Jump to content

Baby sign language

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Enauspeaker (talk | contribs) at 10:06, 10 November 2010 (article needs MAJOR improvements). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Specialized sign language is sometimes used to communicate with infants and toddlers. While infants and toddlers have a desire to communicate their needs and wishes, they lack the ability to do so clearly because the production of speech lags behind cognitive ability in the first months and years of life. Proponents of baby sign language say that this gap between desire to communicate and ability often leads to frustration and tantrums.[1][2] However, since hand–eye coordination develops sooner than acquisition of verbal skills, infants can learn simple signs for common words such as "eat", "sleep", "more", "hug", "play", "cookie", "teddy bear", before they are able to produce understandable speech.[3]

Research

In an article in the British Psychological Society's "The Psychologist" [4] Gwyneth Doherty-Sneddon has considered in detail the theoretical bases behind the growth of this phenomenon and some of the claims made by its supporters [5]

As Doherty-Sneddon points out so-called "baby signing" is not entirely new. Variants have been used by speech and language therapists for decades with children who have speech and/or cognitive impairments (e.g. Clibbens et al., 2002 [6]). It is widely recognised that communication is at the heart of child development, be it cognitive, social, emotional or behavioural (e.g. Vygotsky, 1978 [7]).

Clinicians and researchers have highlighted the association between communicative difficulties and behavioural problems. For example, Paul and Kellog (1997) [8] found children who were late talkers at age two years were more shy, aloof and less outgoing at age six. Similar poor social-emotional adjustment was found in late talking toddlers, along with higher reported parent-child dysfunction by mothers, in a study by Irwin et al. (2002) [9].

While baby signing promoters claim various benefits verified in experimental research, there is in fact a dearth of actual research. The American team led by Acredolo and Goodwyn has been responsible for driving research into the effects of baby signing on child development. They claim babies readily acquire symbolic gestures when exposed to enhanced gesture training. They also propose (Acredolo et al., 1999 [10], Goodwyn et al., 2000 [11]) those taught to sign reap rewards such as:

  • larger expressive and receptive spoken language vocabularies;
  • more advanced mental development;
  • a reduction in problematic behaviours like tantrums resulting from frustration; and
  • improved parent–child relationships.

The mechanisms underlying these benefits are proposed to include:

  • an increased number of episodes of joint visual attention during interactions between parents and toddlers, known to be associated with improved language skills;
  • empowering of the infant to focus the topic and context of conversation;
  • the discussion and clarification of concepts
  • added practice with the symbolic function.

Doherty-Sneddon claims a key issue is ensuring that sufficient and appropriately designed research is available to back the claims made in relation to baby signing. A literature review concluded although benefits were reported in 13 of 17 studies, various methodological weaknesses leave the evidence unconfirmed.[12] Certainly, research into the effects of baby signing needs better control groups, such as children who are involved in equally interesting and fun activities based around adult and child language interaction but not baby signing.

Volterra et al. (2006) [13] conclude enhanced gesture input for hearing children is a catalyst for gesture acquisition, and especially the use of representational form and hence symbolic communicative function. They add this enhancement is short-lived (to between 12 and 15 months of age). Doherty-Sneddon argues, however, this timescale represents only a general norm. The enhancement and advantage is far more extended in the many toddlers who are not speaking until well after their second birthdays.

Doherty-Sneddon concludes by arguing there are three different levels of support for the benefits of baby signing:

  • indicative, if not evidentially strong, evidence from baby signing research;
  • related evidence from deaf sign and hearing gesture/language research;
  • compelling anecdotal support from families who have embraced the approach.

Development

In the United States, teaching sign language to non-signing families to communicate with their hearing infants and toddlers was developed by Linda Acredolo, professor emerita of psychology at the University of California, Davis, and Susan Goodwyn, professor of psychology at California State University, Stanislaus.[citation needed][dubiousdiscuss] Their research began in 1982, and produced a 1985 article in Human Development titled “Symbolic gesturing in language development: A case study.”[14]

Joseph Garcia, an American Sign Language (ASL) interpreter and a leading proponent of use of ASL in communicating with infants and toddlers, began with his graduate thesis in 1986, an analysis of the role sign language could play in early childhood language acquisition. His research indicated babies who are exposed to signs regularly and consistently at six to seven months of age can begin using signs effectively by the eighth or ninth month.[15]

In 1998, a program was conducted at A. Sophie Rogers Infant-Toddler Laboratory School in Ohio State University by Kimberlee Whaley. Infants as young as 9 months old and their teachers began to learn to use some signs from the American Sign Language to communicate with each other. The program was not intended to teach American Sign Language, rather to use signs to communicate effectively. The program found that children would use the signs they learned in the classroom at home. Another finding indicated that girls use signs more than boys. This is something that Whaley wants to research further.[16]

Baby sign language was a plot element in the movie Meet the Fockers, where Jack (Robert De Niro's character) had taught his grandson "Little Jack" sign language. The twins that portrayed Little Jack (Bradley and Spencer Pickren), learned sign language from watching Signing Time! videos.[17]

See also

References

  1. ^ Garcia, Joseph. "Baby Sign Language Research." Sign2Me. Northlight Communications, Inc., 2010. Web. 23 Sept. 2010. <http://sign2me.com/index.php?option=com_content&task=view&id=17&Itemid=33>.
  2. ^ Summary of the Benefits of Signing. Signing Time! Two Little Hands Productions, 2006. Web. 23 Sept. 2010. <http://www.signingtime.net/pdf/st/STResearch_Summary.pdf>.
  3. ^ "Benefits for Babies Using Baby Sign Language". Babies-and-Sign-Language.com. Retrieved 2007-03-27.
  4. ^ "The great baby signing debate". The British Psychological Society. 3 April 2008. Retrieved 2010-07-19.
  5. ^ Doherty-Sneddon, G., "The great baby signing debate", The Psychologist, Vol. 21, Part 4, April 2008, pp300-303
  6. ^ Clibbens, J., Powell, G.G. & Atkinson, E. (2002). Strategies for achieving joint attention when signing to children with Down's syndrome. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37(3), 309–323
  7. ^ Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
  8. ^ Paul, R. & Kellog, L. (1997). Temperament in late talkers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 803–811
  9. ^ Irwin, J.R., Carter, A. & Briggs-Gowan, M.J. (2002). The social-emotional development of ‘late-talking’ toddlers. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1324–1332
  10. ^ Acredolo, L.P., Goodwyn, S.W., Horobin, K. & Emmons, Y. (1999). The signs and sounds of early language development. In L. Balter & C. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.) Child psychology (pp.116–139). New York: Psychology Press
  11. ^ Goodwyn, S., Acredolo, L. & Brown, C.A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 81–103
  12. ^ Johnston, J., Durieux-Smith, A. & Bloom, K. (2005). Teaching gestural signs to infants to advance child development. First Language, 25, 235–251
  13. ^ Volterra, V. Iverson, J.M. & Castrataro, M. (2006). The development of gesture in hearing and deaf children. In B. Schick et al. (Eds.) Sign language development. New York: Oxford University Press
  14. ^ "Supporting Research". Baby Signs. Retrieved 2007-03-27.
  15. ^ "Dr. Joseph Garcia". Stratton/Kehl Publications, Inc. Retrieved 2008-03-19.
  16. ^ "Teaching Infants to Use Sign Language". Ohio State University. Retrieved 2008-11-09. [dead link]
  17. ^ http://www.signingtime.com/forums/showpost.php?p=20499&postcount=117