Jump to content

American Sign Language: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
fuller definition
Sethg (talk | contribs)
corrected some details, added references
Line 1: Line 1:
'''American Sign Language''' is the dominant [[sign language]] in the [[United States]], [[Canada]] and parts of [[Mexico]]. American Sign Language is usually abbreviated '''ASL''' though it has also been known as '''Ameslan'''. As with other sign languages, or more properly, gestural-spatial languages, its [[grammar]] and [[syntax]] are separate and distinct from the spoken language(s) spoken in its area of influence. It originated around 1900 as the sign languages of the American Indians, [[French Sign Language]], and the sign language of the residents of [[Marthas Vineyard|Martha's Vineyard]] merged with one another and probably other linguistic influences at the first school for the deaf in America, established by [[Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet]].
'''American Sign Language''' is the dominant [[sign language]] in the [[United States]], [[Canada]] and parts of [[Mexico]]. American Sign Language is usually abbreviated '''ASL''' though it has also been known as '''Ameslan'''. As with other sign languages, or more properly, gestural-spatial languages, its [[grammar]] and [[syntax]] are separate and distinct from the spoken language(s) spoken in its area of influence. It originated in the early 19th century at the first school for the deaf in America, the American Asylum for the Deaf and Dumb (now [http://www.asd-1817.org/ the American School for the Deaf]), established by [[Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet]]. The first deaf teacher there came from France, and many of its first students were from [[Marthas Vineyard|Martha's Vineyard]], where hereditary deafness was common; the influence of [[French Sign Language]] on ASL is clear, and linguists believe that the Vineyard deaf had their own sign language that merged with FSL to create ASL.


ASL is a [[natural language]] as proved to the satisfaction of the linguistic community by [[William Stokoe]]. It is a manual language meaning that the information is expressed not with combinations of sounds but with combinations of handshapes, movements of the hands, arms and body, and facial expressions. It is used natively and predominantly by the [[deaf]] and [[hard-of-hearing]] of the United States and Canada.
ASL is a [[natural language]] as proved to the satisfaction of the linguistic community by [[William Stokoe]]. It is a manual language meaning that the information is expressed not with combinations of sounds but with combinations of handshapes, movements of the hands, arms and body, and facial expressions. It is used natively and predominantly by the [[deaf]] and [[hard-of-hearing]] of the United States and Canada.
Line 13: Line 13:
Klima and Bellugi used American Sign Language in formulating that classification. The theory that signs are self-explanatory can be conclusively disproved by the fact that non-signers cannot understand fluent, continuous sign language. The majority of signs are opaque.
Klima and Bellugi used American Sign Language in formulating that classification. The theory that signs are self-explanatory can be conclusively disproved by the fact that non-signers cannot understand fluent, continuous sign language. The majority of signs are opaque.


The grammar of ASL uses spatial locations, motion, and context to indicate [[syntax]]. For example:
The grammar of ASL uses spatial locations, motion, and context to indicate [[semantics]]. For example, pronouns involve signing a noun, pointing at a spot near the signer, and then "signing to" that spot as if it were the noun. Differences in the degree of a verb, adjective, or adverb may be indicated by exaggerating movements in its sign. Verb tenses can be expressed by adding a clause to indicate the time frame. To indicate emotional inflection, signers use facial expressions that are exaggerated over what a hearing person would use.

* If a signer signs a noun and then points to a certain spot, he or she can refer back to that noun by pointing again to the same spot.

* To intensify the meaning a verb or adjective (e.g., to say "very calm" instead of "calm"), the signer modulates the way it is expressed, first holding his or her hands rigid and then making the rest of the sign more quickly than usual.

* Raised eyebrows can indicate a yes-or-no question, while lowered eyebrows indicate a wh-question.


In recent years, it has been shown that ASL has had a positive impact on the intellect of hearing children who are exposed to it. When infants are taught the language early, parents are able to respond accordingly to the infant at a [[Child development|developmental stage]] when verbal speech, which requires extremely fine control of many, interacting parts, is not yet able to be formed. The ability of the child to actively communicate and interact earlier than would otherwise be possible accelerates the cognitive development of the child.
In recent years, it has been shown that ASL has had a positive impact on the intellect of hearing children who are exposed to it. When infants are taught the language early, parents are able to respond accordingly to the infant at a [[Child development|developmental stage]] when verbal speech, which requires extremely fine control of many, interacting parts, is not yet able to be formed. The ability of the child to actively communicate and interact earlier than would otherwise be possible accelerates the cognitive development of the child.
Line 25: Line 31:
==External link==
==External link==
*[http://www.deaflibrary.org/asl.html About ASL] - article at [http://www.deaflibrary.org deaflibrary.org]
*[http://www.deaflibrary.org/asl.html About ASL] - article at [http://www.deaflibrary.org deaflibrary.org]

==References==

* Groce, Nora Ellen (1988). ''Everybody Here Spoke Sign Language: Hereditary Deafness on Marthas Vineyard.'' Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 067427041X.

* Klima, Edward, and Bellugi, Ursula (1979). ''The Signs of Language.'' Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0674807952.

* Stokoe, William C. (1976). ''Dictionary of American Sign Language on Linguistic Principles.'' Linstok Press. ISBN 0932130011.


[[Category:Sign language]]
[[Category:Sign language]]

Revision as of 21:55, 17 November 2004

American Sign Language is the dominant sign language in the United States, Canada and parts of Mexico. American Sign Language is usually abbreviated ASL though it has also been known as Ameslan. As with other sign languages, or more properly, gestural-spatial languages, its grammar and syntax are separate and distinct from the spoken language(s) spoken in its area of influence. It originated in the early 19th century at the first school for the deaf in America, the American Asylum for the Deaf and Dumb (now the American School for the Deaf), established by Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet. The first deaf teacher there came from France, and many of its first students were from Martha's Vineyard, where hereditary deafness was common; the influence of French Sign Language on ASL is clear, and linguists believe that the Vineyard deaf had their own sign language that merged with FSL to create ASL.

ASL is a natural language as proved to the satisfaction of the linguistic community by William Stokoe. It is a manual language meaning that the information is expressed not with combinations of sounds but with combinations of handshapes, movements of the hands, arms and body, and facial expressions. It is used natively and predominantly by the deaf and hard-of-hearing of the United States and Canada.

Although it often seems as though the signs are meaningful of themselves, in fact they can be as arbitrary as words in spoken language. For example, hearing children often make the mistake of using "you" to refer to themselves, since others refer to them as "you." Children who acquire the sign YOU (pointing at one's interlocutor) make similar mistakes – they will point at others to mean themselves, indicating that even something as seemingly explicit as pointing is an arbitrary sign in ASL, like words in a spoken language.

However, Edward Klima and Ursula Bellugi have modified the common theory that signs can be self-explanatory by grouping signs into three categories:

  • Transparent: Non-signers can usually correctly guess the meaning
  • Translucent: Meaning makes sense to non-signers once it is explained
  • Opaque: Meaning cannot be guessed by non-signers

Klima and Bellugi used American Sign Language in formulating that classification. The theory that signs are self-explanatory can be conclusively disproved by the fact that non-signers cannot understand fluent, continuous sign language. The majority of signs are opaque.

The grammar of ASL uses spatial locations, motion, and context to indicate syntax. For example:

  • If a signer signs a noun and then points to a certain spot, he or she can refer back to that noun by pointing again to the same spot.
  • To intensify the meaning a verb or adjective (e.g., to say "very calm" instead of "calm"), the signer modulates the way it is expressed, first holding his or her hands rigid and then making the rest of the sign more quickly than usual.
  • Raised eyebrows can indicate a yes-or-no question, while lowered eyebrows indicate a wh-question.

In recent years, it has been shown that ASL has had a positive impact on the intellect of hearing children who are exposed to it. When infants are taught the language early, parents are able to respond accordingly to the infant at a developmental stage when verbal speech, which requires extremely fine control of many, interacting parts, is not yet able to be formed. The ability of the child to actively communicate and interact earlier than would otherwise be possible accelerates the cognitive development of the child.

ASL has allegedly been taught to chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas. Several of the animals have been said to have mastered more than one hundred signs, though not all agree with the ability of the primates to sign. For example, when the Washoe research team asked the handlers of the chimp to write signs down whenever they witnessed them being produced by Washoe, the hearing people on the team turned in long lists of signs while the only deaf native speaker of ASL on the team turned in blank lists. She explained that what she saw were not signs at all, but simply gestures. Further fomenting the controversy, the researchers in the studies of Koko and Washoe refused to share their raw data with the scientific community. The theory that non-human primates have learned ASL, or that they are even capable of learning ASL or any other natural language, is not currently accepted by linguists. However, research on the ability of some primates to learn symbol systems continues and has shown interesting results.

See also

References

  • Groce, Nora Ellen (1988). Everybody Here Spoke Sign Language: Hereditary Deafness on Marthas Vineyard. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 067427041X.
  • Klima, Edward, and Bellugi, Ursula (1979). The Signs of Language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0674807952.
  • Stokoe, William C. (1976). Dictionary of American Sign Language on Linguistic Principles. Linstok Press. ISBN 0932130011.