Jump to content

Ecological fallacy: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
SmackBot (talk | contribs)
m Date maintenance tags and general fixes: build 414:
Line 22: Line 22:
The term comes from a 1950 paper by William S. Robinson.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Robinson, W.S.|year=1950|title=Ecological Correlations and the Behavior of Individuals|journal=American Sociological Review|volume=15|pages=351–357|doi=10.2307/2087176}}</ref> For each of the 48 states in the US as of the [[U.S. Census|1930 census]], he computed the literacy rate and the proportion of the population born outside the US. He showed that these two figures were associated with a positive correlation of 0.53 — in other words, the greater the proportion of immigrants in a state, the higher its average literacy. However, when individuals are considered, the correlation was &minus;0.11 — immigrants were on average less literate than native citizens. Robinson showed that the positive correlation at the level of state populations was because immigrants tended to settle in states where the native population was more literate. He cautioned against deducing conclusions about individuals on the basis of population-level, or "ecological" data.
The term comes from a 1950 paper by William S. Robinson.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Robinson, W.S.|year=1950|title=Ecological Correlations and the Behavior of Individuals|journal=American Sociological Review|volume=15|pages=351–357|doi=10.2307/2087176}}</ref> For each of the 48 states in the US as of the [[U.S. Census|1930 census]], he computed the literacy rate and the proportion of the population born outside the US. He showed that these two figures were associated with a positive correlation of 0.53 — in other words, the greater the proportion of immigrants in a state, the higher its average literacy. However, when individuals are considered, the correlation was &minus;0.11 — immigrants were on average less literate than native citizens. Robinson showed that the positive correlation at the level of state populations was because immigrants tended to settle in states where the native population was more literate. He cautioned against deducing conclusions about individuals on the basis of population-level, or "ecological" data.


An early example of the ecological fallacy was [[Émile Durkheim#Suicide|Émile Durkheim]]'s 1897 [[Suicide (book)|study of suicide]] in Germany, although this has been debated by somebody.{{Citation needed|date=May 2010}}
An early example of the ecological fallacy was [[Émile Durkheim#Suicide|Émile Durkheim]]'s 1897 [[Suicide (book)|study of suicide]] in Germany, although this has been debated by some.{{Citation needed|date=May 2010}}


==Inverse error==
==Inverse error==

Revision as of 21:03, 12 June 2010

An ecological fallacy (or ecological inference fallacy) is an error in the interpretation of statistical data in an ecological study, whereby inferences about the nature of specific individuals are based solely upon aggregate statistics collected for the group to which those individuals belong. This fallacy assumes that individual members of a group have the average characteristics of the group at large. Stereotypes are one form of ecological fallacy, which assumes that groups are homogeneous. For example, if a particular group of people are measured to have a lower average IQ than the general population, it is an error to assume that all members of that group have a lower IQ than the general population. For any given individual from that group, there is no way to know if that person has a lower than average IQ, average IQ, or above average IQ compared to the general population.

Examples

A study is done that shows people from City A score higher on college entry exams, on average, than people from City B. This does not mean that a randomly selected individual from A will usually score higher than a randomly selected individual from B. This is because the distribution of scores might be very different between the cities. Consider this synthetic example:

  • City A: 80% of people got 40 points and 20% of them got 95 points. The average score is 51 points.
  • City B: 50% of people got 45 points and 50% got 55 points. The average score is 50 points.
  • If we pick two people at random from A and B, there are 4 possible outcomes:
    • A - 40, B - 45 (B wins, 40% probability)
    • A - 40, B - 55 (B wins, 40% probability)
    • A - 95, B - 45 (A wins, 10% probability)
    • A - 95, B - 55 (A wins, 10% probability)
  • Although City A has a higher average score, 80% of the time a random inhabitant of A will score lower than a random inhabitant of B.

If a particular sports team is described as performing poorly, it would be fallacious to conclude that each player on that team performs poorly. Because the performance of the team depends on each player, one excellent player and two terrible players may average out to three poor players. This does not diminish the excellence of the one player.

In the United States presidential elections of 2000, 2004, and 2008, wealthier states (states with higher per capita incomes) tended to vote Democratic and poorer states tended to vote Republican. Yet wealthier voters tended to vote Republican and poorer voters tended to vote Democratic. For example, in 2004, the Republican candidate, George W. Bush, won the fifteen poorest states, and the Democratic candidate, John Kerry, won 9 of the 11 wealthiest states. Yet 62% of voters with annual incomes over $200,000 voted for Bush, but only 36% of voters with annual incomes of $15,000 or less voted for Bush.[1]

The ecological fallacy was discussed in a court challenge to the Washington gubernatorial election, 2004 in which a number of illegal voters were identified, after the election; their votes were unknown, because the vote was by secret ballot. The challengers argued that illegal votes cast in the election would have followed the voting patterns of the precincts in which they had been cast, and thus adjustments should be made accordingly.[2] An expert witness said this approach was like trying to figure out Ichiro Suzuki's batting average by looking at the batting average of the entire Seattle Mariners team since the illegal votes were cast only by males and the overall precinct votes included both males and females. The judge determined that the challengers' argument was an ecological fallacy, and rejected it.[3]

Origin of concept

The term comes from a 1950 paper by William S. Robinson.[4] For each of the 48 states in the US as of the 1930 census, he computed the literacy rate and the proportion of the population born outside the US. He showed that these two figures were associated with a positive correlation of 0.53 — in other words, the greater the proportion of immigrants in a state, the higher its average literacy. However, when individuals are considered, the correlation was −0.11 — immigrants were on average less literate than native citizens. Robinson showed that the positive correlation at the level of state populations was because immigrants tended to settle in states where the native population was more literate. He cautioned against deducing conclusions about individuals on the basis of population-level, or "ecological" data.

An early example of the ecological fallacy was Émile Durkheim's 1897 study of suicide in Germany, although this has been debated by some.[citation needed]

Inverse error

The opposite of the ecological fallacy is the Hasty generalization, in which one makes a generalization about a group based on insufficient data. For example, if one made a generalization about an entire group based only on a few members of that group, then one would be making a hasty generalization.

See also

References

  1. ^ Gelman, Andrew; Park, David; Shor, Boris; Bafumi, Joseph; Cortina, Jeronimo (2008). Red State, Blue State, Rich State, Poor State. Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-13927-2.
  2. ^ George Howland Jr. (May 18, 2005). "The Monkey Wrench Trial: Dino Rossi's challenge of the 2004 election is on shaky legal ground. But if he prevails, watch litigation become an option in close races everywhere". Seattle Weekly.
  3. ^ Borders et al. v. King County et al., transcript of the decision by Chelan County Superior Court Judge John Bridges, June 6, 2005, published: June 8, 2005
  4. ^ Robinson, W.S. (1950). "Ecological Correlations and the Behavior of Individuals". American Sociological Review. 15: 351–357. doi:10.2307/2087176.