User talk:Misaacso

Page contents not supported in other languages.
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Psychology of Language[edit]

Welcome to the Psychology of Language Wikipedia course project. Below are links related to our course that may be helpful to you.

Let me know if you have any questions. --Kechambers (talk) 19:49, 16 February 2012 (UTC)[reply]

Articles I am interested in[edit]

Babbling

Baby talk

Andrew's Suggestions[edit]

This looks good overall. The material is well-written, well-researched, and effectively explains what babbling is and how it works. I have one concern, and one suggestion. One random note: you mention "reduplicated babbling" but never actually define it.

I'm not entire sure how this will be integrated into the existing article. I'm sure you guys will figure it out, but as it reads now, it feels like a stand-alone article on babbling. As you move on to your next draft, it may be worth figuring out a way to integrate what you've written into the original article — or creating a new article called "linguistics of babbling" or something.

I would like to see some distinct section titles and a few more paragraph breaks. Big sections, like the physiology of babbling, the stages of babbling, and even the hypothesis about the purpose of babbling could be given headers to break the content apart.

I think your discussion of the stages of babbling can be split into two paragraphs, one describing what babbling is and providing a general introduction and one about the stages. You can put the break before the phrase, "During the canonical stage..." Later, the section about the continuity and discontinuity hypotheses could also be split with one paragraph for each hypothesis.

The section on physiology feels like it would better serve the article if you moved it right before the paragraph that begins with "Although the anatomical changes..." The previous paragraph intimates that a more detailed explanation of physiology is coming up but then it never gets explored in detail. Putting your current section on physiology there would be a nice segue into your later discussions about the differences between anatomy and social influence.

In your section on bird babbling, you mention that humans learn language "through similar means" but don't elaborate on what those means are — it'd be nice to have some sort of clearer idea of the two compare.

This sentence is a bit long and awkward: "Another important factor is the physiology of the animal where brain regions used in analyzing and processing information and the properties of the ear and vocal tract are critical determinants to how song is interpreted and later produced." I wouldn't use "where" in the context.

The birdsong section as a whole is interesting, but it doesn't really seem to have any information regarding babbling. There are a lot of general statements that I'd be okay with if they were setting up background information that led into a discussion about songbird babbling, but as it is now, it feels more like an article summary than an encyclopedia article on bird babbling.

I feel like the pygmy marmoset section is also more of a summary than an article. It does a better job of talking about babbling, but it still feels too summary-ish to me.

The bat section seems less about babbling and more about the differences in bat vocalizations between sexes. It's definitely INTERESTING, but I'm not sure it's relevant.

The animal babbling section may be better served by combining it into smaller, briefer section. It seems like a topic that would be best-served by an entire article, but that's a lot of work to ask you to do.

Your section on deaf infants repeats a few sentences about the canonical stage. You should probably take these out. Maybe you can replace them with a link to that section in the article?

It also seems to be about two things: language deficits in deaf children and manual babbling in deaf children. The first three paragraphs are interesting but not about babbling as much as they are about language development. The really important stuff about manual babbling is at the end, and that's what I think you should stick with. — Preceding unsigned comment added by AndFred (talkcontribs) 14:12, 12 April 2012 (UTC)[reply]

Babbling Edit[edit]

This section will contain edits to the Wikipedia article "Babbling". Those edits will be posted to this page.

  • Move parantheses in first paragraph to physiology

http://www.speech-therapy-information-and-resources.com/speech-development.html

  • Number 3 on babbling page is yellow and blue book, add in babbling patterns differ across language supporting the idea that speech perception determines some aspects of babbling. It is modified to sound like an infants native language, by exposure to the language.
  • Rid of the sentences starting with names
  • Make note on babbling talk page that the human babbling section does not have verifiable resources, post our "replacement" and ask if anyone has any objections

Babbling in Humans[edit]

Babbling is the first and earliest sign of human language. [1] These vocalizations do not generally contain meaning or refer to anything specific. The sounds and expressions of babbling are produced before an infant begins to construct recognizable words. [2] This is mainly due to the immaturity of the vocal tract and neuromusculature at this age in life. [3] Upon reaching an age of 6 months, infants are finally able to control the opening and closing of the vocal tract. They therefore can begin to distinguish between the different sounds of vowels and consonants. This period is known as the beginning of the canonical stage. These first forms of sound production are the easiest for children to use and are different from the growling and squealing that is displayed in earlier periods of life. During the canonical stage, the babbling involves reduplicated sounds containing alternations of vowels and consonants (ie; baba or bobo). Before this stage is reached, infants mainly display cooing and gurgling (ie; oooh and ahh). By the time an infant reaches 8-9 months, they display productions of more advanced sounds known as variegated babbling. Variegated babbling differs greatly from reduplicated babbling. This stage includes more complex combinations of consonant and vowel syllables (ie; babadoobe). (6) By 9-10 months of age, infant babbling begins to resemble the native language of a child. The general structure of the syllables that they are producing is very closely related to the sounds of their native language. The form of babbling significantly predicts the form of early words. (12) Infants also begin to use intonation patterns and timing that matches the characteristics of their language. (6)

Types of Babbling[edit]

There are two types of babbling. Reduplicated babbling consists of repeated syllables, such as "ba-ba-ba-ba-ba-ba-ba" or "goo-goo", whereas variegated, or non-reduplicated babbling consists of a mix of syllables, such as "ka-da-bu-ba-mi-doy-doy-doy".[3]

The consonants that babbling infants produce tend to be any of the following : /p, b, t, m, d, n, k, ɡ, s, h, w, j/. The following consonants tend to be infrequently produced during phonological development : /f, v, θ, ð, ʃ, tʃ, dʒ, l, r, ŋ/. The complex nature of sounds that developing children produce make them difficult to categorize, but the above rules tend to hold true regardless of the language to which children are exposed.[4]

The Link Between Babbling and Language[edit]

Two hypotheses have been devised in order to explain how babbling is related to language development.

  • The Continuity Hypothesis - According to these claims, babbling is a direct forerunner to language. At first, infants produce universal sounds that exist in all areas of the world and in all languages. As infants are reinforced by caregivers and exposed to a specific language, they begin to produce sounds similar to their native language. (3 on babbling page) This hypothesis relates to the claim that the anatomical changes of the vocal tract are very important, but the social environment in which an infant is raised has a greater influence on the development of language. Infants pay close attention to their caregivers reactions and use their feedback as approval to the sounds that they are making. This reinforcement through feedback helps infants to focus their attention on very specific features of sound. Social feedback facilitates faster learning and earlier production of a variety of advanced words. (7)
  • The Discontinuity Hypothesis - This alternative idea suggests that babbling has absolutely no relationship to language development. According to this hypothesis, infants produce sounds in no particular order and as they grow, they may drop certain sounds only to pick them up in later months. This is a giant step backwards and infants may incorrectly and inconsistently use sounds that they have already conquered in the early babbling stages. Sometimes, they lose them all together before learning how to speak. Over time, infants will relearn sounds again during the course and development of language. (6) The hypothesis also implies that when children finally reach the age where they are able to learn their native language, they develop phonological sounds in an orderly manner. There can be a period of silence between productions of babbling and productions of the first words in speech.

There is no clear evidence for either of these hypotheses, leaving them both as possibilities for connections between the speech stages in life. (3 on babbling page, book)

As suggested in the continuity hypothesis, there is some evidence that babbling varies depending on the linguistic environment in which a baby is raised. For example, it has been noted that infants raised in French speaking environments display greater amounts of rising intonation in comparison to infants raised in English speaking environments. This is due to the differences between French and English intonations while speaking. By comparing infants in English, French, Swedish and Japanese linguistic backgrounds, babbling reveals that placement of consonants and vowels also resembles native languages. These findings support another hypothesis; the “babbling drift hypothesis” in which infant babbling resembles the phonetic characteristics of their native language through exposure of speech. When infants are exposed to two languages after birth, they typically babble in their dominant language. The dominant language is considered to be the one that children has the most exposure to. Infants do not produce a blend of languages while babbling and sometimes they may choose which language they prefer to babble in based upon particular features. (10)

Physiology of Babbling[edit]

A human mouth moves in distinct ways during speech production. The International Phonetic Alphabet was formed in 1897 as a representation of the sounds produced by language. When saying each individual sound out loud, it is noticeable that a human uses different parts of their mouth, as well as different methods to differentiate a particular sound. During the beginnings of babbling, infants tend to have greater mouth openings on the right side. This finding suggests that babbling is controlled by the left hemisphere of the brain.

Reduplicated babbling (such as bababa) involves a rhythmic opening and closing of the jaw. (1) According to the Frame Dominance Theory, when the mandible (jaw) is elevated, a consonant sound will be produced. When the mandible is depressed, a vowel like sound is produced. Therefore, during a reduplicated sequence of sounds, the consonant and vowels are alternated as the jaw elevates and depresses. The opening and closing of the mouth alone will not produce babbling. Phonation is necessary during the entire movement in order to create a meaningful sound. Other important body parts involved in articulation, such as the tongue, lips and teeth remain in a stable resting position during babbling. (3) Tongue position is not always controlled during an open and close cycle of the mouth. Sound is not constantly produced throughout these cycles during the babbling period and the motion can be made witout any vocalization at all. (12) Deaf infants produce manual babbling through similar rhythmic alternations of the mouth opening and closing, but they perform with their hands instead. As a baby begins to produce sounds beyond the reduplicated sequences of babbling, they exhibit equal sized mouth openings on the right and left sides. (1)

Crucially, the larynx or voicebox, originally high in the throat to let the baby breathe while swallowing, descends during the first year of life, allowing a pharynx to develop and all the sounds of human speech to be formed (THEIR SOURCE #1) - rid of in first paragraph

Babbling in Deaf Infants[edit]

Vocal Babbling[edit]

It has been moderately challenging to study deaf infants in the past because it is uncommon to diagnose hearing disabilities within the first year of a child’s life. It is also difficult to locate deaf infants that have had severely impaired hearing since birth, have been diagnosed within the first year of their lives, and do not suffer from any other impairments.

Research has been conducted to question whether or not infants who have impaired hearing can demonstrate such vocal sounds. Conclusions have been reached that deaf children acquire signs for the same concepts that are present in English speaking children (12), but deaf infants do not reach this stage of babbling until 10 months or later.(2) This is because deaf infants lack auditory experience. Babbling of any language type appears if the child is exposed to a certain mode of communication so babbling can be delayed or non-existent for deaf children. (12) Not only are they are significantly delayed in language development in comparison to their speaking counterparts, but they also produce fewer noises.(5) Due to the Findings relating to this babbling delay, researchers have rejected the belief that language was an innate capability that humans have and supports the suggestion that auditory speech is necessary in language development. (3)

There are exceptions to these studies on the occasion that infants are not completely impaired of all hearing. Children with varying degrees of hearing loss display different speech signals and babbling. Those with more severe hearing loss have less experience with auditory communication and therefore show worse canonical babbling and language production. Some deaf infants will never reach the canonical stage of babbling, thus never speaking at all. (5) In order for hearing impaired humans to gain auditory experience, a number of solutions have been implemented. Hearing aids can be used to help infants reach babbling stages earlier. (2) Cochlear implants have also been tested. Once the surgical implantation is complete, an infant begins to listen and have experience with language outputs. As soon as language has been heard, they begin to babble and speak in rhythmic patterns just as normal hearing infants do. (3)

Manual Babbling[edit]

Although all infants move their hands in imitation to what is modeled in their environment, at around 9-12 months deaf infants begin to produce gestures that are distinct from all other hand movements. This was originally thought to represent the first indicators of sign language. Just as hearing infants babble with their mouths, deaf infants babble with their hands. Children are able to produce manual articulation of words correctly, which is important since many articulation tendencies of manual babbling transfer to the children’s early sign production and then later into the production of words. (12) Interestingly enough, if a hearing infant has deaf parents, they will still imitate the signs that they see their parents displaying. This is evidence that manual and vocal babbling is possible in both hearing and deaf infants. (1)

After it was established that deaf infants could babble through other means than their mouths, the patterns in which productions occurred were studied. Hearing and speaking infants follow identical maturational paths in language acquisition. Both go through a number of stages, and exhibit similar complexity in their babbling sequences. In studies where deaf and children with hearing were compared, deaf children produced more multi-movement manual babbling than children with their hearing. (12) Deaf babbling was titled manual babbling, and is structurally identical to vocal babbling in its development. The most common sign in manual babbling is where all fingers are extended and spread and it is also related to the first signs an infant will make. (12) There are three main components to manual babbling. The hand gestures contain a restricted set of phonetic units, a syllabic organization and are used without reference or meaning. This is comparable to the important aspects of vocal babblings. (1)


  • Around 9-12 months, deaf infants product prelinguistic gestures that appear to be very different from all other hand movements. This was examined and determined to be the beginnings of the first signs of sign language. Deaf infants could babbling with their hands. (1)
  • Hearing and speaking children follow near identical maturational paths in language acquisition. They experience the same stages, exhibit similar complexity in utterances and gestures. (1)
  • Manual babbling is structurally identical to vocal babbling. (1)
  • Manual babbling has 3 important... (1)
    • Restricted set of phonetic units
    • Possess syllabic organization
    • Is used without meaning or reference to anything
  • By the canonical stage (7-10 months), infants generally utter sounds characterized by repetitions of certain sequences such as dadada or baba. Research has shown that deaf infants reach this stage later in life than regular hearing infants. (2)
  • It has been moderately challenging to study deaf infants during the past because it is uncommon to diagnose hearing disabilities within the first year of a child’s life. It is also difficult to find deaf infants with no other impairments, who have had severely impaired hearing since birth and have been diagnosed within the first year of their lives. (2)
    • Newborn hearing screen is not a required part of care for infants. The average age in which impairment is now identified is 3 months, in comparison to 2.5 years (5)
  • Results showed that normal hearing infants reached the canonical stage of speech by 7-10 months. On the other hand, deaf infants did not reach this stage until after 10 months. (2)
  • Overall, hearing impaired infants show significant delays in reaching the canonical stage of language development. Oller and Eilers concluded this to be due to their inability to hear auditory speech. (2)
  • Hearing aids can assist infants in reaching babbling stages earlier. Completely deaf infants may never reach the canonical stage. (2)
  • Both hearing and deaf infants show similar patterns of growls, squeals and whispers at the precanonical stage, but once the infant reached an age where language was to develop further, audition and modeling played a far more important role. This significantly leaves deaf children behind in the speech department. (2)
  • Rhythmic syllable like vocalizations in canonical babbling require auditory input (3)
  • Cochlear implants can initiate babbling but only after the device has been implanted and the child has listened and had experiences with syllabic outputs (3)
  • Cochlear implants produce significant increases in syllabic production (3)
  • Hearing infants with deaf parents will imitate the signs that they see their parents exhibiting (4)
  • Nonreferential gestures produced before signs (4)
  • Children with varying degrees of hearing loss display difference speech signals and babbling. Those with more severe hearing loss, hear less audibility of speech therefore performing worse in canonical babbling (5)
  • Not only delay in babbling, but also a reduction in how much babbling (5) — Preceding unsigned comment added by Amf14 (talkcontribs) 14:22, 13 April 2012 (UTC)[reply]

Nonhuman Babbling[edit]

Songbirds[edit]

Not only are songbird and human language parallel regarding neural and molecular factors, they also are similar in how their communication is initially produced. Observations about these similarities can be traced back to Darwin and his studies. Avian and mammalian brains are similar in form and connectivity and there may even be a gene that is relevant to speech found in both organisms. The learning of a song is produced through a mix of interaction, experience, and predisposition as shown when young songbirds will imitate their species call when presented with songs from their own and another species. They are physically capable of producing either song, but do not. Humans learn language through similar means, which is why this early vocalization in songbirds is considered babbling. [4]

Another important factor is the physiology of the animal where brain regions used in analyzing and processing information and the properties of the ear and vocal tract are critical determinants to how song is interpreted and later produced. In studies using isolated birds that have not had exposure to song, they produce an abnormal ‘isolate song’ that still retains species-specific aspects. This shows that the neural pathways have predetermined features that allow for such a phenomenon to occur. The pathways are able to allow for plasticity of the songs that can be learned in the future. [4]

There is an important phase in development when song learning is best accomplished. This phase is called the ‘sensitive period’ and the amount of change that a songbird experiences in adulthood varies by species. Young birds have a production phase after a listening phase of development. The production of song is called ‘subsong’ where vocalizations resemble that of an adult as time passes. Memory for songs are able to form before the period where learning to sing occurs. Social interaction is important when dealing with vocal learning where non-singing females can even influence an infant through feedback. [4]


  • In song birds, subsong and plastic song phases have been linked to vocal practice for adult songs , occurs near puberty rather than infancy -REWORD(8) — Preceding unsigned comment added by Amf14 (talkcontribs) 14:38, 22 March 2012 (UTC)[reply]

Pygmy Marmosets (Cebuella pygmaea)[edit]

Pygmy marmosets have been studied and found to produce complex vocalizations 2-3 weeks after birth. Both sexes are capable of creating calls at a rate of 3 calls/second and each bout of calls can last up to 6 or 7 minutes. A normal series of calls for this creature contains approximately 10 different call types. This abundance of call forms produced by pygmy marmosets is comparable to babbling in human infants for a multitude of reasons. Like reduplicative babbling in humans, the call type is often repeated several times before a new sequence of sounds is produced. The vocalizations also gain attention from caregivers and provide practice for future vocal behavior. For these reasons, pygmy marmoset calls are also distinguished as babbling behavior. [5]

There are a total of 16 call types in pygmy marmoset babbling language. Different calls serve different survival functions such as when desiring food and social interaction or during times of alarm. As human infants do, marmoset babies have higher rates of social interaction when producing babbling sounds. During the juvenile age, marmosets often regress back to babbling stages if a new infant is born. It is suggested that their production of babbling calls increases because they are seeking attention and social interaction. Another babbling occurrence during the juvenile age is the addition of territorial calls and mild threat vocalizations. Although babbling is important for practicing adult calls during the juvenile age, babbling decreases with age in pygmy marmosets. Overall, babbling progresses through a series of stages from infancy to adulthood and slowly leads to the construction of adult calls. [5]

Sac-Winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata)[edit]

Babbling-like behavior in songbirds, humans and some nonhuman primates has been previously researched, but it has not been seen until now in non-primate mammals. The Sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata) is a social creature and the vocalizations that they produce depend on the social situation the animal is in. This bat has a large repertoire of vocalizations with males being more vocal than females. Echolocation pulses, barks, chatters, and screeches are used in various social situations including courtship and territorial defense. Infants produce isolation calls if their mothers are absent, but the pups also produce vocalizations that mirror those of adults. Both sexes of infants vocalize, even though as an adult, the vocalizations are solely produced by males. Social context, mothers, and surrounding bats do not influence pups because the multiple vocalizations are combined regardless to the situation. Since there is not a social aspect correlated with the vocalizations, the productions of the sounds suggest that the pups vocalize for training. The pups repeat and combine adult vocalizations so that they resembles babbling in what humans, other primates and some songbirds do as infants. However, there are no social responses to babbling in bats, while in humans babbling increases social interactions. Babbling is common in infants that have a large repertoire of adult vocalizations to learn and this is seen in the pups of Sac-Winged bat. [6]

Citations[edit]

1. Pepito website 1 http://petitto.gallaudet.edu/~petitto/index/lab-info-HowChildrenAcquireLanguages.php

2. The role of audition in infant babbling (psychINFO) Oller, D., & Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Development, 59(2), 441-449. Doi:10.2307/113023 (On babbling page is citation #4)

3. Co-occurrence Patterns in the Babbling of Children with a Cochlear Implant

4. How do deaf infants attain first signs 2

5. The Relationship of Audibility and the Development of Canonical Babbling in Young Children With Hearing Impairment by Sandie M. Bass-Ringdahl

6. INFLUENCES ON INFANT SPEECH PROCESSING: Toward a New Synthesis 3

7. Social Feedback to Infants’ Babbling Facilitates Rapid Phonological Learning by Michael H. Goldstein and Jennifer A. Schwade (On babbling page is citation #2)

8. ‘BABBLING’ IN PYGMY MARMOSETS: DEVELOPMENT AFTER INFANCY

9. Knornschild, M., Behr, O. & Von Helversen, O. (2006). Babbling behavior in the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata). Naturwissenschaften, 93, 451-454. doi: 10.1007/s00114-006-0127-9.

10. Language Differentiation in Bilingual Infants by Diane Poulin-Dubois and Naomi Goodz — Preceding unsigned comment added by Amf14 (talkcontribs) 14:44, 29 March 2012 (UTC)[reply]

11. Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K., & Scharff, C. (2010). Twitter evolution: Converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 747-759. doi:10.1038/nrn2931

12. Cheek, A., Cormier, K., Repp, A., & Meier, R. P. (2001). Prelinguistic gesture predicts mastery and error in the production of early signs. Language, 77(2), 292-323.


Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51(2), 419-439.

Welcome to Wikipedia: check out the Teahouse![edit]

Teahouse logo
Hello! Misaacso, you are invited to the Teahouse, a forum on Wikipedia for new editors to ask questions about editing Wikipedia, and get support from peers and experienced editors. Please join us! Sarah (talk) 22:20, 27 March 2012 (UTC)[reply]
   ==References==
  1. ^ Petitto, Laura Ann. "How Children Acquire Language".
  2. ^ Takei, Wataru (2011). "How do deaf infants attain first signs?". Developmental Science. 4: 71–78. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00150. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  3. ^ Werker, Janet F (1999). "Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis". Annual Review of Psychology. 50: 509–535. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.509.
  4. ^ a b c Bolhuis, J. J. (2010). "Twitter evolution: Converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 11: 747–759. doi:10.1038/nrn2931. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  5. ^ a b Snowdon, C. T. (2001). "'Babbling' in pygmy marmosets: Development after infancy". Behaviour. 138 (10): 1235–1248. doi:10.1163/15685390152822193. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  6. ^ Knornschild, M. (2006). "Babbling behavior in the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata)". Naturwissenschaften. 93: 451–454. doi:10.1007/s00114-006-0127-9. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)

Wikipedia Education Program Student Survey[edit]

Hi! Please take a few minutes to fill out this survey about the Wikipedia Education Program. This is our opportunity to improve the program and resources we provide students, so your feedback and input is integral to our future success. Thank you so much! JMathewson (WMF) (talk) 23:53, 22 May 2012 (UTC)[reply]