Universal Darwinism: Difference between revisions
m →History and Development: punctuation and spaces; unlinked years |
|||
Line 33: | Line 33: | ||
| quote = [...] evolutionary theorizing about cultural, social, and economic phenomena has a long tradition, going back well before Darwin. |
| quote = [...] evolutionary theorizing about cultural, social, and economic phenomena has a long tradition, going back well before Darwin. |
||
}} |
}} |
||
</ref> but was still lacking the concept of natural selection. Darwin himself, together with subsequent 19th century thinkers such as [[Herbert Spencer]], [[Thorstein Veblen]], [[James Mark Baldwin]] and [[William James]], was quick to apply the idea of selection to other domains, such as language, psychology, society, and culture.<ref>Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914</ref> However, this evolutionary tradition was largely banned from the social sciences in the beginning of the 20th century, in part because of the bad reputation of [[Social Darwinism]], |
</ref> but was still lacking the concept of natural selection. Darwin himself, together with subsequent 19th century thinkers such as [[Herbert Spencer]], [[Thorstein Veblen]], [[James Mark Baldwin]] and [[William James]], was quick to apply the idea of selection to other domains, such as language, psychology, society, and culture.<ref>Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914</ref> However, this evolutionary tradition was largely banned from the social sciences in the beginning of the 20th century, in part because of the bad reputation of [[Social Darwinism]], an attempt to use Darwinism to justify social inequality. |
||
Starting in the 1950s, [[Donald T. Campbell]] was one of the first and most influential authors to revive the tradition, and to formulate a generalized Darwinian [[algorithm]] directly applicable to phenomena outside of biology.<ref>Gontier, N. (2006). Evolutionary Epistemology. Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.</ref> In this, he was inspired by [[William Ross Ashby]]'s view of [[self-organization]] and intelligence as fundamental processes of selection.<ref>Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380–400.</ref> His aim was to explain the development of [[science]] and other forms of [[knowledge]] by focusing on the variation and selection of ideas and theories, thus laying the basis for the domain of [[evolutionary epistemology]]. In the 1990s, Campbell's formulation of the mechanism of "blind-variation-and-selective-retention" (BVSR) was further developed and extended to other domains under the labels of "universal selection theory"<ref>Campbell, D. T. (1990). Epistemological roles for selection theory. Evolution, cognition, and realism: Studies in evolutionary epistemology, 1–19.</ref> or "universal selectionism"<ref>Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914.</ref> by his disciples Gary Cziko,<ref>Gary Cziko (1995) [http://faculty.ed.uiuc.edu/g-cziko/wm/ Without Miracles: Universal Selection Theory and the Second Darwinian Revolution] (MIT Press)</ref><ref>Stoelhorst, J. W. (n.d.). Universal Darwinism from the bottom up: An evolutionary view of socio-economic behavior and organization. Wolfram Elsner and Hardy Hanappi, Advances in Evolutionary Institutional Economics: Evolutionary Modules, Non-Knowledge, and Strategy. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishers.</ref> Mark Bickhard,<ref>Bickhard, M. H., & Campbell, D. T. (2003). [http://www.lehigh.edu/~mhb0/TheoryKnowledgeReadings6Nov10/Varvarsel.fos.11Aug03.pdf Variations in variation and selection: The ubiquity of the variation-and-selective-retention ratchet in emergent organizational complexity] Foundations of Science, 8(3), 215–282.</ref> and [[Francis Heylighen]].<ref>Heylighen, F. (1992). [http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.32.7220&rep=rep1&type=pdf Principles of Systems and Cybernetics: an evolutionary perspective]. Cybernetics and Systems' 92, 3-10.</ref><ref>Heylighen F. (1999): [http://pcp.vub.ac.be/Papers/ComplexityGrowth.pdf "The Growth of Structural and Functional Complexity during Evolution"], in: F. Heylighen, J. Bollen & A. Riegler (eds.) The Evolution of Complexity (Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht), p. 17-44.</ref> |
Starting in the 1950s, [[Donald T. Campbell]] was one of the first and most influential authors to revive the tradition, and to formulate a generalized Darwinian [[algorithm]] directly applicable to phenomena outside of biology.<ref>Gontier, N. (2006). Evolutionary Epistemology. Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.</ref> In this, he was inspired by [[William Ross Ashby]]'s view of [[self-organization]] and intelligence as fundamental processes of selection.<ref>Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380–400.</ref> His aim was to explain the development of [[science]] and other forms of [[knowledge]] by focusing on the variation and selection of ideas and theories, thus laying the basis for the domain of [[evolutionary epistemology]]. In the 1990s, Campbell's formulation of the mechanism of "blind-variation-and-selective-retention" (BVSR) was further developed and extended to other domains under the labels of "universal selection theory"<ref>Campbell, D. T. (1990). Epistemological roles for selection theory. Evolution, cognition, and realism: Studies in evolutionary epistemology, 1–19.</ref> or "universal selectionism"<ref>Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914.</ref> by his disciples Gary Cziko,<ref>Gary Cziko (1995) [http://faculty.ed.uiuc.edu/g-cziko/wm/ Without Miracles: Universal Selection Theory and the Second Darwinian Revolution] (MIT Press)</ref><ref>Stoelhorst, J. W. (n.d.). Universal Darwinism from the bottom up: An evolutionary view of socio-economic behavior and organization. Wolfram Elsner and Hardy Hanappi, Advances in Evolutionary Institutional Economics: Evolutionary Modules, Non-Knowledge, and Strategy. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishers.</ref> Mark Bickhard,<ref>Bickhard, M. H., & Campbell, D. T. (2003). [http://www.lehigh.edu/~mhb0/TheoryKnowledgeReadings6Nov10/Varvarsel.fos.11Aug03.pdf Variations in variation and selection: The ubiquity of the variation-and-selective-retention ratchet in emergent organizational complexity] Foundations of Science, 8(3), 215–282.</ref> and [[Francis Heylighen]].<ref>Heylighen, F. (1992). [http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.32.7220&rep=rep1&type=pdf Principles of Systems and Cybernetics: an evolutionary perspective]. Cybernetics and Systems' 92, 3-10.</ref><ref>Heylighen F. (1999): [http://pcp.vub.ac.be/Papers/ComplexityGrowth.pdf "The Growth of Structural and Functional Complexity during Evolution"], in: F. Heylighen, J. Bollen & A. Riegler (eds.) The Evolution of Complexity (Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht), p. 17-44.</ref> |
Revision as of 02:47, 2 September 2011
Universal Darwinism (also known as generalized Darwinism or universal selection theory [1]) refers to a variety of approaches that extend the theory of Darwinism beyond its original domain of biological evolution on Earth. The idea is to formulate a generalized version of the mechanisms of variation, selection and heredity proposed by Charles Darwin, so that they can be applied to explain evolution in a wide variety of other domains, including psychology, economics, culture, medicine, computer science and physics.
Basic Mechanisms
At the most fundamental level, Charles Darwin's theory of evolution states that organisms evolve and adapt to their environment by an iterative process. This process can be conceived as an evolutionary algorithm that searches the space of possible forms (the fitness landscape) for the one that are best adapted. The process has three components:
- variation of a given form or template. This is usually (but not necessarily) considered to be blind or random, and happens typically by mutation or recombination.
- selection of the fittest variants, i.e. those that are best suited to survive and reproduce in their given environment. The unfit variants are eliminated.
- heredity or retention, meaning that the features of the fit variants are retained and passed on, e.g. in offspring.
After those fit variants are retained, they can again undergo variation, either directly or in their offspring, starting a new round of the iteration. The overall mechanism is similar to the problem-solving procedures of trial-and-error or generate-and-test: evolution can be seen as searching for the best solution for the problem of how to survive and reproduce.
The generalization made in "universal" Darwinism is to replace "organism" by any recognizable pattern, phenomenon, or system. The first requirement is that the pattern can "survive" (maintain, be retained) long enough or "reproduce" (replicate, be copied) sufficiently frequently so as not to disappear immediately. This is the inheritance component: the information in the pattern must be retained or passed on. The second requirement is that during survival and reproduction variation (small changes in the pattern) can occur. The final requirement is that there is a selective "preference" so that certain variants tend to survive or reproduce "better" than others. If these conditions are met, then, by the logic of natural selection, the pattern will evolve towards more adapted forms.
Examples of patterns that have been postulated to undergo variation and selection, and thus adaptation, are genes, ideas (memes), neurons and their connections, words, computer programs, firms, antibodies, institutions, quantum states and even whole universes.
History and Development
Conceptually, "evolutionary theorizing about cultural, social, and economic phenomena" preceded Darwin,[2] but was still lacking the concept of natural selection. Darwin himself, together with subsequent 19th century thinkers such as Herbert Spencer, Thorstein Veblen, James Mark Baldwin and William James, was quick to apply the idea of selection to other domains, such as language, psychology, society, and culture.[3] However, this evolutionary tradition was largely banned from the social sciences in the beginning of the 20th century, in part because of the bad reputation of Social Darwinism, an attempt to use Darwinism to justify social inequality.
Starting in the 1950s, Donald T. Campbell was one of the first and most influential authors to revive the tradition, and to formulate a generalized Darwinian algorithm directly applicable to phenomena outside of biology.[4] In this, he was inspired by William Ross Ashby's view of self-organization and intelligence as fundamental processes of selection.[5] His aim was to explain the development of science and other forms of knowledge by focusing on the variation and selection of ideas and theories, thus laying the basis for the domain of evolutionary epistemology. In the 1990s, Campbell's formulation of the mechanism of "blind-variation-and-selective-retention" (BVSR) was further developed and extended to other domains under the labels of "universal selection theory"[6] or "universal selectionism"[7] by his disciples Gary Cziko,[8][9] Mark Bickhard,[10] and Francis Heylighen.[11][12]
Richard Dawkins may have first coined the term "universal Darwinism" in 1983 to describe his conjecture that any possible life forms existing outside the solar system would evolve by natural selection just as they do on Earth.[13] Henry Plotkin in his 1997 book[14] on Darwin Machines makes the link between universal Darwinism and Campbell's evolutionary epistemology. Susan Blackmore, in her 1999 book The Meme Machine, devotes a chapter titled 'Universal Darwinism' to a discussion of the applicability of the Darwinian process to a wide range of scientific subject matters.
The philosopher of mind Daniel Dennett, in his 1995 book Darwin's Dangerous Idea, developed the idea of a Darwinian process, involving variation, selection and retention, as a generic algorithm that is substrate-neutral and could be applied to many fields of knowledge outside of biology. He described the idea of natural selection as a "universal acid" that cannot be contained in any vessel, as it seeps through the walls and spreads ever further, touching and transforming ever more domains. He notes in particular the field of memetics in the social sciences.[15][16]
In agreement with Dennett's prediction, over the past decades the Darwinian perspective has spread ever more widely, in particular across the social sciences as the foundation for numerous schools of study including memetics, evolutionary economics, evolutionary psychology, evolutionary anthropology, neural Darwinism, and evolutionary linguistics.[17] Researchers have postulated Darwinian processes as operating at the foundations of physics, cosmology and chemistry via the theories of Quantum Darwinism[18] and cosmological natural selection.[19][20] Similar mechanisms are extensively applied in computer science in the domains of genetic algorithms and evolutionary computation, which develop solutions to complex problems via a process of variation and selection.
Examples of Universal Darwinist theories
The following approaches can all be seen as exemplifying a generalization of Darwinian ideas outside of their original domain of biology. These "Darwinian extensions" can be grouped in two categories, depending on whether they discuss implications of biological (genetic) evolution in other disciplines (e.g. medicine or psychology), or discuss processes of variation and selection of entities other than genes (e.g. computer programs, firms or ideas). However, there is no strict separation possible, since most of these approaches (e.g. in sociology, psychology and linguistics) consider both genetic and non-genetic (e.g. cultural) aspects of evolution, as well as the interactions between them (see e.g. gene-culture coevolution).
Gene-based Darwinian extensions
- Evolutionary psychology assumes that our emotions, preferences and cognitive mechanisms are the product of natural selection
- Evolutionary educational psychology applies evolutionary psychology to education
- Evolutionary developmental psychology applies evolutionary psychology to cognitive development
- Darwinian Happiness applies evolutionary psychology to understand the optimal conditions for human well-being
- Darwinian literary studies tries to understand the characters and plots of narrative on the basis of evolutionary psychology
- Evolutionary anthropology studies the evolution of human beings
- Sociobiology proposes that social systems in animals and humans are the product of Darwinian evolution
- Human behavioral ecology investigates how human behavior has become adapted to its environment via variation and selection
- Evolutionary epistemology of mechanisms studies how our abilities to gather knowledge (perception, cognition) have evolved
- Darwinian medicine investigates the origin of diseases by looking at the evolution both of the human body and of its parasites
- Paleolithic diet proposes that the most healthy nutrition is the one to which our hunter-gatherer ancestors have adapted over millions of years
- Paleolithic lifestyle generalizes the paleolithic diet to include exercise, behavior and exposure to the environment
- Molecular evolution studies evolution at the level of DNA, RNA and proteins
Other Darwinian extensions
- Quantum darwinism sees the emergence of classical states in physics as a natural selection of the most stable quantum properties
- Cosmological natural selection hypothesizes that universes reproduce and are selected for having fundamental constants that maximize "fitness"
- Complex adaptive systems models the dynamics of complex systems in part on the basis of the variation and selection of its components
- Evolutionary computation is a Darwinian approach to the generation of adapted computer programs
- Genetic algorithms, a subset of evolutionary computation, models variation by "genetic" operators (mutation and recombination)
- Evolutionary robotics applies Darwinian algorithms to the design of autonomous robots
- Artificial Life uses Darwinian algorithms to let organism-like computer agents evolve in a software simulation
- Evolutionary art uses variation and selection to produce works of art
- Clonal selection theory sees the creation of adapted antibodies in the immune system as a process of variation and selection
- Neural darwinism proposes that neurons and their synapses are selectively pruned during brain development
- Evolutionary epistemology of theories assumes that scientific theories develop through variation and selection
- Memetics is a theory of the variation, transmission and selection of cultural items, such as ideas, fashions, and traditions
- Cultural selection theory is a theory of cultural evolution related to memetics
- Evolutionary linguistics studies the evolution of language, biologically as well as culturally
- Evolutionary economics views competition between firms and institutions as a process of "survival of the fittest"
References
- ^ Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914.
- ^
Nelson, Richard R. (2007). "Universal Darwinism and evolutionary social science". Biology and Philosophy. 22 (1). Springer Netherlands: 73–94. ISSN 1572-8404. Retrieved 2010-01-02.
[...] evolutionary theorizing about cultural, social, and economic phenomena has a long tradition, going back well before Darwin.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914
- ^ Gontier, N. (2006). Evolutionary Epistemology. Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
- ^ Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380–400.
- ^ Campbell, D. T. (1990). Epistemological roles for selection theory. Evolution, cognition, and realism: Studies in evolutionary epistemology, 1–19.
- ^ Hodgson, G. M. (2005). Generalizing Darwinism to social evolution: Some early attempts. Journal of economic issues, 899–914.
- ^ Gary Cziko (1995) Without Miracles: Universal Selection Theory and the Second Darwinian Revolution (MIT Press)
- ^ Stoelhorst, J. W. (n.d.). Universal Darwinism from the bottom up: An evolutionary view of socio-economic behavior and organization. Wolfram Elsner and Hardy Hanappi, Advances in Evolutionary Institutional Economics: Evolutionary Modules, Non-Knowledge, and Strategy. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishers.
- ^ Bickhard, M. H., & Campbell, D. T. (2003). Variations in variation and selection: The ubiquity of the variation-and-selective-retention ratchet in emergent organizational complexity Foundations of Science, 8(3), 215–282.
- ^ Heylighen, F. (1992). Principles of Systems and Cybernetics: an evolutionary perspective. Cybernetics and Systems' 92, 3-10.
- ^ Heylighen F. (1999): "The Growth of Structural and Functional Complexity during Evolution", in: F. Heylighen, J. Bollen & A. Riegler (eds.) The Evolution of Complexity (Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht), p. 17-44.
- ^ Dawkins, R. (1983) Universal Darwinism. In: Evolution from molecules to man, ed. D. S. Bendall. Cambridge University Press.
- ^ Plotkin, H. C. (1997). Darwin Machines and the Nature of Knowledge. Harvard University Press.
- ^ Dennett, Daniel C. (2005), Darwin's Dangerous Idea, Touchstone Press, New York. pp. 352–360.
- ^ Stoelhorst, J. W. (n.d.). Universal Darwinism from the bottom up: An evolutionary view of socio-economic behavior and organization. Wolfram Elsner and Hardy Hanappi, Advances in Evolutionary Institutional Economics: Evolutionary Modules, Non-Knowledge, and Strategy. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishers.
- ^ Oudeyer, P.-Y. and Kaplan, F. (2007) Language Evolution as a Darwinian Process: Computational Studies. Cognitive Processing, 8(1):21–35
- ^ Blume-Kohou, Robin and Zurek, Wojciech. (2006). Quantum Darwinism: Entanglement, branches, and the emergent classicality of redundantly stored quantum information, Journal-ref: Phys. Rev. A 73, 062310 (2006)
- ^ Smolin, Lee (1997). The Life of the Cosmos. Oxford University Press
- ^ Smolin, Lee (2008), Scientific Alternatives to the Anthropic Principle, Contribution to "Universe or Multiverse", ed. by Bernard Carr et al., to be published by Cambridge University Press.