |last4=Mills |first4=Daniel |date=2021-02-26 |title=Social buffering in horses (Equus caballus): Influence of context, companion familiarity and companion habituation |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-266932/v1 |access-date=2023-11-27 |website=dx.doi.org}}</ref> rhesus monkeys,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gust |first=Deborah A. |last2=Gordon |first2=Thomas P. |last3=Hambright |first3=M.Karen |last4=Wilson |first4=Mark E. |date=September 1993 |title=Relationship between Social Factors and Pituitary-Adrenocortical Activity in Female Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1993.1024 |journal=Hormones and Behavior |volume=27 |issue=3 |pages=318–331 |doi=10.1006/hbeh.1993.1024 |issn=0018-506X}}</ref> and pigs.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=McGlone |first=John J. |last2=Stansbury |first2=Wayne F. |last3=Tribble |first3=Leland F. |date=1986-09-01 |title=Aerosolized 5α-androst-16-en-3-one Reduced Agonistic Behavior and Temporarily Improved Performance of Growing Pigs1 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas1986.633679x |journal=Journal of Animal Science |volume=63 |issue=3 |pages=679–684 |doi=10.2527/jas1986.633679x |issn=0021-8812}}</ref> Studies have found support for social buffering both from a physiological, in the form of reduced HPA activity or lower cortisol levels, and behavioral perspective. Behavioral observations in rats, for example, include increased locomotor activity, indicating less fear, in a stress inducing open space in the presence of other rats, and an increase in the seeking of the proximity of other rats when they were exposed to stress.<ref name=tk/>


Research on animals has also revealed several sensory cues for social buffering:
Research on animals has also revealed several sensory cues for social buffering:


* Olfactory cues - Highly social animals use chemical communication to transmit certain information, such as emotional status in times of stress that alters both the physiological responses and behavior of animals.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kikusui |first=Takefumi |last2=Takigami |first2=Shu |last3=Takeuchi |first3=Yukari |last4=Mori |first4=Yuji |date=2001-01 |title=Alarm pheromone enhances stress-induced hyperthermia in rats |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00370-x |journal=Physiology &amp; Behavior |volume=72 |issue=1-2 |pages=45–50 |doi=10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00370-x |issn=0031-9384}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |last2=Kikusui |first2=Takefumi |last3=Takeuchi |first3=Yukari |last4=Mori |first4=Yuji |date=2005-05 |title=Mapping the neural circuit activated by alarm pheromone perception by c-Fos immunohistochemistry |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061 |journal=Brain Research |volume=1043 |issue=1-2 |pages=145–154 |doi=10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061 |issn=0006-8993}}</ref> Animals may also use appeasing pheromones to calm others down and reduce fighting behavior.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=McGlone |first=John J. |last2=Stansbury |first2=Wayne F. |last3=Tribble |first3=Leland F. |date=1986-09-01 |title=Aerosolized 5α-androst-16-en-3-one Reduced Agonistic Behavior and Temporarily Improved Performance of Growing Pigs1 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas1986.633679x |journal=Journal of Animal Science |volume=63 |issue=3 |pages=679–684 |doi=10.2527/jas1986.633679x |issn=0021-8812}}</ref> <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Guiraudie |first=G. |date=2003-09-01 |title=Functional Characterization of Olfactory Binding Proteins for Appeasing Compounds and Molecular Cloning in the Vomeronasal Organ of Pre-pubertal Pigs |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjg052 |journal=Chemical Senses |volume=28 |issue=7 |pages=609–619 |doi=10.1093/chemse/bjg052 |issn=1464-3553}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sheppard |first=G. |last2=Mills |first2=D. S. |date=2003-04 |title=Evaluation of dog‐appeasing pheromone as a potential treatment for dogs fearful of fireworks |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/vr.152.14.432 |journal=Veterinary Record |volume=152 |issue=14 |pages=432–436 |doi=10.1136/vr.152.14.432 |issn=0042-4900}}</ref> An example of this in humans is that maternal breast odor is both attractive to infants and has positive emotional effects. <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Porter |first=RichardH. |last2=Winberg |first2=Jan |date=1999-01 |title=Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00044-x |journal=Neuroscience &amp; Biobehavioral Reviews |volume=23 |issue=3 |pages=439–449 |doi=10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00044-x |issn=0149-7634}}</ref>
* Olfactory cues - Highly social animals use chemical communication to transmit certain information, such as emotional status in times of stress that alters both the physiological responses and behavior of animals.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kikusui |first=Takefumi |last2=Takigami |first2=Shu |last3=Takeuchi |first3=Yukari |last4=Mori |first4=Yuji |date=January 2001 |title=Alarm pheromone enhances stress-induced hyperthermia in rats |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00370-x |journal=Physiology &amp; Behavior |volume=72 |issue=1-2 |pages=45–50 |doi=10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00370-x |issn=0031-9384}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kiyokawa |first=Yasushi |last2=Kikusui |first2=Takefumi |last3=Takeuchi |first3=Yukari |last4=Mori |first4=Yuji |date=May 2005 |title=Mapping the neural circuit activated by alarm pheromone perception by c-Fos immunohistochemistry |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061 |journal=Brain Research |volume=1043 |issue=1-2 |pages=145–154 |doi=10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061 |issn=0006-8993}}</ref> Animals may also use appeasing pheromones to calm others down and reduce fighting behavior.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=McGlone |first=John J. |last2=Stansbury |first2=Wayne F. |last3=Tribble |first3=Leland F. |date=1986-09-01 |title=Aerosolized 5α-androst-16-en-3-one Reduced Agonistic Behavior and Temporarily Improved Performance of Growing Pigs1 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas1986.633679x |journal=Journal of Animal Science |volume=63 |issue=3 |pages=679–684 |doi=10.2527/jas1986.633679x |issn=0021-8812}}</ref> <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Guiraudie |first=G. |date=2003-09-01 |title=Functional Characterization of Olfactory Binding Proteins for Appeasing Compounds and Molecular Cloning in the Vomeronasal Organ of Pre-pubertal Pigs |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjg052 |journal=Chemical Senses |volume=28 |issue=7 |pages=609–619 |doi=10.1093/chemse/bjg052 |issn=1464-3553}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sheppard |first=G. |last2=Mills |first2=D. S. |date=April 2003 |title=Evaluation of dog‐appeasing pheromone as a potential treatment for dogs fearful of fireworks |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/vr.152.14.432 |journal=Veterinary Record |volume=152 |issue=14 |pages=432–436 |doi=10.1136/vr.152.14.432 |issn=0042-4900}}</ref> An example of this in humans is that maternal breast odor is both attractive to infants and has positive emotional effects.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Porter |first=RichardH. |last2=Winberg |first2=Jan |date=January 1999 |title=Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00044-x |journal=Neuroscience &amp; Biobehavioral Reviews |volume=23 |issue=3 |pages=439–449 |doi=10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00044-x |issn=0149-7634}}</ref>
* Vocal cues - Vocal communication can be used to indicate familiarity, emotions, and help with attachment formation. In marmosets, a species that use vocal communication, stress hormone levels decrease more when hearing a familiar voice than an unfamiliar one.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rukstalis |first=Michael |last2=French |first2=Jeffrey A. |date=2005-01 |title=Vocal buffering of the stress response: exposure to conspecific vocalizations moderates urinary cortisol excretion in isolated marmosets |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15579259/ |journal=Hormones and Behavior |volume=47 |issue=1 |pages=1–7 |doi=10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.004 |issn=0018-506X |pmc=2987614 |pmid=15579259}}</ref>
* Vocal cues - Vocal communication can be used to indicate familiarity, emotions, and help with attachment formation. In marmosets, a species that use vocal communication, stress hormone levels decrease more when hearing a familiar voice than an unfamiliar one.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rukstalis |first=Michael |last2=French |first2=Jeffrey A. |date=January 2005 |title=Vocal buffering of the stress response: exposure to conspecific vocalizations moderates urinary cortisol excretion in isolated marmosets |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15579259/ |journal=Hormones and Behavior |volume=47 |issue=1 |pages=1–7 |doi=10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.004 |issn=0018-506X |pmc=2987614 |pmid=15579259}}</ref>
* Visual cues - In certain animals, visual cues may be enough to evoke the effects of social buffering. These effects were shown in sheep, where seeing pictures of other sheep's faces reduced behavioral and physiological stress responses.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=da Costa |first=Ana P. |last2=Leigh |first2=Andrea E. |last3=Man |first3=Mei-See |last4=Kendrick |first4=Keith M. |date=2004-10-07 |title=Face pictures reduce behavioural, autonomic, endocrine and neural indices of stress and fear in sheep |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2831 |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences |volume=271 |issue=1552 |pages=2077–2084 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2004.2831 |issn=0962-8452}}</ref><br />
* Visual cues - In certain animals, visual cues may be enough to evoke the effects of social buffering. These effects were shown in sheep, where seeing pictures of other sheep's faces reduced behavioral and physiological stress responses.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=da Costa |first=Ana P. |last2=Leigh |first2=Andrea E. |last3=Man |first3=Mei-See |last4=Kendrick |first4=Keith M. |date=2004-10-07 |title=Face pictures reduce behavioural, autonomic, endocrine and neural indices of stress and fear in sheep |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2831 |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences |volume=271 |issue=1552 |pages=2077–2084 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2004.2831 |issn=0962-8452}}</ref>

=== References ===
=== References ===



Revision as of 14:52, 27 November 2023

In (social) psychology, social buffering is a phenomenon explaining that social connections can alleviate negative consequences of stressful events.

Although there are other models and theories to describe how social support can help reduce individuals’ stress responses, social buffering hypothesis is one of the dominant ones. According to this idea social partners, who can be familiar others or conspecifics, act as buffers in the face of stressful events, specifically while the stress is happening. The model further describes that social support is especially beneficial when levels of stress are also high, but buffering effects are not as relevant when levels of stress are low.

Social buffering has been explored in humans and other social animals, and is important to questions about physical and mental health.

Research has attempted to gain insight about the protective effects of social support in several domains, such as biological, developmental, neurological, and clinical settings.

Social buffering is also relevant to other psychological processes, including fear, social bonding, and emotional reactivity.

Background

Early History

Psychological research in the mid-twentieth century began to increasingly reveal the role of stressful life events on psychological well-being.[1] This was also around the time that there was a focus on creating standardized approach to diagnosing mental illnesses, with the first Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (commonly referred to as the DSM) being published in 1952.[2] With a honed focus on effective, universal ways to measure mental well-being, and the application of experimental psychology on social issues, a large literature on the effects of social support began to form. This occurred in an effort to fill in the gaps on the specific factors that mediate the relationship between life events and psychological consequences.[3]

Specific focus on the attenuation of social support on the negative impacts of stressful events on physical and mental health began in the mid-1970’s.[4] This is around the time when the idea of social buffering began to take shape.

It is unclear where the phrase social buffering hypothesis was first mentioned, but one of the most credited and cited works on the topic was published by researchers Sheldon Cohen and Thomas A. Wills in 1985.[5]

Social buffering has been an important feature in psychology since its early use, specifically relevant to social psychology. The framework has been applied to several other areas as well, and methods of measurement and definitions of relevant terminology continue to be refined and built upon.

Social Support

Social buffering is a subset of social support, and not all occurrences of social support are social buffering. Social support encompasses both the expectation and actual act of being assisted, nurtured, attended to, or participation in a social network.[6]

Research on social support makes the distinction between perceived support and received social support. Perceived social support refers to the amount of support people believe that they could potentially receive from their available support system, while received social support is the level of support that people have received.[7][8] Some studies have shown that perceived social support may be better for mitigating the negative effects of stress on health and psychological well-being,[9][10] meaning there is a measurable difference between the types of social support and proper definition of variables is important to research in the area.

Social support is robustly associated with positive health outcomes. Research has shown that people with larger social networks, higher quality relationships, and greater access to social support resources have better mental and physical health.[11] The effects of social support have been studied in many different domains of psychology, such as social, developmental, clinical, health, and neuroscience.[12][8][13][14]

Competing Model: Direct Effects Hypothesis

The social buffering hypothesis is often compared to or evaluated with the direct effect hypothesis.[15][16] This hypothesis differs from social buffering in that it suggests that social support enhances physical and psychological well-being regardless of the presence of stressors. This model says that social support is beneficial all the time, and that people with high social support have overall better health than those without it.[17]

The two models tend to deal with different measures of social support. The direct effects hypothesis measures the level at which a person is integrated into a social network, while the social buffering hypothesis assesses how available the social resources are that help people respond to stressful events.[15] The language around both hypotheses also tends to be different, with direct effects hypothesis often looking at the enhancement of health and well-being as a result of the perception of support and integration in a network, whereas the buffering hypothesis is more concerned with protection (or prevention), especially in times of need.[15]

Statistically, the direct effect hypothesis holds that there is no interaction between stress and social support,[18] meaning the same beneficial effects will be observed notwithstanding of the level of stress. Conversely, according to the social buffering hypothesis, the magnitude of the beneficial effect from social support is larger when stress is present, which is reflected in a statistically significant observable interaction when the two effects are studied experimentally.[15] This also means that knowledge of the degree of stress is required for the social buffering hypothesis, where this level not as relevant for the direct effects hypothesis.[18]

Despite these models providing somewhat separate explanations, research has found support for both hypotheses,[8] and some work even suggests that both processes happen simultaneously.[16] Researchers have suggested that work directed at critically comparing the two hypotheses may not actually benefit the field studying social support.[15] Instead it may be more beneficial to use either as a model that aims to explain specific questions about how social support relates to mediators of health, that can be behavioral, psychological, emotional, or biological.

Biology

A large proportion of research done on social buffering and health consequences shows that the HPA axis plays a central role in the link between the two.

The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is a crucial regulator of neuroendocrinal responses in the body. The HPA axis is made up of a series of pathways and feedback loops that involve the hypothalamus, anterior pituitary gland, and adrenal gland.[19] It modulates several physiological processes, including the autonomic nervous system, immune system reactions, metabolism, and several other processes that are active during short-term physiological responses to stress.[19] The HPA axis also plays a major role in bodily homeostasis, which includes regulating the cardiovascular system, reproductive system, and central nervous system in addition to those previously mentioned.[19]

Proper functioning of the HPA axis is very important for adaptation and development, and both over and under reactivity can lead to a series of consequences.[19] It is important for humans to experience high levels of circulating stress hormones early in life so that they can learn to effectively respond to threat and adapt to their environments.[20] However, too much stress in childhood can lead to long term and often irreversible dysfunction of the HPA axis.[21]

HPA axis activity goes up during aversive or arousing situations, which can be induced by physical or psychosocial events. The HPA axis is particularly sensitive to psychological stressors,[22] including uncertainty, novelty, and the feeling of being out of control.[23][24] In addition to being influenced by psychological stressors, one of the most powerful and widely studied moderators of HPA axis activity induced by stressful events is social support.[21] This is why the HPA axis is often a focal point in physiological research examining social buffering effects.

The biological mechanisms of social buffering have been explored, and several components have been identified as relevant:

Endocrine

There are several hormones involved in HPA axis endocrine regulation. One is corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF). CRF stimulates the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), which is the crucial physiological regulator of increased activity of the HPA axis.[25] Another is vasopressin (AVP), and glucocorticoids are the final step in the process.[26] Social buffering is observed when high levels of social support are correlated with lower levels of stress hormone and HPA axis activation.

Oxytocin

Oxytocin has been recognized as an important hormone involved in the mechanism of social buffering. Oxytocin is a molecule that is often called the “love hormone”.[27] It is released into the blood in response to physiological and psychological stress, and increased oxytocin release from the hypothalamus inhibits activation of the HPA axis.[28][29] Oxytocin is involved in stress processes by inhibiting the release of corticosteroids, ACTH release from the pituitary gland, and release of CRF from the hypothalamus.[25] The central nervous system also meditates the inhibitory effect of oxytocin on the HPA axis.[25] Oxytocin is also involved in the behavioral side of social buffering. Oxytocin's role in bonding means that it is related to the process of social support, in addition to being released in response to a stressor.[30]

Opioids

Endogenous opioids also appear to play a major role in social buffering, via a reinforcing effect that is active in social attachment.[25] Opioid release is also observed when animals recognize each other, which supports the idea that animals find social support in others of the same species. This is necessary to see social buffering effects in animals, and opioid release also helps explain the seeking of affiliation in animals as well. Opioid release as a result of social stimulation has also been found to reduce CRF activity in the brain and body.[25]

Neural Circuitry

Research on the brain regions involved in social buffering is less extensive as the HPA axis and cortisol.

However, the prefrontal cortex has been identified as a region involved in the social buffering and stress responses. Higher activity in the prefrontal cortex has been found to be correlated with higher degrees of cortisol responses to stress. A similar relationship has been observed in the anterior cingulate cortex.[14] Both the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex have been shown to be involved in emotional responses and reactivity to stressful situations,[31] and activity in these areas is closely related with HPA activation. The presence of social support causes cortisol levels to go down as well as decreased activity in these regions.

Social buffering has also been observed in regions associated with responding to safety, which include the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, posterior insula, and posterior cingulate cortex.[32]

Clinical Applications

Social support has been historically identified as very important for people’s well-being, and it can be even more important for populations that are vulnerable to high stress and loneliness. Work on the social buffering hypothesis has been done on these populations, which include racial and ethnic minorities, sexual minorities, middle-aged and elderly, impoverished individuals, and other adversely affective demographic groups. This type of work aims to find specific applications of social buffering, often to provide frameworks for developing and/or assessing effectiveness of treatments or build an understanding for prevention of the negative consequences of stressful life events.

A social buffering effect was observed in work done on suicidality, and findings suggest that focusing on buffering is a possible key focus in developing interventions.[33] The buffering effect has also been found to be strong in individuals with depression,[34][35] meaning that social support can reduce symptoms of depression under times of stress. A relationship has also been drawn between social buffering and drug and alcohol use disorders, which lower likelihood of developing substance abuse disorders with higher social support.[36]

Another issue that social buffering relates to is loneliness. The United States has a growing rate of loneliness, some consider it a “loneliness epidemic”,[37] and there is a documented rise in the number of people living alone in many cultures globally.[38][39] Loneliness is strongly linked with many psychiatric disorders, including depression and anxiety, as well as several physical disorders, such as cardiovascular diseases and hypertension.[40] Additionally, loneliness increases with age and has serious health consequences particularly in older populations.[41] Research shows that high levels of social connectedness can help alleviate negative effects of loneliness that frequently accompany getting older.[42] The robust connection between loneliness and poor mental and physical health is difficult to debate, and social buffering research can highlight the specific aspects of loneliness that are most damaging.

Social buffering is also relevant to the process of acculturation.[43] Immigrants, guest workers, and international students my experience increased likelihood of isolation and experiencing psychological difficulty and consequences, such as depressive symptoms.[44] Research shows that those who have more interpersonal connections and participate in acculturation at higher degrees, benefit from the effects of social buffering.[43] Increasing the size of one’s social network has been shown to have salient buffering effects particularly in older immigrants as well.[44]

It Is important to note that social buffering works differently in different groups in society.[45] There are gender, age, and cultural differences.[21] Additionally, it can be difficult to study the effects of social support on stress in individuals who have impaired social functioning.[46] These include individuals with autism spectrum disorders, social phobias, and social anxiety disorder.[47]

Development

Social buffering is recognized as an essential way through which childhood experiences affect development and subsequent health.[48]

During infancy, parents play a large role in regulating the negative consequences of childhood, especially regarding fear or pain responses.[49] Attachment is also very relevant to studies on development and stress reduction. Attachment theory posits that the type of attachment relationship children form with their parents influences the ability to regulate emotional states whether or not the parents are immediately present.[49] Stress buffering effects are often seen with securely attached children,[49][50] indicating that the type and stability of relationships is crucial to how well a child recovers from stressful events.

Some research shows that parents are especially important for social buffering up until around puberty or late childhood,[49] when primary caregivers tend to become less influential than peers in social settings.[51] This does not take away from the role that parents play in social buffering as a whole, and instead reflects how they are replaced by friends who become the source of social support with buffering. Furthermore, major stressors during adolescence and puberty tend to be peers and social standing, so the social buffering provided by friends during this time is heavily interwoven with social networks and relates to an idea called the friendship protection hypothesis.[49] The friendship protection hypothesis reflects the idea of the social buffering hypothesis and explains how children with at least one supportive friend are less negatively affected by bullying and peer rejection.[52]

When looking at social buffering and development, a common approach to measuring stress responses involves looking at the HPA axis. The HPA axis is referred to as one of the primary hormonal stress systems.[53] Research looking at stress and social buffering in development consistently shows that parents play a role in shaping HPA axis function, which is evidenced in part how early social deprivation can lead to long term dysfunction of stress reactivity.[54] Social buffering effects have also been observed in that the presence of a parent during a threatening event can lower or completely block the activation of the HPA axis.[55]

Additional support for the social buffering hypothesis and social neuroscience involves fear conditioning. The amygdala has been recognized as an important part in the process of fear learning, and research has shown that children have reduced amygdala activity when around a parent.[56] In addition to this, greater connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala was observed when a child viewed their mother compared to a stranger.[57] The prefrontal cortex is involved in emotional processes so communication between the two brain areas suggests that parents play a large role in emotional regulation and provide neurological support for social buffering.

Although stress processes and fear learning are evolutionarily crucial to learn, research on social buffering shows that social support can reduce the negative developmental consequences of too high stress and potentially aid with proper biological functioning.

Animal Research

Phenomena consistent with the social buffering hypothesis have been widely observed in non-human animals. The literature on animals uses the word conspecifics to refer to animals of the same species, and in each case it is clear that the effects are seen between like-animals and not due to the presence of other beings in general.

Social interaction and communication are very important for survival in many species of animals, aiding with cooperation and essential for protection from threats.[58] Highly social animals, such as non-human primates, rodents, and birds, show positive physiological and psychological effects when they are together with animals of the same species.[25] This was an idea that gained more attention in the early 2000’s, while many of the experiments conducted in earlier research focused on the stress-inducing factors of social interaction.[58]

Social buffering has been done on a wide range of animals, including guinea pigs,[59] horses,[60] rhesus monkeys,[61] and pigs.[62] Studies have found support for social buffering both from a physiological, in the form of reduced HPA activity or lower cortisol levels, and behavioral perspective. Behavioral observations in rats, for example, include increased locomotor activity, indicating less fear, in a stress inducing open space in the presence of other rats, and an increase in the seeking of the proximity of other rats when they were exposed to stress.[58]

Research on animals has also revealed several sensory cues for social buffering:

  • Olfactory cues - Highly social animals use chemical communication to transmit certain information, such as emotional status in times of stress that alters both the physiological responses and behavior of animals.[63][64] Animals may also use appeasing pheromones to calm others down and reduce fighting behavior.[65] [66][67] An example of this in humans is that maternal breast odor is both attractive to infants and has positive emotional effects.[68]
  • Vocal cues - Vocal communication can be used to indicate familiarity, emotions, and help with attachment formation. In marmosets, a species that use vocal communication, stress hormone levels decrease more when hearing a familiar voice than an unfamiliar one.[69]
  • Visual cues - In certain animals, visual cues may be enough to evoke the effects of social buffering. These effects were shown in sheep, where seeing pictures of other sheep's faces reduced behavioral and physiological stress responses.[70]

References

  1. ^ Thoits, Peggy A. (1982). "Conceptual, Methodological, and Theoretical Problems in Studying Social Support as a Buffer Against Life Stress". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 23 (2): 145–159. doi:10.2307/2136511. ISSN 0022-1465.
  2. ^ "DSM History". American Psychiatric Association.
  3. ^ Wilcox, Brian L. (August 1981). "Social support, life stress, and psychological adjustment: A test of the buffering hypothesis". American Journal of Community Psychology. 9 (4): 371–386. doi:10.1007/bf00918169. ISSN 0091-0562.
  4. ^ Lin, Nan; Woelfel, Mary W.; Light, Stephen C. (1985). "The Buffering Effect of Social Support Subsequent to an Important Life Event". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 26 (3): 247–263. doi:10.2307/2136756. ISSN 0022-1465.
  5. ^ Cohen, Sheldon; McKay, Garth (2020-08-31), "Social Support, Stress and the Buffering Hypothesis: A Theoretical Analysis", Handbook of Psychology and Health (Volume IV), Routledge, pp. 253–267, ISBN 978-1-003-04430-7, retrieved 2023-11-26
  6. ^ Gunnar, Megan R. (May 2017). "Social Buffering of Stress in Development: A Career Perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 12 (3): 355–373. doi:10.1177/1745691616680612. ISSN 1745-6916.
  7. ^ Helgeson, Vicki S. (May 1993). "Two Important Distinctions in Social Support: Kind of Support and Perceived Versus Received1". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 23 (10): 825–845. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01008.x. ISSN 0021-9029.
  8. ^ a b c Taylor, Shelley E. (2012-09-18), "Social Support: A Review", The Oxford Handbook of Health Psychology, Oxford University Press, pp. 190–214, ISBN 0-19-534281-X, retrieved 2023-11-27
  9. ^ Uchino, Bert N. (May 2009). "Understanding the Links Between Social Support and Physical Health: A Life-Span Perspective With Emphasis on the Separability of Perceived and Received Support". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 4 (3): 236–255. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x. ISSN 1745-6916.
  10. ^ Taylor, Shelley E.; Sherman, David K.; Kim, Heejung S.; Jarcho, Johanna; Takagi, Kaori; Dunagan, Melissa S. (2004). "Culture and Social Support: Who Seeks It and Why?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (3): 354–362. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.354. ISSN 1939-1315.
  11. ^ Hornstein, Erica A.; Eisenberger, Naomi I. (2017-05-02). Brown, Kirk Warren (ed.). "Unpacking the buffering effect of social support figures: Social support attenuates fear acquisition". PLOS ONE. 12 (5): e0175891. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0175891. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 5413011. PMID 28463999.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  12. ^ Schwarzer, Ralf; Leppin, Anja (January 1989). "Social support and health: A meta-analysis". Psychology & Health. 3 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1080/08870448908400361. ISSN 0887-0446.
  13. ^ Winemiller, David R.; Mitchell, M. Ellen; Sutliff, Jane; Cline, Daniel I. (September 1993). <638::aid-jclp2270490505>3.0.co;2-7 "Measurement strategies in social support: A descriptive review of the literature". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 49 (5): 638–648. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199309)49:5<638::aid-jclp2270490505>3.0.co;2-7. ISSN 0021-9762.
  14. ^ a b Eisenberger, Naomi I.; Taylor, Shelley E.; Gable, Shelly L.; Hilmert, Clayton J.; Lieberman, Matthew D. (2007-05-01). "Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses". NeuroImage. 35 (4): 1601–1612. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038. ISSN 1053-8119.
  15. ^ a b c d e Schaefer, Catherine (March 1985). "Book Reviews : Social Support and Health, Sheldon Cohen and S. Leonard Syme, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, 1984". Health Education Quarterly. 12 (1): 109–112. doi:10.1177/109019818501200110. ISSN 0195-8402.
  16. ^ a b Hobfoll, Stevan E., ed. (2014-03-18). "Stress, Social Support, And Women". doi:10.4324/9781315803128.
  17. ^ Cohen, Sheldon; Wills, Thomas A. (September 1985). "Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis". Psychological Bulletin. 98 (2): 310–357. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310. ISSN 1939-1455.
  18. ^ a b Whelan, Christopher T. (January 1993). "The role of social support in mediating the psychological consequences of economic stress". Sociology of Health & Illness. 15 (1): 86–101. doi:10.1111/1467-9566.ep11343797. ISSN 0141-9889.
  19. ^ a b c d Sheng, Julietta A.; Bales, Natalie J.; Myers, Sage A.; Bautista, Anna I.; Roueinfar, Mina; Hale, Taben M.; Handa, Robert J. (2021-01-13). "The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis: Development, Programming Actions of Hormones, and Maternal-Fetal Interactions". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 14. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601939. ISSN 1662-5153.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  20. ^ Sapolsky, R. M. (2000-02-01). "How Do Glucocorticoids Influence Stress Responses? Integrating Permissive, Suppressive, Stimulatory, and Preparative Actions". Endocrine Reviews. 21 (1): 55–89. doi:10.1210/er.21.1.55. ISSN 0163-769X.
  21. ^ a b c Hennessy, Michael B.; Kaiser, Sylvia; Sachser, Norbert (2009-10-01). "Social buffering of the stress response: Diversity, mechanisms, and functions". Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. Hormones & Social Behavior. 30 (4): 470–482. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001. ISSN 0091-3022.
  22. ^ Mason, John W. (June 1975). "A Historical View of the Stress Field". Journal of Human Stress. 1 (2): 22–36. doi:10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940405. ISSN 0097-840X.
  23. ^ Ketz, H.‐A. (January 1979). "J. P. Henry und P. M. Stephens: Stress, Health and the Social Environment, A Sociobiologic Approach to Medicine. 282 Seiten, Springer Verlag, New York, Heidelberg, Berlin 1977. Preis: 66,‐ DM, 30.40 $". Food / Nahrung. 23 (2): 196–196. doi:10.1002/food.19790230239. ISSN 0027-769X.
  24. ^ Hennessy, John W.; Smotherman, William P.; Levine, Seymour (April 1976). "Conditioned taste aversion and the pituitary-adrenal system". Behavioral Biology. 16 (4): 413–424. doi:10.1016/s0091-6773(76)91571-6. ISSN 0091-6773.
  25. ^ a b c d e f Kiyokawa, Yasushi (2018), "Relief From Stress Provided by Conspecifics: Social Buffering", Neuronal Correlates of Empathy, Elsevier, pp. 137–149, retrieved 2023-11-27
  26. ^ Aguilera, Greti (2012), "The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis and Neuroendocrine Responses to Stress", Handbook of Neuroendocrinology, Elsevier, pp. 175–196, retrieved 2023-11-27
  27. ^ "Oxytocin: The love hormone". Harvard Health. 2021-07-20. Retrieved 2023-11-27.
  28. ^ DeVries, A.Courtney; Glasper, Erica R; Detillion, Courtney E (August 2003). "Social modulation of stress responses". Physiology & Behavior. 79 (3): 399–407. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00152-5. ISSN 0031-9384.
  29. ^ Neumann, Inga D. (2002), "Chapter 12 Involvement of the brain oxytocin system in stress coping: interactions with the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis", Vasopressin and Oxytocin: From Genes to Clinical Applications, Elsevier, pp. 147–162, retrieved 2023-11-27
  30. ^ Crockford, Catherine; Deschner, Tobias; Wittig, Roman M. (2018). "The Role of Oxytocin in Social Buffering: What Do Primate Studies Add?". Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 35: 155–173. doi:10.1007/7854_2017_12. ISSN 1866-3370. PMID 28864973.
  31. ^ Eisenberger, Naomi I.; Lieberman, Matthew D.; Williams, Kipling D. (2003-10-10). "Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion". Science. 302 (5643): 290–292. doi:10.1126/science.1089134. ISSN 0036-8075.
  32. ^ Eisenberger, Naomi I. (July 2013). "An Empirical Review of the Neural Underpinnings of Receiving and Giving Social Support: Implications for Health". Psychosomatic Medicine. 75 (6): 545–556. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e31829de2e7. ISSN 0033-3174. PMC 3763941. PMID 23804014.
  33. ^ Trujillo, Michael A.; Perrin, Paul B.; Sutter, Megan; Tabaac, Ariella; Benotsch, Eric G. (2017-01-02). "The buffering role of social support on the associations among discrimination, mental health, and suicidality in a transgender sample". International Journal of Transgenderism. 18 (1): 39–52. doi:10.1080/15532739.2016.1247405. ISSN 1553-2739. PMC 5996383. PMID 29904324.
  34. ^ Dalgard, Odd Steffen; Bj⊘rk, Sven; Tambs, Kristian (January 1995). "Social Support, Negative Life Events and Mental Health". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 166 (1): 29–34. doi:10.1192/bjp.166.1.29. ISSN 0007-1250.
  35. ^ Takizawa, Tohru; Kondo, Tsuyoshi; Sakihara, Seizou; Ariizumi, Makoto; Watanabe, Naoki; Oyama, Hirofumi (December 2006). "Stress buffering effects of social support on depressive symptoms in middle age: Reciprocity and community mental health". Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 60 (6): 652–661. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01579.x. ISSN 1323-1316.
  36. ^ Stockdale, Susan E.; Wells, Kenneth B.; Tang, Lingqi; Belin, Thomas R.; Zhang, Lily; Sherbourne, Cathy D. (2007-11-01). "The importance of social context: Neighborhood stressors, stress-buffering mechanisms, and alcohol, drug, and mental health disorders". Social Science & Medicine. Placing Health in Context. 65 (9): 1867–1881. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.045. ISSN 0277-9536.
  37. ^ "New Surgeon General Advisory Raises Alarm about the Devastating Impact of the Epidemic of Loneliness and Isolation in the United States". www.hhs.gov. Office of the Assistant Secretary for Health. 2023-05-03. Retrieved 2023-11-27.
  38. ^ Schafer, Markus H; Sun, Haosen; Lee, Jin A (2021-11-30). "Compensatory Connections? Living Alone, Loneliness, and the Buffering Role of Social Connection Among Older American and European Adults". The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 77 (8): 1550–1560. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbab217. ISSN 1079-5014.
  39. ^ "Increased loneliness has become a global public health issue". Open Access Government. February 10, 2022.
  40. ^ Mushtaq, Raheel (2014). "Relationship Between Loneliness, Psychiatric Disorders and Physical Health ? A Review on the Psychological Aspects of Loneliness". Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research. doi:10.7860/jcdr/2014/10077.4828. ISSN 2249-782X.
  41. ^ "Loneliness and Social Isolation Linked to Serious Health Conditions". www.cdc.gov. 2022-12-08. Retrieved 2023-11-27.
  42. ^ Schafer, Markus H; Sun, Haosen; Lee, Jin A (2021-11-30). "Compensatory Connections? Living Alone, Loneliness, and the Buffering Role of Social Connection Among Older American and European Adults". The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 77 (8): 1550–1560. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbab217. ISSN 1079-5014.
  43. ^ a b Lee, Jee-Sook; Koeske, Gary F.; Sales, Esther (2004-09-01). "Social support buffering of acculturative stress: a study of mental health symptoms among Korean international students". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 28 (5): 399–414. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.08.005. ISSN 0147-1767.
  44. ^ a b Jang, Heejung; Tang, Fengyan (2021-12-15). "Loneliness, age at immigration, family relationships, and depression among older immigrants: A moderated relationship". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 39 (6): 1602–1622. doi:10.1177/02654075211061279. ISSN 0265-4075.
  45. ^ Kawachi, Ichiro; Berkman, Lisa F. (2001-09-01). "Social ties and mental health". Journal of Urban Health. 78 (3): 458–467. doi:10.1093/jurban/78.3.458. ISSN 1468-2869. PMC 3455910. PMID 11564849.
  46. ^ Ditzen, Beate; Heinrichs, Markus (2014). "Psychobiology of social support: The social dimension of stress buffering". Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 32 (1): 149–162. doi:10.3233/RNN-139008.
  47. ^ Reinelt, Eva; Aldinger, Maren; Stopsack, Malte; Schwahn, Christian; John, Ulrich; Baumeister, Sebastian E.; Grabe, Hans Jörgen; Barnow, Sven (2014-08-01). "High social support buffers the effects of 5-HTTLPR genotypes within social anxiety disorder". European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 264 (5): 433–439. doi:10.1007/s00406-013-0481-5. ISSN 1433-8491.
  48. ^ Hostinar, Camelia E.; Sullivan, Regina M.; Gunnar, Megan R. (January 2014). "Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (1): 256–282. doi:10.1037/a0032671. ISSN 1939-1455.
  49. ^ a b c d e Gunnar, Megan R.; Hostinar, Camelia E. (2015-09-03). "The social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in humans: Developmental and experiential determinants". Social Neuroscience. 10 (5): 479–488. doi:10.1080/17470919.2015.1070747. ISSN 1747-0919. PMC 4618716. PMID 26230646.
  50. ^ Gunnar, Megan R.; Brodersen, Laurie; Nachmias, Melissa; Buss, Kristin; Rigatuso, Joseph (April 1996). <191::aid-dev1>3.0.co;2-m "Stress reactivity and attachment security". Developmental Psychobiology. 29 (3): 191–204. doi:10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::aid-dev1>3.0.co;2-m. ISSN 0012-1630.
  51. ^ Brown, B. Bradford; Larson, James (2009-10-30). "Peer Relationships in Adolescence". Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. doi:10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy002004.
  52. ^ Kendrick, Kristin; Jutengren, Göran; Stattin, Håkan (2012-03-29). "The protective role of supportive friends against bullying perpetration and victimization". Journal of Adolescence. 35 (4): 1069–1080. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014. ISSN 0140-1971.
  53. ^ Aguilera, Greti (2012), "The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis and Neuroendocrine Responses to Stress", Handbook of Neuroendocrinology, Elsevier, pp. 175–196, retrieved 2023-11-27
  54. ^ Hostinar, Camelia E.; Johnson, Anna E.; Gunnar, Megan R. (November 2015). "Early social deprivation and the social buffering of cortisol stress responses in late childhood: An experimental study". Developmental Psychology. 51 (11): 1597–1608. doi:10.1037/dev0000029. ISSN 1939-0599.
  55. ^ Gunnar, Megan R.; Sullivan, Regina M. (2017-01-02). "The neurodevelopment of social buffering and fear learning: integration and crosstalk". Social Neuroscience. 12 (1): 1–7. doi:10.1080/17470919.2016.1151824. ISSN 1747-0919.
  56. ^ Gee, Dylan G.; Humphreys, Kathryn L.; Flannery, Jessica; Goff, Bonnie; Telzer, Eva H.; Shapiro, Mor; Hare, Todd A.; Bookheimer, Susan Y.; Tottenham, Nim (2013-03-06). "A Developmental Shift from Positive to Negative Connectivity in Human Amygdala–Prefrontal Circuitry". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (10): 4584–4593. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3446-12.2013. ISSN 0270-6474.
  57. ^ Tottenham, Nim; Shapiro, Mor; Telzer, Eva H.; Humphreys, Kathryn L. (May 2012). "Amygdala response to mother". Developmental Science. 15 (3): 307–319. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01128.x. ISSN 1363-755X. PMC 3522470. PMID 22490172.
  58. ^ a b c Kikusui, Takefumi; Winslow, James T; Mori, Yuji (2006-12-29). "Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 361 (1476): 2215–2228. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.1941. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 1764848. PMID 17118934.
  59. ^ Hennessy, Michael B.; Zate, Ryan; Maken, Deborah S. (March 2008). "Social buffering of the cortisol response of adult female guinea pigs". Physiology & Behavior. 93 (4–5): 883–888. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.005. ISSN 0031-9384.
  60. ^ Ricci-Bonot, Claire; Romero, Teresa; Nicol, Christine; Mills, Daniel (2021-02-26). "Social buffering in horses (Equus caballus): Influence of context, companion familiarity and companion habituation". dx.doi.org. Retrieved 2023-11-27.
  61. ^ Gust, Deborah A.; Gordon, Thomas P.; Hambright, M.Karen; Wilson, Mark E. (September 1993). "Relationship between Social Factors and Pituitary-Adrenocortical Activity in Female Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta)". Hormones and Behavior. 27 (3): 318–331. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1993.1024. ISSN 0018-506X.
  62. ^ McGlone, John J.; Stansbury, Wayne F.; Tribble, Leland F. (1986-09-01). "Aerosolized 5α-androst-16-en-3-one Reduced Agonistic Behavior and Temporarily Improved Performance of Growing Pigs1". Journal of Animal Science. 63 (3): 679–684. doi:10.2527/jas1986.633679x. ISSN 0021-8812.
  63. ^ Kikusui, Takefumi; Takigami, Shu; Takeuchi, Yukari; Mori, Yuji (January 2001). "Alarm pheromone enhances stress-induced hyperthermia in rats". Physiology & Behavior. 72 (1–2): 45–50. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00370-x. ISSN 0031-9384.
  64. ^ Kiyokawa, Yasushi; Kikusui, Takefumi; Takeuchi, Yukari; Mori, Yuji (May 2005). "Mapping the neural circuit activated by alarm pheromone perception by c-Fos immunohistochemistry". Brain Research. 1043 (1–2): 145–154. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061. ISSN 0006-8993.
  65. ^ McGlone, John J.; Stansbury, Wayne F.; Tribble, Leland F. (1986-09-01). "Aerosolized 5α-androst-16-en-3-one Reduced Agonistic Behavior and Temporarily Improved Performance of Growing Pigs1". Journal of Animal Science. 63 (3): 679–684. doi:10.2527/jas1986.633679x. ISSN 0021-8812.
  66. ^ Guiraudie, G. (2003-09-01). "Functional Characterization of Olfactory Binding Proteins for Appeasing Compounds and Molecular Cloning in the Vomeronasal Organ of Pre-pubertal Pigs". Chemical Senses. 28 (7): 609–619. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjg052. ISSN 1464-3553.
  67. ^ Sheppard, G.; Mills, D. S. (April 2003). "Evaluation of dog‐appeasing pheromone as a potential treatment for dogs fearful of fireworks". Veterinary Record. 152 (14): 432–436. doi:10.1136/vr.152.14.432. ISSN 0042-4900.
  68. ^ Porter, RichardH.; Winberg, Jan (January 1999). "Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 23 (3): 439–449. doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00044-x. ISSN 0149-7634.
  69. ^ Rukstalis, Michael; French, Jeffrey A. (January 2005). "Vocal buffering of the stress response: exposure to conspecific vocalizations moderates urinary cortisol excretion in isolated marmosets". Hormones and Behavior. 47 (1): 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.004. ISSN 0018-506X. PMC 2987614. PMID 15579259.
  70. ^ da Costa, Ana P.; Leigh, Andrea E.; Man, Mei-See; Kendrick, Keith M. (2004-10-07). "Face pictures reduce behavioural, autonomic, endocrine and neural indices of stress and fear in sheep". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 271 (1552): 2077–2084. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2831. ISSN 0962-8452.