First impression (psychology): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m →‎Amygdala: rm hyphen
Sarahay (talk | contribs)
No edit summary
Line 1: Line 1:
<!-- EDIT BELOW THIS LINE -->
In [[psychology]], a '''first impression''' is the event when one person first encounters another person and forms a [[mental image]] of that person. It can sometimes form an accurate representation of the person, depending on the observer and the person being observed.<ref>{{cite web |accessdate=2011-02-20 |url=http://web.archive.org/web/20110220025155/http://www.psychologytoday.com/articles/200405/the-first-impression |title=The First Impression |work=Psychology Today |date=2004-05-14 |author=Flora, Carlin }}</ref>
In [[psychology]], a first impression is the event when one person first encounters another person and forms a [[mental image]] of that person. Impression accuracy varies depending on the observer and the target (person, object, scene, etc.) being observed.<ref name=Mackie>{{cite book|last=Mackie|first=Eliot R. Smith, Diane M.|title=Social psychology|date=2007|publisher=Psychology Press|location=Hove|isbn=1841694096|edition=3rd|url=http://psypress.co.uk/smithandmackie/resources/|accessdate=05/08/14}}</ref><ref name=Carlin>{{cite web|last=Flora|first=Carlin|title="The First Impression"|url=http://web.archive.org/web/20110220025155/http://www.psychologytoday.com/articles/200405/the-first-impression|publisher=Psychology Today|accessdate=2011-02-20.}}</ref>
First impressions are based on a wide range of characteristics: [[age]], [[race]], [[culture]], [[language]], [[gender]], [[physical appearance]], [[accent (sociolinguistics)]], [[posture (psychology)]], [[voice]], number of people present, and [[time]] allowed to process.<ref name=Mackie/><ref name=Aronson>{{cite book|last=Aronson|first=Elliot, Robin M. Akert, Timothy D. Wilson, Robin M.|title=Social psychology|date=2007|publisher=Pearson Prentice-Hall|location=Upper Saddle River, NJ|isbn=0132382458|edition=6th|url=http://wps.prenhall.com/hss_aronson_socpsych_6/64/16425/4205041.cw/-/t/index.html|accessdate=05/08/14}}}}</ref><ref name=Biesanz>{{cite journal|last=Biesanz|first=J.C.|coauthors=Human, L. J., Paquin, A., Chan, M., Parisotto, K. L., Sarracino, J., & Gillis, R. L.|title=Do we know when our impressions of others are valid? evidence for realistic accuracy awareness in first impressions of personality|journal=Social Psychological and Personality Science|date=2011|volume=2|issue=5|doi=10.1177/1948550610397211}}</ref><ref name=Bhargrave>{{cite journal|last=Bhargrave|first=R|coauthors=Montgomery, N.V.|title=The social context of temporal sequences: Why first impressions shape shared experiences|journal=Journal of Consumer Research|volume=40|issue=3|pages=501-517|doi=10.1086/671053}}</ref><ref name=Ding>{{cite journal|last=Ding|first=Jonathan Y. C.|coauthors=Rule, Nicholas O.|title=Gay, Straight, or Somewhere in Between: Accuracy and Bias in the Perception of Bisexual Faces|journal=Journal of Nonverbal Behavior|date=12 January 2012|volume=36|issue=2|pages=165–176|doi=10.1007/s10919-011-0129-y}}</ref><ref name=Naumann>{{cite journal|last=Naumann|first=L. P.|coauthors=Vazire, S.; Rentfrow, P. J.; Gosling, S. D.|title=Personality Judgments Based on Physical Appearance|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin|date=17 September 2009|volume=35|issue=12|pages=1661–1671|doi=10.1177/0146167209346309}}</ref><ref name=Trudeau>{{cite news|last=Trudeau|first=M.|title=You Had Me At Hello: The Science Behind First Impressions|url=http://www.npr.org/blogs/health/2014/05/05/308349318/you-had-me-at-hello-the-science-behind-first-impressions?utm_source=facebook.com&utm_medium=social&utm_campaign=npr&utm_term=nprnews&utm_content=20140505|accessdate=6 May 2014|newspaper=NPR|date=5 May 2014}}</ref> The first impressions individuals give to others could greatly impact how they are treated and viewed in many contexts of everyday life.<ref name=Guadagno>{{cite journal|last=Guadagno|first=Rosanna E.|coauthors=Cialdini, Robert B.|title=Gender Differences in Impression Management in Organizations: A Qualitative Review|journal=Sex Roles|date=21 March 2007|volume=56|issue=7-8|pages=483–494|doi=10.1007/s11199-007-9187-3}}</ref><ref name=Wood>{{cite journal|last=Wood|first=T.J.|title=Exploring the role of first impressions in rater-based assessments|journal=Advances in Health Sciences Education|doi=10.1007/s10459-013-9453-9}}</ref>


==Speed and Accuracy==
==Precursors==


It takes just one-tenth of a second for us to judge someone and make a first impression.<ref name=Willis>{{cite journal|last=Willis|first=J.|coauthors=Todorov, A.|title=First impressions: Making up your mind after 100 ms exposure to a face|journal=Psychological Science|date=2006|issue=17|pages=592-598.}}</ref> Research finds that the more time participants are afforded to form the impression, the more confidence in impressions they report.<ref name=Willis /><ref name=Wargo>{{cite journal|last=Wargo|first=E|title=How many seconds to a first impression?|journal=The Observer|date=2006|volume=19|url=http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/publications/observer/2006/july-06/how-many-seconds-to-a-first-impression.html}}</ref> Not only are people quick to form first impressions, they are also fairly accurate when the target presents him or herself genuinely. People are generally not good at perceiving feigned emotions or detecting lies.<ref name=Carlin /> Research participants who reported forming accurate impressions of specific targets did tend to have more accurate perceptions of specific targets that aligned with others’ reports of the target.<ref name=Biesanz/> Individuals are also fairly reliable at understanding the first impression that he/she will project to others.<ref name=Carlson>{{cite journal|last=Carlson|first=E. N.|coauthors=Furr, R. M.; Vazire, S.|title=Do We Know the First Impressions We Make? Evidence for Idiographic Meta-Accuracy and Calibration of First Impressions|journal=Social Psychological and Personality Science|date=1 January 2010|volume=1|issue=1|pages=94–98|doi=10.1177/1948550609356028}}</ref>
'The phrase "first impressions" comes directly from the terminology of [[Sentimentality|sentimental]] literature...where "first impressions" exhibit the strength and truth of the heart's immediate and intuitive response'.<ref>Brian Southam, in Tony Tanner ed., ''Pride and Prejudice'' (Penguin 1975) p. 10</ref> ''[[Pride and Prejudice]]'' has been seen as a 'glancing blow aimed at the conventions of the sentimental novel'<ref>Tanner, p. 11</ref> - at the "prejudices" inherent in the casual adoption of first impressions.


The rate at which different qualities are detected in first impressions may be linked to what has been important to survival from an evolutionary perspective.<ref name=Willis /> For example, trustworthiness and attractiveness were the two traits most quickly detected and evaluated in a study of human faces.<ref name=Willis /><ref name=Wargo/> People are fairly good at assessing personality traits of others in general, but there appears to be a difference in first impression judgments between older and younger adults. Older adults judged young adult target photos as healthier, more trustworthy, and less hostile, but more aggressive, than younger adults did of the same photos.<ref name=Zebrowitz2>{{cite journal|last=Zebrowitz|first=Leslie A.|coauthors=Franklin, Robert G.; Hillman, Suzanne; Boc, Henry|title=Older and younger adults' first impressions from faces: Similar in agreement but different in positivity.|journal=Psychology and Aging|volume=28|issue=1|pages=202–212|doi=10.1037/a0030927}}</ref> Older adults could have a lower response to negative cues due to a slower processing speed, causing them to see facial features on young adults as more positive than younger adults do.<ref name=Zebrowitz2 />
As 'a novel in which the adage "first impressions are lasting impressions" proves a test rather than a truth', it charts the movement 'from first impressions and prejudice, to reflections and revisions'.<ref>C. L. Johnson/C. Tuite, ''A Companion to Jane Austen'' (2009) p. 113-5</ref>


==Social processing==
==Number of observers==
One's first impressions are affected by whether he/she is alone or with any number of people.<ref name=Bhargrave /> Joint experiences are more globally processed (see [[global precedence]] for more on processing), as in collectivist cultures. Global processing emphasizes first impressions more because the collective first impression tends to remain stable over time. Solo experiences tend to facilitate local processing, causing the viewer to take a more critical look at the target. Thus, individuals are more likely to have negative first impressions than groups of two or more viewers of the same target. At the same time, individuals are more likely to experience an upward trend over the course of a series of impressions, e.g. individual viewers will like the final episode of a TV season more than the first even if it is really the same quality.<ref name=Bhargrave />


When viewing pieces of art in an experiment, participants in a solo context rated art in an improving sequence significantly higher than when the targets are presented in a declining sequence.<ref name=Bhargrave/> When viewing the art in a joint context, participants evaluated the first and last pieces similarly in both kinds of sequence. Simply priming viewers to feel like they were in solo or joint contexts or to process analytically or holistically was enough to produce the same viewing effects.<ref name=Bhargrave/>
'First impressions are lasting impressions', and although sometimes misleading, 'research shows that in many situations, our impressions of other people can be quite accurate'.<ref>Eliot R. Smith/Diane M. Mackie, ''Social Psychology'' (2007) p. 86 and p. 57</ref> Only in more serious situations is 'going beyond first impressions to seek greater accuracy in person perception sometimes important'.<ref>Smith, p. 92</ref>


==Cultural influences==
<br />
===Individualism versus Collectivism===
It takes just one-tenth of a second for us to judge someone and make our first impression, with confidence in impression formation increasing with increasing time taken to form the impression.<ref>Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after 100
ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592-598.</ref>


Similar to the number of viewers present, [[collectivism]] versus [[individualism]] can influence impression formation.<ref name=Aronson /> Collectivists are at ease as long as their impressions are largely in alignment with the larger group’s impressions. When a collectivist wants to change his/her impression, he/she may be compelled to change the views of all group members.<ref name=Fang /> However, this could be challenging for collectivists, who tend to be less confrontational than individualists. Individualists are willing change their own views at will and are generally more comfortable with uncertainty, which makes them naturally more willing to change their impressions.<ref name=Fang />
==Amygdala==


===Influence of Media Richness===
Brain circuitry allows a bypassing of the neo-cortex by way of the so-called [[amygdala hijack]]: 'this smaller and shorter pathway allows the amygdala to receive some direct inputs from the senses and start a response ''before'' they are fully registered by the neo-cortex'.<ref>Daniel Goleman, ''Emotional Intelligence'' (1996) p. 18</ref>


Research is mixed on whether national culture mediates the relationship between media richness and bias in impression formation. Some studies that manipulated media richness have found that information presented in text form yields similar impressions (measured by reported appraisal scores) among cultures,<ref name=Fang>{{cite journal|last=Fang|first=X.|coauthors=Rajkumar, T. M.|title=The Role of National Culture and Multimedia on First Impression Bias Reduction: An Experimental Study in US and China|journal=IEEE Transactions On Professional Communication|date=2013|volume=56|issue=4|pages=354-371|doi=10.1109/TPC.2013.2251503}}</ref> while other studies found that richer forms of information such as videos reduce cross-cultural bias more effectively.<ref name=Lim>{{cite journal|last=Lim|first=K. H.|coauthors=Benbasat, I., & Ward, L. M.|title=The role of multimedia in changing first impression bias|journal=Information Systems Research|date=2000|volume=11|issue=2|pages=115-136|doi=10.1287/isre.11.2.115.11776}}</ref> The latter findings support [[Media Richness Theory]].<ref name=Lim />
Research has shown that 'in the first few milliseconds of our perceiving something we not only unconsciously comprehend what it is, but decide whether we like it or not: the "cognitive unconscious"'.<ref>Goleman, p. 20</ref>


==See also==
===Accents and Speech===
{{Columns-list|2|
*[[Love at first sight]]
*[[Superficiality]]
}}
==References==
{{Reflist|colwidth=40em}}


Accents and unique speech patterns can influence how people are perceived by those to whom they are speaking. For example, when hypothetically interviewing an applicant with a Midwestern U.S. accent, Colombian accent, or French accent, participants evaluated the U.S. or Colombian accent as more positive than the applicant with the French accent due to perceived similarity to themselves. <ref name=Deprez-Sims>{{cite journal|last=Deprez-Sims|first=Anne-Sophie|coauthors=Morris, Scott B.|title=Accents in the workplace: Their effects during a job interview|journal=International Journal of Psychology|volume=45|issue=6|pages=417–426|doi=10.1080/00207594.2010.499950}}</ref> First impressions can be heavily influenced by a similarity-attraction hypothesis where others are immediately put into “similar" or “dissimilar” categories from the viewer and judged accordingly.<ref name=Deprez-Sims />
{{psychology-stub}}

[[Category:Psychology terminology]]
===Physical Characteristics and Personality===

Although populations from different cultures can be quick to view others as dissimilar, there are several first impression characteristics that are universal across cultures.<ref name=Aronson /> When comparing trait impressions of faces among U.S. and the culturally isolated Tsimane’ people of Bolivia, there was between-culture agreement when ascribing certain physical features to descriptive traits such as attractiveness, intelligence, health, and warmth.<ref name=Zebrowitz>{{cite journal|last=Zebrowitz|first=L. A.|coauthors=Wang, R.; Bronstad, P. M.; Eisenberg, D.; Undurraga, E.; Reyes-Garcia, V.; Godoy, R.|title=First Impressions From Faces Among U.S. and Culturally Isolated Tsimane' People in the Bolivian Rainforest|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|date=19 December 2011|volume=43|issue=1|pages=119–134|doi=10.1177/0022022111411386}}</ref> Both cultures also show a strong attractiveness halo when forming impressions, meaning that those seen as attractive were also rated as more competent, sociable, intelligent, and healthy.<ref name=Zebrowitz />

==Physical appearance==

===Faces and features===

Physical appearance gives us clear clues as to a person’s personality without him/her ever having to speak or move.<ref name=Aronson /><ref name=Ding /><ref name=Naumann /><ref name=Workman>{{cite journal|last=Workman|first=J. E.|coauthors=Johnson, K. K. P.|title=The Role of Cosmetics in Impression Formation|journal=Clothing and Textiles Research Journal|date=1 September 1991|volume=10|issue=1|pages=63–67|doi=10.1177/0887302X9101000109}}</ref> Women tend to be better than men at judging nonverbal behavior.<ref name=Aronson /> After viewing pictures of people in a neutral position and in a self-chosen posed position, observers were accurate at judging the target’s levels of [[extraversion]], emotional stability, openness, [[self-esteem]], and religiosity.<ref name=Naumann /> The combined impression of physical characteristics, body posture, facial expression, and clothing choices lets observers form accurate images of a target’s personality, so long as the person observed is presenting themselves genuinely.<ref name=Carlin /><ref name=Naumann /> However, there is some conflicting data in this field. Other evidence suggests that people sometimes rely too much on appearance cues over actual information.<ref name=Olivola>{{cite journal|last=Olivola|first=Christopher Y.|coauthors=Todorov, Alexander|title=Fooled by first impressions? Reexamining the diagnostic value of appearance-based inferences|journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology|volume=46|issue=2|pages=315–324|doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.002}}</ref><ref name=Todorov>{{cite journal|last=Todorov|first=A.|title=Inferences of Competence from Faces Predict Election Outcomes|journal=Science|date=10 June 2005|volume=308|issue=5728|pages=1623–1626|doi=10.1126/science.1110589}}</ref> When provided with descriptive information about a target, participants still rely on physical appearance cues when making judgments about others' personalities and capabilities. Participants struggle to look past physical appearance cues even when they know information contrary to their initial judgment.<ref name=Olivola /> Physical cues are also used to make judgments about political candidates based on extremely brief exposures to their pictures.<ref name=Todorov /> Perceived competence level of a candidate measured from first impressions of facial features can directly predict voting results.<ref name=Todorov />

The “beautiful is good” effect is a very present phenomenon when dealing with first impressions of others.<ref name=Aronson /> Targets who are attractive are rated more positively and as possessing more unique characteristics than those who are unattractive.<ref name=Lorenzo>{{cite journal|last=Lorenzo|first=G. L.|coauthors=Biesanz, J. C.; Human, L. J.|title=What Is Beautiful Is Good and More Accurately Understood: Physical Attractiveness and Accuracy in First Impressions of Personality|journal=Psychological Science|date=4 November 2010|volume=21|issue=12|pages=1777–1782|doi=10.1177/0956797610388048}}</ref> Beauty is also found to be somewhat subjective so that even targets who are not universally attractive can receive the benefit of this effect if the observer is attracted to them.<ref name=Lorenzo />

===Apparel and cosmetics===

Cosmetic use is also an important cue for forming impressions, particularly of women. Those wearing heavy makeup are seen as significantly more feminine than those wearing moderate makeup or no makeup and those wearing heavy or moderate makeup are seen as more attractive than those wearing no makeup.<ref name=Workman /> While a woman wearing no makeup is perceived as being more moral than the other two conditions, there is no difference between experimental conditions when judging personality or personal temperament.<ref name=Workman />

First impression formation can be influenced by the use of cognitive short hands such as [[stereotypes]] and [[representative heuristic]]s.<ref name=Aronson /> When asked to rate the [[socioeconomic status]] (SES) and degree of interest in friendship with African American and Caucasian female models wearing either a [[K-Mart]], [[Abercrombie & Fitch]], or non-logoed sweatshirt, Caucasian models were rated more favorably than the African American models.<ref name=McDermott/> Abercrombie & Fitch wearers were rated as higher SES than the other sweatshirts. Interestingly, participants wanted to be friends with the Caucasian model most when she was wearing a plain sweatshirt and the African American model most when she was wearing either the plain or K-Mart sweatshirt. It is unclear why the plain sweatshirt was most associated with friendship, but the general results suggest that mismatching class and race reduced the model's friendship appeal.<ref name=McDermott>{{cite journal|last=McDermott|first=L.|coauthors=Pettijohn, T.|title=The influence of clothing fashion and race on the perceived socioeconomic status and person perception of college students|journal=Psychology & Society|date=2011|volume=4|pages=64-75}}</ref>

==Specific Contexts==

===Online===

Online profiles and communication channels such as email provide fewer cues than in-person interactions, which makes targets more difficult to understand.<ref name=Aronson /> When research participants were asked to evaluate a person's facial attractiveness and perceived ambition based on an online dating profile, amount of time permitted for processing and reporting an evaluation of the target produced a difference in impression formation.<ref name=Sritharan /> Spontaneous evaluations relied on physical attractiveness almost exclusively, whereas deliberate evualuations weighed both types of information. Although deliberate evaluations used the information provided on both physical attractiveness and ambition of each target, the particular impact of each kind of information appeared to depend on the consistency between the two. A significant effect of attractiveness on deliberate evaluations was found only when perceived ambition was consistent with the perceived level of attractiveness. <ref name=Sritharan>{{cite journal|last=Sritharan|first=R.|coauthors=Heilpern, K., Wilbur, C. J., & Gawronski, B.|title=I think I like you: Spontaneous and deliberate evaluations of potential romantic partners in an online dating context|journal=European Journal of Social Psychology|date=2010|volume=40|page=1062-1077|doi=10.1002/ejsp.703}}</ref> The consistency found in profiles seemed to particularly influence deliberate evaluations.

In a study of online impressions, participants who were socially expressive and disclosed a lot about themselves both on their webpages and in person were better liked than those who were less open. Social expressivity includes liveliness in voice, smiling, etc.<ref name=Weisbuch>{{cite journal|last=Weisbuch|first=M.|coauthors=Ivcevic, Z., & Ambady, N.|title=On being liked on the web and in the 'real world': Consistency in first impressions across personal web pages and spontaneous behavior|journal=Journal Of Experimental Social Psychology|date=2009|volume=45|issue=3|pages=573-576|doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.009}}</ref>

===Dating and Sexuality===

Upon seeing photographs of straight, gay, and bisexual people, participants correctly identified gay versus straight males and females at above-chance levels based solely on seeing a picture of their face, however, bisexual targets were only identified at chance.<ref name=Ding /> The data suggest that people do not differentiate between bisexual targets and gay targets, but do perceive a difference between straight targets and those who identify as either bisexual or gay.

The more time participants are allowed to make some judgment about a person, the more they will weigh information beyond physical appearance. Specific manipulations include identifying men as gay versus straight<ref name=Wood2014 /> and people as trustworthy or not.<ref name=Biesanz/><ref name=Wargo/> In a study of the interaction between ratings of people in speed dating and the form of media used to present them, impression accuracy in a speed dating task was not significantly different when a potential date was presented in person versus in a video. However, impressions of dates made via video were to be much more negative than those made in person. An additional study that looked at characterization of a romantic partner suggested that people are more likely to rely on “gut reactions” when meeting in person, but there isn’t sufficient information for this kind of evaluation when viewing someone online.

===Professional===

Non-verbal behaviors are particularly important to forming first impressions when meeting a business acquaintance.<ref name=Goman>{{cite book|last=Goman|first=Carol Kinsey|title=The nonverbal advantage : secrets and science of body language at work|date=2008|publisher=Berrett-Koehler Publishers|location=San Francisco, Calif.|isbn=1576754928|edition=[Online-Ausg.].}}</ref> Specifically, components of social expressivity, such as smiling, eyebrow position, emotional expression, and eye contact are emphasized.<ref name=Carlin /><ref name=Weisbuch/><ref name=Goman/> Straightening one’s posture, leaning in slightly, and giving a firm handshake promotes favorable impression formation in the American business context.<ref name=Goman/> Other impression management tactics in the business world include researching the organization and interviewers beforehand, preparing specific questions for the interviewer, showing confidence, and dressing appropriately.<ref name=Rowh>{{cite web|last=Rowh|first=Mark|title=First Impressions Count|url=http://www.apa.org/gradpsych/2012/11/first-impressions.aspx|publisher=American Psychological Association|accessdate=04/18/14}}</ref>

A qualitative review of previous literature looking at self-report data suggests that men and women use [[impression management]] tactics in the corporate world that are consistent with stereotypical gender roles when presenting themselves to others.<ref name=Guadagno /> This research proposes that women are put in a double bind where those who portray themselves as more communal and submissive are overlooked for leadership positions and women who try to utilize male tactics (such as being more aggressive) receive negative consequences for violating normative gender roles.<ref name=Guadagno /> To change this dynamic the authors suggest that managerial positions should be re-advertised to highlight the feminine qualities needed for a position and staff training should involve a segment accentuating gender issues in the office to make everyone aware of possible discrimination.<ref name=Guadagno />

Data collected from interviews with physicians distinguishes between first impressions and intuition and contributes to understanding the occurrence of gut feelings in the medical field.<ref name=Woolley>{{cite journal|last=Woolley|first=A.|coauthors=Kostopoulou, O.|title=Clinical intuition in family medicine: More than first impressions|journal=Annals Of Family Medicine|date=2013|volume=11|pages=60-66|doi=10.1370/afm.1433}}</ref> Gut feelings go beyond first impressions: Physicians expressed feeling doubtful about their initial impressions as they gathered more data from their patients. More experienced physicians reported more instances of gut feelings than those less experienced, but the quality of the intuition was related to the quality of feedback received during the data collection process in general. Emotional engagement enhanced learning just as it does in first impressions.<ref name=Woolley/>

==Neuroscience==

First impressions are formed within milliseconds of seeing a target. When intentionally forming a first impression, encoding relies on the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC).<ref name=Gilron>{{cite journal|last=Gilron|first=R.|coauthors=Gutchess, A. H.|title=Remembering first impressions: Effects of intentionality and diagnosticity on subsequent memory|journal=Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience|date=2012|volume=12|issue=1|pages=85-98|doi=10.3758/s13415-011-0074-6}}</ref> Readings from [[fMRI]]s of research participants show that processing of diagnostic information (e.g. distinguishing features) engaged the dmPFC more than processing neutral information.<ref name=Gilron/>

Participants generally formed more negative impressions of the faces that showed the negative emotion compared to neutral faces.<ref name=Iidaka>{{cite journal|last=Iidaka|first=T.|coauthors=Harada, T., & Sadato, N.|title=Forming a negative impression of another person correlates with activation in medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala|journal=Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosciene|date=2011|volume=6|issue=4|pages=516-525}}</ref> Results suggest that the dmPFC and [[amygdala]] together play a large role in negative impression formation. When forming immediate impressions based on emotion, the stimulus can bypass the [[neo-cortex]] by way of the “[[amygdala hijack]].”<ref name=Goleman>{{cite book|last=Goleman|first=Daniel|title=Working with Emotional Intelligence|date=1999|pages=87}}</ref>

===Familiarity===

Research indicates that people are efficient evaluators when forming impressions based on existing biases.<ref name=Schiller>{{cite journal|last=Schiller|first=D.|coauthors=Freeman, J. B., Mitchell, J. P., Uleman, J. S., & Phelps, E. A.|title=A neural mechanism of first impressions|journal=Nature Neuroscience|date=2009|volume=12|pages=508-514|doi=10.1038/nn.2278}}</ref> The [[posterior cingulate cortex]] (PCC), amygdala, and the thalamus sort of relevant versus irrelevant information according to these biases. The dmPFC is also involved in the impression formation process, especially with person-descriptive information.<ref name=Schiller/>

FMRI results show activation of the fusiform cortex, [[posterior cingulate gyrus]], and amygdala when individuals are asked to identify previously seen faces that were encoded as either “friends” or “foes.”<ref name=Vrticka>{{cite journal|last=Vrticka|first=Pascal|coauthors=Andersson, Frédéric; Sander, David; Vuilleumier, Patrik|title=Memory for friends or foes: The social context of past encounters with faces modulates their subsequent neural traces in the brain|journal=Social Neuroscience|volume=4|issue=5|pages=384–401|doi=10.1080/17470910902941793}}</ref> Additionally, the [[caudate]] and [[anterior cingulate cortex]] are more activated when looking at faces of “foes” versus “friends."<ref name=Vrticka /> This research suggests that quick first impressions of hostility or support from unknown people can lead to long-term effects on memory that will later be associated with that person.

===Alcohol and Impressions===

[[Alcohol]] consumption and belief of consumption influenced [[emotion]] detection in ten second clips.<ref name=Walter>{{cite journal|last=Walter|coauthors=Walter, M., Matthiä, C., Wiebking, C., Rotte, M., Tempelmann, C., Bogerts, B., Heinze, H.-J. and Northoff, G.|title=Preceding attention and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex: Process specificity versus domain dependence. Hum. Brain Mapp.|date=2009|volume=30|pages=312-326|doi=10.1002/hbm.20506}}</ref> Participants who thought they had consumed an alcoholic beverage rated one facial expression (approximately 3% of the facial expressions they saw) more in each clip as happy compared to the control group. Thus, impression formation may be affected by even the perception of alcohol consumption.<ref name=Walter />

===Cross-cultural Differences===

There appears to be cross-cultural similarities in brain responses to first impression formations. In a [[mock election]] both [[American]] and [[Japanese]] individuals voted for the candidate that elicited a stronger response in their bilateral amygdala than those who did not, regardless of the candidate’s culture.<ref name=Rule>{{cite journal|last=Rule|first=N. O.|coauthors=Freeman, J. B.; Moran, J. M.; Gabrieli, J. D. E.; Adams, R. B.; Ambady, N.|title=Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across cultures|journal=Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience|date=5 December 2009|volume=5|issue=2-3|pages=349–355|doi=10.1093/scan/nsp046}}</ref> Individuals also showed a stronger response to cultural [[outgroup]] faces than cultural [[ingroup]] faces because the amygdala is presumably more sensitive to novel stimuli.<ref name=Rule /> However, this finding was unrelated to actual voting decisions.<ref name=Rule />

==Stability of First Impressions==

Once formed, first impressions tend to be stable. A review of the literature on the accuracy and impact of first impressions on rater-based assessments found that raters’ first impressions are highly correlated with later scores, but it is unclear exactly why.<ref name=Wood/> One study tested stability by asking participants to form impressions people based purely on [[photographs]]. Participants’ opinions of the people in photographs did not significantly differ after interacting with that person a month later.<ref name=Wood2014>{{cite web|last=Wood|first=J.|title=The Power of a First Impression|url=http://psychcentral.com/news/2014/02/15/the-power-of-a-first-impression/65944.html|publisher=Psych Central|accessdate=6 May 2014}}</ref> One potential reason for this stability is that one's first impressions could serve as a guide for his/her next steps, such as what questions are asked and how raters go about scoring. More research needs to be done on the stability of first impressions to fully understand how first impressions guide subsequent treatment, [[self-fulfilling prophecies]], and the [[halo effect]].<ref name=Aronson /> Assessment tools are created can influence impressions too, ex. if a question provides only a dichotomous “yes” or “no” response or if a rater uses a [[scale (ratio)]]. Although this study was conducted with the intention of improving rating methods in medical education, the literature review was sufficiently broad enough to generalize.<ref name=Wood2014 />

==References==
{{Reflist}}

Revision as of 15:29, 8 May 2014

In psychology, a first impression is the event when one person first encounters another person and forms a mental image of that person. Impression accuracy varies depending on the observer and the target (person, object, scene, etc.) being observed.[1][2] First impressions are based on a wide range of characteristics: age, race, culture, language, gender, physical appearance, accent (sociolinguistics), posture (psychology), voice, number of people present, and time allowed to process.[1][3][4][5][6][7][8] The first impressions individuals give to others could greatly impact how they are treated and viewed in many contexts of everyday life.[9][10]

Speed and Accuracy

It takes just one-tenth of a second for us to judge someone and make a first impression.[11] Research finds that the more time participants are afforded to form the impression, the more confidence in impressions they report.[11][12] Not only are people quick to form first impressions, they are also fairly accurate when the target presents him or herself genuinely. People are generally not good at perceiving feigned emotions or detecting lies.[2] Research participants who reported forming accurate impressions of specific targets did tend to have more accurate perceptions of specific targets that aligned with others’ reports of the target.[4] Individuals are also fairly reliable at understanding the first impression that he/she will project to others.[13]

The rate at which different qualities are detected in first impressions may be linked to what has been important to survival from an evolutionary perspective.[11] For example, trustworthiness and attractiveness were the two traits most quickly detected and evaluated in a study of human faces.[11][12] People are fairly good at assessing personality traits of others in general, but there appears to be a difference in first impression judgments between older and younger adults. Older adults judged young adult target photos as healthier, more trustworthy, and less hostile, but more aggressive, than younger adults did of the same photos.[14] Older adults could have a lower response to negative cues due to a slower processing speed, causing them to see facial features on young adults as more positive than younger adults do.[14]

Number of observers

One's first impressions are affected by whether he/she is alone or with any number of people.[5] Joint experiences are more globally processed (see global precedence for more on processing), as in collectivist cultures. Global processing emphasizes first impressions more because the collective first impression tends to remain stable over time. Solo experiences tend to facilitate local processing, causing the viewer to take a more critical look at the target. Thus, individuals are more likely to have negative first impressions than groups of two or more viewers of the same target. At the same time, individuals are more likely to experience an upward trend over the course of a series of impressions, e.g. individual viewers will like the final episode of a TV season more than the first even if it is really the same quality.[5]

When viewing pieces of art in an experiment, participants in a solo context rated art in an improving sequence significantly higher than when the targets are presented in a declining sequence.[5] When viewing the art in a joint context, participants evaluated the first and last pieces similarly in both kinds of sequence. Simply priming viewers to feel like they were in solo or joint contexts or to process analytically or holistically was enough to produce the same viewing effects.[5]

Cultural influences

Individualism versus Collectivism

Similar to the number of viewers present, collectivism versus individualism can influence impression formation.[3] Collectivists are at ease as long as their impressions are largely in alignment with the larger group’s impressions. When a collectivist wants to change his/her impression, he/she may be compelled to change the views of all group members.[15] However, this could be challenging for collectivists, who tend to be less confrontational than individualists. Individualists are willing change their own views at will and are generally more comfortable with uncertainty, which makes them naturally more willing to change their impressions.[15]

Influence of Media Richness

Research is mixed on whether national culture mediates the relationship between media richness and bias in impression formation. Some studies that manipulated media richness have found that information presented in text form yields similar impressions (measured by reported appraisal scores) among cultures,[15] while other studies found that richer forms of information such as videos reduce cross-cultural bias more effectively.[16] The latter findings support Media Richness Theory.[16]

Accents and Speech

Accents and unique speech patterns can influence how people are perceived by those to whom they are speaking. For example, when hypothetically interviewing an applicant with a Midwestern U.S. accent, Colombian accent, or French accent, participants evaluated the U.S. or Colombian accent as more positive than the applicant with the French accent due to perceived similarity to themselves. [17] First impressions can be heavily influenced by a similarity-attraction hypothesis where others are immediately put into “similar" or “dissimilar” categories from the viewer and judged accordingly.[17]

Physical Characteristics and Personality

Although populations from different cultures can be quick to view others as dissimilar, there are several first impression characteristics that are universal across cultures.[3] When comparing trait impressions of faces among U.S. and the culturally isolated Tsimane’ people of Bolivia, there was between-culture agreement when ascribing certain physical features to descriptive traits such as attractiveness, intelligence, health, and warmth.[18] Both cultures also show a strong attractiveness halo when forming impressions, meaning that those seen as attractive were also rated as more competent, sociable, intelligent, and healthy.[18]

Physical appearance

Faces and features

Physical appearance gives us clear clues as to a person’s personality without him/her ever having to speak or move.[3][6][7][19] Women tend to be better than men at judging nonverbal behavior.[3] After viewing pictures of people in a neutral position and in a self-chosen posed position, observers were accurate at judging the target’s levels of extraversion, emotional stability, openness, self-esteem, and religiosity.[7] The combined impression of physical characteristics, body posture, facial expression, and clothing choices lets observers form accurate images of a target’s personality, so long as the person observed is presenting themselves genuinely.[2][7] However, there is some conflicting data in this field. Other evidence suggests that people sometimes rely too much on appearance cues over actual information.[20][21] When provided with descriptive information about a target, participants still rely on physical appearance cues when making judgments about others' personalities and capabilities. Participants struggle to look past physical appearance cues even when they know information contrary to their initial judgment.[20] Physical cues are also used to make judgments about political candidates based on extremely brief exposures to their pictures.[21] Perceived competence level of a candidate measured from first impressions of facial features can directly predict voting results.[21]

The “beautiful is good” effect is a very present phenomenon when dealing with first impressions of others.[3] Targets who are attractive are rated more positively and as possessing more unique characteristics than those who are unattractive.[22] Beauty is also found to be somewhat subjective so that even targets who are not universally attractive can receive the benefit of this effect if the observer is attracted to them.[22]

Apparel and cosmetics

Cosmetic use is also an important cue for forming impressions, particularly of women. Those wearing heavy makeup are seen as significantly more feminine than those wearing moderate makeup or no makeup and those wearing heavy or moderate makeup are seen as more attractive than those wearing no makeup.[19] While a woman wearing no makeup is perceived as being more moral than the other two conditions, there is no difference between experimental conditions when judging personality or personal temperament.[19]

First impression formation can be influenced by the use of cognitive short hands such as stereotypes and representative heuristics.[3] When asked to rate the socioeconomic status (SES) and degree of interest in friendship with African American and Caucasian female models wearing either a K-Mart, Abercrombie & Fitch, or non-logoed sweatshirt, Caucasian models were rated more favorably than the African American models.[23] Abercrombie & Fitch wearers were rated as higher SES than the other sweatshirts. Interestingly, participants wanted to be friends with the Caucasian model most when she was wearing a plain sweatshirt and the African American model most when she was wearing either the plain or K-Mart sweatshirt. It is unclear why the plain sweatshirt was most associated with friendship, but the general results suggest that mismatching class and race reduced the model's friendship appeal.[23]

Specific Contexts

Online

Online profiles and communication channels such as email provide fewer cues than in-person interactions, which makes targets more difficult to understand.[3] When research participants were asked to evaluate a person's facial attractiveness and perceived ambition based on an online dating profile, amount of time permitted for processing and reporting an evaluation of the target produced a difference in impression formation.[24] Spontaneous evaluations relied on physical attractiveness almost exclusively, whereas deliberate evualuations weighed both types of information. Although deliberate evaluations used the information provided on both physical attractiveness and ambition of each target, the particular impact of each kind of information appeared to depend on the consistency between the two. A significant effect of attractiveness on deliberate evaluations was found only when perceived ambition was consistent with the perceived level of attractiveness. [24] The consistency found in profiles seemed to particularly influence deliberate evaluations.

In a study of online impressions, participants who were socially expressive and disclosed a lot about themselves both on their webpages and in person were better liked than those who were less open. Social expressivity includes liveliness in voice, smiling, etc.[25]

Dating and Sexuality

Upon seeing photographs of straight, gay, and bisexual people, participants correctly identified gay versus straight males and females at above-chance levels based solely on seeing a picture of their face, however, bisexual targets were only identified at chance.[6] The data suggest that people do not differentiate between bisexual targets and gay targets, but do perceive a difference between straight targets and those who identify as either bisexual or gay.

The more time participants are allowed to make some judgment about a person, the more they will weigh information beyond physical appearance. Specific manipulations include identifying men as gay versus straight[26] and people as trustworthy or not.[4][12] In a study of the interaction between ratings of people in speed dating and the form of media used to present them, impression accuracy in a speed dating task was not significantly different when a potential date was presented in person versus in a video. However, impressions of dates made via video were to be much more negative than those made in person. An additional study that looked at characterization of a romantic partner suggested that people are more likely to rely on “gut reactions” when meeting in person, but there isn’t sufficient information for this kind of evaluation when viewing someone online.

Professional

Non-verbal behaviors are particularly important to forming first impressions when meeting a business acquaintance.[27] Specifically, components of social expressivity, such as smiling, eyebrow position, emotional expression, and eye contact are emphasized.[2][25][27] Straightening one’s posture, leaning in slightly, and giving a firm handshake promotes favorable impression formation in the American business context.[27] Other impression management tactics in the business world include researching the organization and interviewers beforehand, preparing specific questions for the interviewer, showing confidence, and dressing appropriately.[28]

A qualitative review of previous literature looking at self-report data suggests that men and women use impression management tactics in the corporate world that are consistent with stereotypical gender roles when presenting themselves to others.[9] This research proposes that women are put in a double bind where those who portray themselves as more communal and submissive are overlooked for leadership positions and women who try to utilize male tactics (such as being more aggressive) receive negative consequences for violating normative gender roles.[9] To change this dynamic the authors suggest that managerial positions should be re-advertised to highlight the feminine qualities needed for a position and staff training should involve a segment accentuating gender issues in the office to make everyone aware of possible discrimination.[9]

Data collected from interviews with physicians distinguishes between first impressions and intuition and contributes to understanding the occurrence of gut feelings in the medical field.[29] Gut feelings go beyond first impressions: Physicians expressed feeling doubtful about their initial impressions as they gathered more data from their patients. More experienced physicians reported more instances of gut feelings than those less experienced, but the quality of the intuition was related to the quality of feedback received during the data collection process in general. Emotional engagement enhanced learning just as it does in first impressions.[29]

Neuroscience

First impressions are formed within milliseconds of seeing a target. When intentionally forming a first impression, encoding relies on the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC).[30] Readings from fMRIs of research participants show that processing of diagnostic information (e.g. distinguishing features) engaged the dmPFC more than processing neutral information.[30]

Participants generally formed more negative impressions of the faces that showed the negative emotion compared to neutral faces.[31] Results suggest that the dmPFC and amygdala together play a large role in negative impression formation. When forming immediate impressions based on emotion, the stimulus can bypass the neo-cortex by way of the “amygdala hijack.”[32]

Familiarity

Research indicates that people are efficient evaluators when forming impressions based on existing biases.[33] The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), amygdala, and the thalamus sort of relevant versus irrelevant information according to these biases. The dmPFC is also involved in the impression formation process, especially with person-descriptive information.[33]

FMRI results show activation of the fusiform cortex, posterior cingulate gyrus, and amygdala when individuals are asked to identify previously seen faces that were encoded as either “friends” or “foes.”[34] Additionally, the caudate and anterior cingulate cortex are more activated when looking at faces of “foes” versus “friends."[34] This research suggests that quick first impressions of hostility or support from unknown people can lead to long-term effects on memory that will later be associated with that person.

Alcohol and Impressions

Alcohol consumption and belief of consumption influenced emotion detection in ten second clips.[35] Participants who thought they had consumed an alcoholic beverage rated one facial expression (approximately 3% of the facial expressions they saw) more in each clip as happy compared to the control group. Thus, impression formation may be affected by even the perception of alcohol consumption.[35]

Cross-cultural Differences

There appears to be cross-cultural similarities in brain responses to first impression formations. In a mock election both American and Japanese individuals voted for the candidate that elicited a stronger response in their bilateral amygdala than those who did not, regardless of the candidate’s culture.[36] Individuals also showed a stronger response to cultural outgroup faces than cultural ingroup faces because the amygdala is presumably more sensitive to novel stimuli.[36] However, this finding was unrelated to actual voting decisions.[36]

Stability of First Impressions

Once formed, first impressions tend to be stable. A review of the literature on the accuracy and impact of first impressions on rater-based assessments found that raters’ first impressions are highly correlated with later scores, but it is unclear exactly why.[10] One study tested stability by asking participants to form impressions people based purely on photographs. Participants’ opinions of the people in photographs did not significantly differ after interacting with that person a month later.[26] One potential reason for this stability is that one's first impressions could serve as a guide for his/her next steps, such as what questions are asked and how raters go about scoring. More research needs to be done on the stability of first impressions to fully understand how first impressions guide subsequent treatment, self-fulfilling prophecies, and the halo effect.[3] Assessment tools are created can influence impressions too, ex. if a question provides only a dichotomous “yes” or “no” response or if a rater uses a scale (ratio). Although this study was conducted with the intention of improving rating methods in medical education, the literature review was sufficiently broad enough to generalize.[26]

References

  1. ^ a b Mackie, Eliot R. Smith, Diane M. (2007). Social psychology (3rd ed.). Hove: Psychology Press. ISBN 1841694096. Retrieved 05/08/14. {{cite book}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ a b c d Flora, Carlin. ""The First Impression"". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2011-02-20.. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i Aronson, Elliot, Robin M. Akert, Timothy D. Wilson, Robin M. (2007). Social psychology (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice-Hall. ISBN 0132382458. Retrieved 05/08/14. {{cite book}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)}}
  4. ^ a b c Biesanz, J.C. (2011). "Do we know when our impressions of others are valid? evidence for realistic accuracy awareness in first impressions of personality". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2 (5). doi:10.1177/1948550610397211. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  5. ^ a b c d e Bhargrave, R. "The social context of temporal sequences: Why first impressions shape shared experiences". Journal of Consumer Research. 40 (3): 501–517. doi:10.1086/671053. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  6. ^ a b c Ding, Jonathan Y. C. (12 January 2012). "Gay, Straight, or Somewhere in Between: Accuracy and Bias in the Perception of Bisexual Faces". Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 36 (2): 165–176. doi:10.1007/s10919-011-0129-y. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  7. ^ a b c d Naumann, L. P. (17 September 2009). "Personality Judgments Based on Physical Appearance". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 35 (12): 1661–1671. doi:10.1177/0146167209346309. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  8. ^ Trudeau, M. (5 May 2014). "You Had Me At Hello: The Science Behind First Impressions". NPR. Retrieved 6 May 2014.
  9. ^ a b c d Guadagno, Rosanna E. (21 March 2007). "Gender Differences in Impression Management in Organizations: A Qualitative Review". Sex Roles. 56 (7–8): 483–494. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9187-3. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  10. ^ a b Wood, T.J. "Exploring the role of first impressions in rater-based assessments". Advances in Health Sciences Education. doi:10.1007/s10459-013-9453-9.
  11. ^ a b c d Willis, J. (2006). "First impressions: Making up your mind after 100 ms exposure to a face". Psychological Science (17): 592-598. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  12. ^ a b c Wargo, E (2006). "How many seconds to a first impression?". The Observer. 19.
  13. ^ Carlson, E. N. (1 January 2010). "Do We Know the First Impressions We Make? Evidence for Idiographic Meta-Accuracy and Calibration of First Impressions". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 1 (1): 94–98. doi:10.1177/1948550609356028. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  14. ^ a b Zebrowitz, Leslie A. "Older and younger adults' first impressions from faces: Similar in agreement but different in positivity". Psychology and Aging. 28 (1): 202–212. doi:10.1037/a0030927. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  15. ^ a b c Fang, X. (2013). "The Role of National Culture and Multimedia on First Impression Bias Reduction: An Experimental Study in US and China". IEEE Transactions On Professional Communication. 56 (4): 354–371. doi:10.1109/TPC.2013.2251503. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  16. ^ a b Lim, K. H. (2000). "The role of multimedia in changing first impression bias". Information Systems Research. 11 (2): 115–136. doi:10.1287/isre.11.2.115.11776. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  17. ^ a b Deprez-Sims, Anne-Sophie. "Accents in the workplace: Their effects during a job interview". International Journal of Psychology. 45 (6): 417–426. doi:10.1080/00207594.2010.499950. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  18. ^ a b Zebrowitz, L. A. (19 December 2011). "First Impressions From Faces Among U.S. and Culturally Isolated Tsimane' People in the Bolivian Rainforest". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 43 (1): 119–134. doi:10.1177/0022022111411386. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  19. ^ a b c Workman, J. E. (1 September 1991). "The Role of Cosmetics in Impression Formation". Clothing and Textiles Research Journal. 10 (1): 63–67. doi:10.1177/0887302X9101000109. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  20. ^ a b Olivola, Christopher Y. "Fooled by first impressions? Reexamining the diagnostic value of appearance-based inferences". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 46 (2): 315–324. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.002. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  21. ^ a b c Todorov, A. (10 June 2005). "Inferences of Competence from Faces Predict Election Outcomes". Science. 308 (5728): 1623–1626. doi:10.1126/science.1110589.
  22. ^ a b Lorenzo, G. L. (4 November 2010). "What Is Beautiful Is Good and More Accurately Understood: Physical Attractiveness and Accuracy in First Impressions of Personality". Psychological Science. 21 (12): 1777–1782. doi:10.1177/0956797610388048. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  23. ^ a b McDermott, L. (2011). "The influence of clothing fashion and race on the perceived socioeconomic status and person perception of college students". Psychology & Society. 4: 64–75. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  24. ^ a b Sritharan, R. (2010). "I think I like you: Spontaneous and deliberate evaluations of potential romantic partners in an online dating context". European Journal of Social Psychology. 40: 1062-1077. doi:10.1002/ejsp.703. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  25. ^ a b Weisbuch, M. (2009). "On being liked on the web and in the 'real world': Consistency in first impressions across personal web pages and spontaneous behavior". Journal Of Experimental Social Psychology. 45 (3): 573–576. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.009. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  26. ^ a b c Wood, J. "The Power of a First Impression". Psych Central. Retrieved 6 May 2014.
  27. ^ a b c Goman, Carol Kinsey (2008). The nonverbal advantage : secrets and science of body language at work ([Online-Ausg.]. ed.). San Francisco, Calif.: Berrett-Koehler Publishers. ISBN 1576754928.
  28. ^ Rowh, Mark. "First Impressions Count". American Psychological Association. Retrieved 04/18/14. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help)
  29. ^ a b Woolley, A. (2013). "Clinical intuition in family medicine: More than first impressions". Annals Of Family Medicine. 11: 60–66. doi:10.1370/afm.1433. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  30. ^ a b Gilron, R. (2012). "Remembering first impressions: Effects of intentionality and diagnosticity on subsequent memory". Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 12 (1): 85–98. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0074-6. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  31. ^ Iidaka, T. (2011). "Forming a negative impression of another person correlates with activation in medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala". Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosciene. 6 (4): 516–525. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  32. ^ Goleman, Daniel (1999). Working with Emotional Intelligence. p. 87.
  33. ^ a b Schiller, D. (2009). "A neural mechanism of first impressions". Nature Neuroscience. 12: 508–514. doi:10.1038/nn.2278. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  34. ^ a b Vrticka, Pascal. "Memory for friends or foes: The social context of past encounters with faces modulates their subsequent neural traces in the brain". Social Neuroscience. 4 (5): 384–401. doi:10.1080/17470910902941793. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  35. ^ a b Walter (2009). "Preceding attention and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex: Process specificity versus domain dependence. Hum. Brain Mapp". 30: 312–326. doi:10.1002/hbm.20506. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  36. ^ a b c Rule, N. O. (5 December 2009). "Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across cultures". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 5 (2–3): 349–355. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp046. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)