User:Cmiddlebrooks/Negativity bias
This article is currently the subject of an educational assignment. |
Explanations
[edit]Drs. Paul Rozin and Edward Royzman proposed four elements of the negativity bias in order to explain its manifestation: negative potency, steeper negative gradients, negativity dominance, and negative differentiation.[1]
Negative potency refers to the notion that, while possibly of equal magnitude or emotionality, negative and positive items/events/etc. are not equally salient. Rozin and Royzman note that this characteristic of the negativity bias is only empirically demonstrable in situations with inherent measurability, such as comparing how positively or negatively a change in temperature is interpreted.
With respect to positive and negative gradients, it appears to be the case that negative events are thought to be perceived as increasingly more negative than positive events are increasingly positive the closer one gets (spatially or temporally) to the affective event itself. In other words, there is a steeper negative gradient than positive gradient. For example, the negative experience of an impending dental surgery is perceived as increasingly more negative the closer one gets to the date of surgery than the positive experience of an impending party is perceived as increasingly more positive the closer one gets to the date of celebration (assuming for the sake of this example that these events are equally positive and negative). Rozin and Royzman argue that this characteristic is distinct from that of negative potency because there appears to be evidence of steeper negative slopes relative to positive slopes even when potency itself is low.
Negativity dominance describes the tendency for the combination of positive and negative items/events/etc. to skew towards an overall more negative interpretation than would be suggested by the summation of the individual positive and negative components. Phrasing in more Gestalt-friendly terms, the whole is more negative than the sum of its parts.
Negative differentiation is consistent with evidence suggesting that the conceptualization of negativity is more elaborate and complex than that of positivity. For instance, research indicates that negative vocabulary is more richly descriptive of the affective experience than that of positive vocabulary[2]. Furthermore, there appear to be more terms employed to indicate negative emotions than positive emotions[3][4]. The notion of negative differentiation is consistent with the mobilization-minimization hypothesis[5], which posits that negative events, as a consequence of this complexity, require a greater mobilization of cognitive resources to deal with the affective experience and a greater effort to minimize the consequences.
References
[edit][6] [7] [8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] [18] [19] [20] [21] [22] [23] [24] [25] [26] [27] [28] [29] [30] [31] [32] [33] [34] [35] [36] [37] [38] [39] [40] [41] [42] [43] [44] [45] [46] [47] [48] [49] [1] [50] [51] [52] [53] [54] [55] [56] [57] [58] [59] [60]
Repeat citation for Baumeister et al... [9] Repeat citation for Boucher & Osgood... [11] Repeat citation for Fiske 1980... [61] Repeat citation for Hamilton & Zanna... [26] Repeat citation for Rozin & Royzman... [1] Repeat citation for Kanouse & Hanson... [35] Repeat citation for Pratto & John... [48] Repeat citation for Ohira et al... [45] Repeat citation for Costantini & Hoving [15] Repeat citation for Vaish et al. [56] Repeat citation for Lewicka et al., 1992... [38]
- ^ a b c Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296-320.
- ^ Peeters, G. (1971). The positive-negative asymmetry: On cognitive consistency and positivity bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 455-474.
- ^ Averill, J. R., (1980). On the paucity of positive emotions. In K. R. Blankstein, P. Pliner, & J. Polivy (Eds.), Advances in the study of communication and affect, Vol. 6 (pp. 7-45). New York: Plenum.
- ^ Carlson, E. R. (1966). The affective tone of psychology. Journal of General Psychology, 75, 65-78.
- ^ Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110,67-85.
- ^ Abele, A. (1985). Thinking about thinking: Causal, evaluative and finalistic cognitions about social situations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 315-332.
- ^ Anderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 1-9.
- ^ Averill, J. R., (1980). On the paucity of positive emotions. In K. R. Blankstein, P. Pliner, & J. Polivy (Eds.), Advances in the study of communication and affect, Vol. 6 (pp. 7-45). New York: Plenum.
- ^ a b Baumeister, R. F., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370.
- ^ Berry, D. S., Pennebaker, J. W., Mueller, J. S., & Hiller, W. S. (1997). Linguistic bases of social perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 526-537.
- ^ a b Boucher, J., & Osgood, C. E. (1969). The Pollyanna hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 1-8.
- ^ Bruner, J. S., & Tagiuri, R. (1954). Person perception. In G. Lindsey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 634-654). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
- ^ Carlson, E. R. (1966). The affective tone of psychology. Journal of General Psychology, 75, 65-78.
- ^ Carver, L. J., & Vaccaro, B. G. (2007). 12-month-old infants allocate increased neural resources to stimuli associated with negative adult emotion. Developmental Psychology, 43, 54-69.
- ^ a b Costantini, A. F., & Hoving, K. L. (1973). The effectiveness of reward and punishment contingencies on response inhibition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 484-494.
- ^ Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of evaluative and item information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior-based impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1758-1768.
- ^ Ebbesen, E. B., Kjos, G. L., & Konecni, V. J. (1976). Spatial ecology: Its effects on the choice of friends and enemies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 505-518.
- ^ Farroni, T., Menon, E., Rigato, s., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). The perception of facial expressions in newborns. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 2-13.
- ^ Feldman, S. (1966). Motivational aspects of attitudinal elements and their place in cognitive interaction. In S. Feldman (Ed.), Cognitive consistency: Motivational antecedents and behavioral consequences (pp. 75-108). New York: Academic Press.
- ^ Festinger, L., Schacter, S., & Back, K. (1950). Social pressures in informal groups: A study of a housing community. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
- ^ Finkenauer, C., & Rimé, B. (1998). Socially shared emotional experiences vs. emotional experiences kept secret: Differential characteristics and consequences. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 295-318.
- ^ Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 38, 889-906.
- ^ Fogarty, C., & Stern, J. A. (1989). Eye movements and blinks: Their relationship to higher cognitive processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 8, 35-42.
- ^ Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to the old: Age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 163-178.
- ^ Hamilton, D. L., & Huffman, L. J. (1971). Generality of impression formation processes for evaluative and nonevaluative judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20, 200-207.
- ^ a b Hamilton, D. L., & Zanna, M. P. (1972). Differential weighting of favorable and unfavorable attributes in impressions of personality. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 6, 204-212.
- ^ Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13, 923-929.
- ^ Hertenstein, M. J., & Campos, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation via maternal touch. Infancy, 2, 549-566.
- ^ Hodges, B. H. (1974). Effect of valence on relative weighting in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 378-381.
- ^ Holland, A. C., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). Younger, middle-aged, and older adults' memories for the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 163-170.
- ^ Huang, Y., & Luo, Y. (2006). Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 398, 91-96.
- ^ Irwin , M. Tripodi , T., & Bieri , J. (1967). Affective stimulus value and cognitive complexity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 444-448.
- ^ Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence of implicit and explicit categorization processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 660-676.
- ^ Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887-900.
- ^ a b Kanouse, D. E., & Hanson, L. (1972). Negativity in evaluations. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, S. Valins, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
- ^ Klein, J. G. (1991). Negativity effects in impression formation: A test in the political arena. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 412-418.
- ^ Klein, J. G. (1998). Negativity in intradimensional judgments of presidential candidates. Advances in Consumer Research, 25, 574-577.
- ^ a b Lewicka, M., Czapinski, J., & Peeters, G. (1992). Positive-negative asymmetry or "When the heart needs a reason." European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 425-434.
- ^ Martijn, C., Spears, R., van der Pligt, J., & Jakobs, E. (1992). Negativity and positivity effects in person perception and inference: Ability versus morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 453-463.
- ^ Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 496-502.
- ^ Mumme, D. L., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child Development, 74, 221-237.
- ^ Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants' responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219-3237.
- ^ Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Ohta, N. (Eds.) (2012). Memory and aging: Current issues and future directions. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
- ^ Ohira, H. (1996). Eyeballing activity in a word-naming task as a function of semantic priming and cognitive load. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 835-842.
- ^ a b Ohira, H., Winton, W. M., & Oyama, M. (1998). Effects of stimulus valence on recognition memory and endogenous eyeblinks: Further evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 986-993.
- ^ Peeters, G. (1971). The positive-negative asymmetry: On cognitive consistency and positivity bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 455-474.
- ^ Penny, R. K., & Lupton, A. A. (1961). Children's discrimination learning as a function of reward and punishment. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 449-451.
- ^ a b Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391.
- ^ Robinson-Riegler, G. L., & Winton, W. M. (1996). The role of conscious recollection in recognition of affective material: Evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Journal of General Psychology, 123, 93-104.
- ^ Schwartz, G. M., Izard, C. E., & Ansul, S. E. (1985). The 5-month-old's ability to discriminate facial expressions of emotion. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 65-77.
- ^ Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 689-699.
- ^ Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1992). Caught in the act: When impressions based on highly diagnostic behaviors are resistant to contradiction. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 435-452.
- ^ Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110,67-85.
- ^ Thomas, D. L., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 291-297.
- ^ Tindall, R. C., & Ratliff, R. G. (1974). Interaction of reinforcement conditions and development level in a two-choice discrimination task with children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 183-189.
- ^ a b Vaish, A., Grossman, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383-403.
- ^ Wells, J. D., Hobfoll, S. E., & Lavin, J. (1999). When it rains, it pours: The greater impact of resource loss compared to gain on psychological distress. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1171-1182.
- ^ Wilcox, B., & Clayton, F. (1968). Infant visual fication on motion pictures of human faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 6, 22-32.
- ^ Wood, S., & Kisley, M. A. (2006). The negativity bias is eliminated in older adults: Age-related reduction in event-related brain potentials associated with evaluative categorization. Psychology and Aging, 21, 815-820.
- ^ Wyer, R. S., & Hinkle, R. L. (1971). Informational factors underlying inferences about hypothetical persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 481-495.
- ^ Cite error: The named reference
Fiske 1980
was invoked but never defined (see the help page).