User:Jdonnini/Black-fronted titi
This is the sandbox page where you will draft your initial Wikipedia contribution.
If you're starting a new article, you can develop it here until it's ready to go live. If you're working on improvements to an existing article, copy only one section at a time of the article to this sandbox to work on, and be sure to use an edit summary linking to the article you copied from. Do not copy over the entire article. You can find additional instructions here. Remember to save your work regularly using the "Publish page" button. (It just means 'save'; it will still be in the sandbox.) You can add bold formatting to your additions to differentiate them from existing content. |
The black-fronted titi (Callicebus nigrifrons) is a species of titi, a type of New World monkey.
The black-fronted titi is a small diurnal primate. [1] The body of this primate is covered in grey to brown fur with black fur concentrated around the face, the tail is slightly orange in color. [2][3] Body weight ranges from 1 to 2 kilograms and the head to body length is around 270 to 450 millimeters.[2] This species does not exhibit sexual dimorphism. [3] Members of this species can live up to 12 years of age in captivity.[4]
Habitat and Distribution
[edit]The black-fronted titi is endemic to the Atlantic forest region of Brazil [5][6][2] and has a home range averaging 20 hectares.[6] The black-fronted titi is arboreal and prefers the middle to upper canopy of the forest.[3] However, it will move to the forest floor at times to forage, travel, and play.[4] Play behavior on the forest floor has been documented between black-fronted titis and marmosets in Brazil.[7]
Ecology
[edit]Diet
[edit]The diet of the black-fronted titi is frugivorous and they forage in dense vegetation.[3] They are also known to eat leaves, seeds, invertebrates, and flowers. [5][8][6] Due to their highly frugivorous diet, they play a key role in seed dispersion.[9]
Predation
[edit]Predators of the black-fronted titi include raptors, mammalian carnivores, snakes, and other primates.[3][1] Black-fronted titis are particularly vulnerable to raptor attacks when they move to the upper portion of the canopy to sunbathe on cold mornings.[3] After detecting a raptor black-fronted titis respond quickly using alarm calls and hiding.[3]
Behaviour
[edit]The black-fronted titi is socially monogamous[10][3][2] and is typically found in a group of two to six individuals, which includes the adult pair and their offspring.[4][2] Females give birth to one offspring per year, usually in July or August.[11] Parental care and social activities with the offspring are carried out by the male of this species, while the female only provides milk.[11] Both males and females disperse from their natal group at three years of age. [4]
The black-fronted titi is territorial and will defend territories, food resources, and mates with loud calls individually or in duets or choruses.[6][4] Loud calls are used for within and between group communication and have a high amplitude and a low frequency which allows them to be heard over long distances.[12] Loud calls are broadcasted at dawn and when titis see or hear another group.[6] When confronted by a predator, duets and choruses can last up to two hours, with group members alternating between soft and loud calls. [13] There are no sex differences in calling behavior during predator interactions, both males and females will call. [3] The black-fronted titi can produce calls which encode the predator type (aerial or terrestrial) and predator location to nearby conspecifics.[14]
Characteristic of the Callicebinae family, black-fronted titis can be observed with interwoven tails, a behavior thought to reinforce pair bonds and strengthen social relationships.[15][16][17]
Conservation
[edit]The black-fronted titi is classified as near threatened by the IUCN due to extensive habitat loss, forest fragmentation, and an estimated population decline of more than 20% in the past 24 years.[4] Small, isolated populations are common due to fragmentation and in some areas this has led to the species going locally extinct.[2] Noise pollution can also negatively impact this species. One study found that noise from mining operations restricted the black-fronted titis long distance communication due to the overlap in frequency between mining noise and loud calls, this is significant for a species that relies heavily on vocal communication in social interactions.[6]
References
[edit]- ^ a b Caselli, Christini B.; Gestich, Carla C.; Nagy-Reis, Mariana B. (2017-08-01). "Sleeping above the enemy: Sleeping site choice by black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons )". American Journal of Primatology. 79 (10): e22688. doi:10.1002/ajp.22688. ISSN 0275-2565.
{{cite journal}}
: line feed character in|title=
at position 100 (help) - ^ a b c d e f dos Santos, Glênio Pereira; Galvão, Cristiane; Young, Robert J. (2012-02-05). "The diet of wild black-fronted titi monkeys Callicebus nigrifrons during a bamboo masting year". Primates. 53 (3): 265–272. doi:10.1007/s10329-012-0295-5. ISSN 0032-8332.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Cristiane., Cäsar, (2012). Anti-predator behaviour of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). University of St Andrews. OCLC 806194348.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f Berthet, Mélissa; Mesbahi, Geoffrey; Duvot, Guilhem; Zuberbühler, Klaus; Cäsar, Cristiane; Bicca‐Marques, Júlio Cèsar (2021-10-05). "Dramatic decline in a titi monkey population after the 2016–2018 sylvatic yellow fever outbreak in Brazil". American Journal of Primatology. 83 (12). doi:10.1002/ajp.23335. ISSN 0275-2565.
- ^ a b Caselli, Christini Barbosa; Setz, Eleonore Zulnara Freire (2011-08-23). "Feeding ecology and activity pattern of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons) in a semideciduous tropical forest of southern Brazil". Primates. 52 (4): 351–359. doi:10.1007/s10329-011-0266-2. ISSN 0032-8332.
- ^ a b c d e f Duarte, M. H. L.; Kaizer, M. C.; Young, R. J.; Rodrigues, M.; Sousa-Lima, R. S. (2017-09-11). "Mining noise affects loud call structures and emission patterns of wild black-fronted titi monkeys". Primates. 59 (1): 89–97. doi:10.1007/s10329-017-0629-4. ISSN 0032-8332.
- ^ Souza-Alves, João Pedro; Mourthe, Italo; Hilário, Renato R.; Bicca-Marques, Júlio César; Rehg, Jennifer; Gestich, Carla C.; Acero-Murcia, Adriana C.; Adret, Patrice; Aquino, Rolando; Berthet, Mélissa; Bowler, Mark (2019-10-01). "Terrestrial Behavior in Titi Monkeys (Callicebus, Cheracebus, and Plecturocebus): Potential Correlates, Patterns, and Differences between Genera". International Journal of Primatology. 40 (4): 553–572. doi:10.1007/s10764-019-00105-x. ISSN 1573-8604.
- ^ Nagy-Reis, Mariana B.; Setz, Eleonore Z. F. (2016-08-02). "Foraging strategies of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons) in relation to food availability in a seasonal tropical forest". Primates. 58 (1): 149–158. doi:10.1007/s10329-016-0556-9. ISSN 0032-8332.
- ^ Gestich, Carla Cristina; Nagy-Reis, Mariana B.; Caselli, Christini Barbosa (2019-10). "From dropping to dropping: The contribution of a small primate to seed dispersal in Atlantic Forest". Acta Oecologica. 100: 103464. doi:10.1016/j.actao.2019.103464. ISSN 1146-609X.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help) - ^ Souza-Alves, João Pedro; Caselli, Christini B.; Gestich, Carla C.; Nagy-Reis, Mariana B. (2019-02-20). "Should I store, or should I sync? The breeding strategy of two small Neotropical primates under predictable resource availability". Primates. 60 (2): 113–118. doi:10.1007/s10329-019-00716-1. ISSN 0032-8332.
- ^ a b Cäsar, Cristiane; Young, Robert John (2007-10-16). "A case of adoption in a wild group of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons)". Primates. 49 (2): 146–148. doi:10.1007/s10329-007-0066-x. ISSN 0032-8332.
- ^ Caselli, Christini; Mennill, Daniel; Bicca-Marques, Júlio César; Setz, Eleonore (2014-03-03). "Vocal behavior of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons): Acoustic properties and behavioral contexts of loud calls". American Journal of Primatology. 76: 788–800.
- ^ Narbona Sabaté, Lara; Mesbahi, Geoffrey; Dezecache, Guillaume; Cäsar, Cristiane; Zuberbühler, Klaus; Berthet, Mélissa (2022-01-09). "Animal linguistics in the making: the Urgency Principle and titi monkeys' alarm system". Ethology Ecology & Evolution. 0 (0): 1–17. doi:10.1080/03949370.2021.2015452. ISSN 0394-9370.
- ^ Cäsar, Cristiane; Zuberbühler, Klaus; Young, Robert J.; Byrne, Richard W. (2013-10-23). "Titi monkey call sequences vary with predator location and type". Biology Letters. 9 (5): 20130535. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0535. PMC 3971693. PMID 24004492.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Moynihan, M. (1966). "Communication in the Titi monkey, Callicebus". Journal of Zoology. 150 (1): 77–127. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1966.tb02999.x. ISSN 1469-7998.
- ^ "Titi monkey". Smithsonian's National Zoo. 2016-04-25. Retrieved 2022-02-09.
- ^ Bezerra, BM; Ferrari, S; Boyle, SA; Veiga, LM (August 2009). "Pitheciine Action Group Newsletter" (PDF). p. 13. Retrieved 2022-02-09.