User:Craigjclemson/sandbox3

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

NEW REFERENCES: 1 - 5

In mathematics, physics, and engineering, spatial frequency is a characteristic of any structure that is periodic across position in space. The spatial frequency is a measure of how often sinusoidal components (as determined by the Fourier transform) of the structure repeat per unit of distance. The SI unit of spatial frequency is cycles per meter. In image processing applications, spatial frequency is often expressed in units of cycles per millimeter and sometimes incorrectly in units of line pairs per millimeter (incorrect since a line pair may describe a square wave but not a sinusoidal wave).

In wave mechanics, the spatial frequency is related to the wavelength by the formula

Likewise, the wave number (e.g. in radians per meter) is related to spatial frequency and wavelength by

Visual perception[edit]

In the study of visual perception, sinusoidal gratings are frequently used to probe the capabilities of the visual system. In these stimuli, spatial frequency is expressed as the number of cycles per degree of visual angle. Sine-wave gratings also differ from one another in amplitude (the magnitude of difference in intensity between light and dark stripes), and angle.

Spatial-frequency theory[edit]

The spatial-frequency theory refers to the theory that the visual cortex operates on a code of spatial frequency, not on the code of straight edges and lines hypothesised by Hubel and Wiesel. [1] [2] In support of this theory is the experimental observation that the visual cortex neurons respond even more robustly to sine-wave gratings that are placed at specific angles in their receptive fields that they do to edges or bars. Most neurons in the primary visual cortex respond best when a sine-wave grating of a particular frequency is presented at a particular angle in a particular location in the visual field [3]

The spatial-frequency theory of vision is based on two physical principles:

  1. Any visual stimulus can be represented by plotting the intensity of the light along lines running through it.
  2. Any curve can be broken down into constituent sine waves by Fourier analysis.

The theory states that in each functional module of the visual cortex Fourier analysis is performed on the receptive field and the neurons in each module are thought to respond selectively to various orientations and frequencies of sine wave gratings. When all of the visual cortex neurons that are influenced by a specific scene respond together the perception of the scene is created by the summation of the various sine-wave gratings. One is generally not aware of the individual spatial frequency components since all of the elements are essentially blended together into one smooth representation. However, computer-based filtering procedures can be used to deconstruct an image into it's individual spatial frequency components. [4] Research on spatial frequency detection by visual neurons complements and extends previous research using straight edges rather than refuting it. [5]

Further research shows that different spatial frequencies convey different information about the appearance of a stimulus. High spatial frequencies represent abrupt spatial changes in the image, such as edges, and generally correspond to featural information and fine detail. Low spatial frequencies, on the other hand, represent global information about the shape, such as general orientation and proportions.[6] Rapid and specialised perception of faces is known to rely more on low spatial frequency information. [7] In the general population of adults, the threshold for spatial frequency discrimination is about 7%. It is often poorer in dyslexic individuals.[8]

Sinusoidal gratings and Michelson equation[edit]

There is an important quantitative concept related to spatial frequency, known as the Michelson equation:

In layman's terms, this is the ratio of the crest-trough distance to the maximum thereof, which is twice the average. One degree on the visual field represents four cycles on the wave pattern.[9]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Martinez, L. M., & ALonzo, J. M. (2203). Complex receptive fields in primary visual cortex. The Neuroscientist, 9, 317-331
  2. ^ De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K. (1988). Spatial vision. New York: Oxford University Press.
  3. ^ Issa, N. P., Trepel, C., & Stryker, M. P. (2000). Spatial frequency maps in cat visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8504-8514
  4. ^ Blake, R. and Sekuler, R., Perception, 3rd ed. Chapter 3. ISBN 978-0072887600
  5. ^ Pinel, J. P. J., Biopsychology, 6th ed. 293-294. ISBN 0-205-42651-4
  6. ^ Bar M (Aug 2004). "Visual objects in context". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5 (8): 617–29. doi:10.1038/nrn1476. PMID 15263892.
    Box 2: Spatial frequencies and the information they convey
    {{cite journal}}: External link in |quote= (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  7. ^ Awasthi B, Friedman J, Williams, MA (2011). "Faster, stronger, lateralized: Low spatial frequency information supports face processing". Neuropsychologia. 49 (13): 3583–3590. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.027. PMID 21939676.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Ben-Yehudah G, Ahissar M (May 2004). "Sequential spatial frequency discrimination is consistently impaired among adult dyslexics". Vision Res. 44 (10): 1047–63. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2003.12.001. PMID 15031099.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  9. ^ "Vision" McGraw-Hill Encyclopedia of Science & Technology, vol. 19, p.292 1997

External links[edit]

Category:Mathematical physics Category:Visual perception