Jump to content

On Killing

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is the current revision of this page, as edited by Οἶδα (talk | contribs) at 03:47, 19 April 2024 (added Category:Books about post-traumatic stress disorder using HotCat). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this version.

(diff) ← Previous revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society
AuthorDave Grossman
PublisherBack Bay Books
Publication date
1996
Pages400
ISBN0-316-33000-0
OCLC36544198
Followed byOn Combat: The Psychology and Physiology of Deadly Conflict in War and in Peace 

On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society is a book by Dave Grossman exploring the psychology of the act of killing and the military law enforcement establishments attempt to understand and deal with the consequences of killing. The book is based on S.L.A. Marshall's theory that the majority of soldiers in war do not ever fire their weapons due to an innate resistance to killing.[1]

Overview

[edit]

The book is based on S.L.A. Marshall's studies from World War II, which proposed that, contrary to popular perception,[1] the majority of soldiers in war do not ever fire their weapons because of innate resistance to killing. Based on Marshall's studies, the military instituted training measures to break down this resistance, raising soldiers' firing rates to over 90 percent during the Vietnam War.[2]

Grossman points out that there are great psychological costs that weigh heavily on the combat soldier or police officer who kill if they are not mentally prepared for what may happen; if their actions (killing) are not supported by their commanders and/or peers; and if they are unable to justify their actions (or if no one else justifies the actions for them).

The second edition of the book, along with an audio version, was released in 2009.

The soldier's choice

[edit]

Grossman claims in his book On Killing that soldiers are faced with four options once they have entered into combat.[3]

  1. Fight: As the name implies, this is the standard that defines the soldier's role as actively trying to defeat the enemy by use of their training.
  2. Flight: This option involves the combatant fleeing the engagement.
  3. Posture: This action involves the soldier falsely showing active participation in combat. In actuality they are not being effective in deterring the enemy from success. This is a major point of concern for commanders as it is difficult to tell the difference between a soldier posturing or fighting.
  4. Submit: Submission to the enemy during an engagement is a direct act of surrender. In the animal kingdom, this is used by lesser combatants to avoid being injured upon ascertaining the futility of the battle.

The problem of non- or miss-firing soldiers

[edit]

S.L.A. Marshall did a study on the firing rates of soldiers in World War II. He found that the ratio of rounds fired vs. hits was low; he also noted that the majority of soldiers were not aiming to hit their targets. This is attributable to the inherent humanity inside the soldiers who grew up in a peaceful, equitable society.[4] This was a problem for the US military and its allies during World War II. New training was developed and hit rates improved. The changes were small, but effective. First, instead of shooting at bull's-eye type targets, the United States Army switched to silhouette targets that mimic an average human. Training also switched from 300 yard slow fire testing to rapid fire testing with different time and distance intervals from 20 to 300 yards. With these two changes, hitting targets became a reaction that was almost automatic.

Some authors have discredited S.L.A. Marshall's book, stating that the book may be more of an idea of what was occurring and not a scientific study of what was happening. Other historians and journalists have outright accused Marshall of fabricating his study.[5]

Another important factor that increased fire and hit rates is the development of camaraderie in training. Soldiers are taught that their actions do not only help or harm themselves, but the whole unit.[6][original research?] This recurring theme in recollections collected from war veterans is the idea that they were not fighting for themselves at the time but more concerned for the people to their left and right. This ideology is ancient, recorded for example by Sun Tzu in his book The Art of War: "If those who are sent to draw water begin by drinking themselves, the army is suffering from thirst."[7]

Increase in PTSD since World War II

[edit]

Some research indicates that the increased incidence of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in the military is related to the increase in firing rates.[3] This brings up the classic debate of correlation vs. causation. Many believe that other factors have been influencing PTSD such as decompression time after conflicts, and the lack of moral sovereignty in engagements.[3]

World War II and Vietnam

[edit]

The Vietnam War, when compared to World War II, had markedly more anti-war demonstrations which showed the public's unwillingness to involve itself in Vietnam. Along with demonstrations at home, people who were sent to fight thought there was no reason for the war and so did not feel a moral obligation to fight. In contrast, many U.S. soldiers in World War II felt they were stopping an evil empire from conquering the globe. This helped the World War II troops' mettle to be steadfast.[original research?]

Another problem with PTSD rates after World War II is the fact that there has been far less decompression time for the soldiers.[3] During World War II the main way back home was aboard ships, a trip that could take weeks. This time was spent with others who had had similar experiences and could understand the problems faced by others.[3] During Vietnam soldiers were sent via draft to one year rotations by plane. Draftees arrived to their unit usually by themselves and were often shunned. This shunning was due to the senior members being afraid to befriend someone with a much higher chance of being killed than experienced combatants. Once the draftees’ time in country was over they were sent back home by themselves. Travel may have been with other veterans but from a mix of units without enough familiarity to share hardships they had seen.

Finally one of the worst displays of environmental stressors was once back home they were demonized by the public and dismissed as human beings. This was in stark contrast with the treatment World War II veterans received when they came home from the European Theatre or the Pacific Theatre. After World War II, parades were thrown, many people thanked the soldiers, and V for Victory was made to quickly show military members support. That symbol was changed into the Peace sign and used to show disapproval of the war in Vietnam just a few years later. These factors among many others caused the Vietnam War to have the highest post-war depression, suicide, and PTSD rates. To this day many are only now getting the counselling needed to overcome mental problems brought from their service in Vietnam.[3]

Modern engagements

[edit]

In recent engagements such as the Persian Gulf War through the Iraq War and War in Afghanistan there is still a problem with a lack of decompression time. The training has improved so soldiers train and deploy with the people they will be fighting with.[8] Many times, when they reach home, they are given time off. If one is in a reserve unit, they most likely go back to work and only see their fellow soldiers once a month. This lack of time to debrief and decompress can cause a feeling of isolation that only worsens the effects of PTSD. Grossman states in his book that everyone who experiences combat comes back with PTSD, the only question is to what extent their mind and psyche are damaged and how they cope with it.

Claims

[edit]

Grossman's theory, based on the World War II research of S.L.A. Marshall, is that most of the population deeply resists killing another human. Some veterans and historians have cast doubt on Marshall's research methodology.[1] Professor Roger J. Spiller (deputy director of the Combat Studies Institute, US Army Command and General Staff College) argues in his 1988 article, "S.L.A. Marshall and the Ratio of Fire" (RUSI Journal, Winter 1988, pages 63–71), that Marshall had not actually conducted the research upon which he based his ratio-of-fire theory. "The 'systematic collection of data' appears to have been an invention."[9] This revelation has called into question the authenticity of some of Marshall's other books and has lent academic weight to doubts about his integrity that had been raised in military circles even decades earlier.[10]

As a result of Marshall's work, modern military training was modified to attempt to override this instinct, by:

  • using man-shaped targets instead of bullseye targets in marksmanship practice
  • practicing and drilling how soldiers would actually fight
  • dispersing responsibility for the killing throughout the group
  • displacing responsibility for the killing onto an authority figure, i.e., the commanding officer and the military hierarchy (see the Milgram experiment)

By the time of the United States involvement in the Vietnam War, says Grossman, 90% of U.S. soldiers would fire their weapons at other people.

He also says the act of killing is psychologically traumatic for the killer, even more so than constant danger or witnessing the death of others.

Grossman further argues that violence in television, movies and video games contributes to real-life violence by a similar process of training and desensitization.

In On Combat (Grossman's sequel to On Killing, based on ten years of additional research and interviews), he addresses the psychology and physiology of human aggression.

Reception

[edit]

Robert Engen, in a paper for the Canadian Military Journal critiquing On Killing, both praised and criticized Grossman's works, saying: "On Killing and On Combat form an excellent starting point, there are too many problems with their interpretation for them to be considered the final word on the subject."[1] Grossman's response to Engen, printed in the same journal, addresses the criticisms by showing that S.L.A. Marshall's findings, even after having doubt cast on their methodology, have borne out in further scientific studies and real world experience and, furthermore, have been the cornerstone of military and police training for over a half century.[11]

On Killing is on the United States Marine Corps' recommended reading list.[12]

Legacy

[edit]

The series 3 Black Mirror episode, "Men Against Fire" (2016), was partly inspired by Men Against Fire: The Problem of Battle Command and On Killing, and explores the same themes.[13]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c d Engen, Robert. "Killing for Their Country: A New Look At 'Killology'". Canadian Military Journal. 9 (2). Archived from the original on 2011-07-21. As a military historian, I am instinctively skeptical of any work or theory that claims to overturn all existing scholarship – indeed, overturn an entire academic discipline – in one fell swoop...[however] Lieutenant Colonel Grossman's appeals to biology and psychology are flawed, and the bulwark of his historical evidence – S.L.A. Marshall's assertion that soldiers do not fire their weapons – can be verifiably disproven.
  2. ^ Steuter, Erin; Wills, Deborah (15 July 2009). At War with Metaphor: Media, Propaganda, and Racism in the War on Terror. Lexington Books. p. 57. ISBN 978-0-7391-3031-5.
  3. ^ a b c d e f Grossman, Dave (1996). On killing : the psychological cost of learning to kill in war and society. Boston: Little, Brown. p. 367. ISBN 978-0-316-33000-8. OCLC 32312539.
  4. ^ Marshall, S. L. A. (Samuel Lyman Atwood) (2000). Men against fire: the problem of battle command. Norman, Okla.: University of Oklahoma Press. ISBN 978-0-8061-3280-8.
  5. ^ Hunter, Evan (December 12, 2007). "Fire Away". Newsweek.
  6. ^ Kyle, Chris (2012). American Sniper: The Autobiography of the Most Lethal Sniper in U.S. Military History. New York: W. Morrow. ISBN 9780062082374.
  7. ^ Sun Tzu, 6th century BC; Minford, John (2006). The Art of War. New York: Penguin Books. ISBN 978-0-14-303752-1.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Wright, Evan (2005). Generation kill: Devil Dogs, Iceman, Captain America, and the new face of American war. New York: Berkeley Caliber. ISBN 9780425200407.
  9. ^ Spiller, Roger J. (Winter 1988). "S.L.A. Marshall and the Ratio of Fire". RUSI Journal. pp. 63–71..
    (Extracts are available on-line in an article criticizing Marshall Archived 2005-12-10 at the Wayback Machine)
  10. ^ Hunter, Evan (December 12, 2007). "Fire Away". Newsweek.
  11. ^ Dave Grossman. "S.L.A. MARSHALL REVISITED...?". Canadian Military Journal. Archived from the original on November 5, 2013.
  12. ^ Freeman, Sharon Morgillo; Moore, Bret A; Freeman, Arthur; Edd Arthur Freeman, Edd, Abpp (3 June 2009). Living and Surviving in Harm's Way: A Psychological Treatment Handbook for Pre- and Post-Deployment of Military Personnel. Taylor & Francis. p. 21. ISBN 978-1-135-85934-3.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  13. ^ "Black Mirror postmortem: Showrunner talks season 3 twists". Entertainment Weekly. 21 October 2016. Retrieved 24 October 2016.