Jump to content

User:Maryam Edrisi/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

The Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis[edit]

The Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis (CPH) states that women are more likely to show disgust and pathogen avoidance in the luteal phase of their menstrual cycle when the increase in the progesterone represses their immune system [1][2][3]. In this case, disgust sensitivity is hypothesized as a compensatory mechanism may contribute to avoiding pathogens [4]. The CPH is a psychological response that contributes to behavioral avoidance of pathogens [4]. Additionally, disgust sensitivity has per se investigated in the three domains of pathogen, sexual, and moral disgust [5].

Progesterone[edit]

Progesterone plays multiple roles in women’s bodies and helps regulate their reproductive systems. Additionally, as a sex hormone that is primarily produced in the luteal phase, it can affect reproductive immune modulation [6][7][1][2]. The reproductive immunomodulation mechanism driven by progesterone leads to restraint on natural killer (NK) cells’ activity [8][9]. Another widely accepted hypothesis discusses the connection between the surge of progesterone during pregnancy and its impacts on compensatory prophylaxis [10].

Pregnancy[edit]

Similar to the spike of progesterone observed during the luteal period, an increase in the concentration of progesterone has been noticed close to the end of pregnancy. Hence, it has been hypothesized that progesterone spikes could be correlated with feelings of disgust during this period in pregnant women. In the meantime, pregnant women report feelings of disgust and nausea more often during the first trimester of their pregnancy [11] while the surge in progesterone levels happens close to the end of the pregnancy [10]. This can potentially raise doubts about the connection between higher progesterone levels and disgust feelings [12].  In other words, the hypothesized connection could be stronger if higher progesterone concentrations and increased feelings of disgust would both happen close to the end of pregnancy. Another observation that cast doubts on progesterone being the driving factor in the increased disgust feelings during pregnancy has been presented by social neuroendocrine research. In these studies, the higher concentration of progesterone is even connected to reduced social anxiety which can lead to increased willingness for social affiliation [13][14]. All these would indicate the possibility of other reproductive immunomodulation factors driving pathogen disgust during the menstrual cycle [12].

Evolutionary significance[edit]

An evolutionary explanation for the modifications resulting in the weakening of the immune system during the luteal phase and pregnancy is protecting the embryo from possible attacks of the immune system against the half-foreign blastocyst [1][15]. This protection enables the embryo to complete the implementation process. On the other hand, the weakened immune system in woman makes her (and the future embryo) more vulnerable to infections. For this reason, a mechanism that contributes to avoidance behavior towards pathogens could be preferred by natural selection [16][17][18][19].

Disgust Sensitivity[edit]

Feelings of disgust can be manifested in a variety of domains [1] but most common ones include pathogens, sexual, and moral disgust [5].

Pathogen Disgust[edit]

Pathogen disgust can drive the feeling of avoidance of infection factors and increased hygienic behavior [20][21][22][23][10]. Other observations have also shown an increased cleaning behavior in some women during the luteal phase [24] as well as a domain of OCD behavior in which they showed an increased desire for washing and cleaning activities that can reduce the possibility of infections [3]. Similarly, it was predicted that women show a decreased craving for meat, a major pathogenic food, in their diet during the luteal phase [4]. Although the effect of the cycle phase on meat consumption was not supported in the studies, observations showed decreased consumption of meat intake in sexually active women [4]. On the contrary, while some research disagrees with the connection between progesterone concentrations and pathogen disgust [25], a comprehensive study conducted by Karoline (2021), has shown the link between increased progesterone and increased pathogen disgust [10].

Sexual Disgust[edit]

Sexual disgust can be defined as a behavior in which the person avoids from participation in suboptimal sexual activities [1]. In this definition, suboptimal sexual activity is considered an activity that could not have optimal fitness impacts [1][26][27]. Examples of such activities include incest, age-disparate unions, and bestiality [26][28][29][30]. Since sexual disgust aims to prevent sexual activities with less optimal outcomes, sexual sensitivity is expected to vary throughout the menstrual cycle regarding to the variation of conceptus [1]. Some findings consistent with this prediction, and they show that women have an increased tendency to refrain from activities that could expose them to sexual assault [31][32]. On the other hand, there is research questioning the increased sexual disgust in the preovulatory period. These studies argued that there is not sufficient evidence that feelings of sexual disgust can be driven by hormones [25].   

Moral Disgust[edit]

Moral disgust can be defined as the negative feeling when social norms are violated. Also, studies show that moral disgust could happen even when less serious social values are disregarded such as gossip, lesser acts of aggression [33][34], or some feeling of disgust towards pathogen or sexual acts, which are known as taboos, could be considered morally inappropriate and need to be punished [5]. Therefore, in the same vein as the pathogen and the sexual disgust, moral disgust may increase in the luteal phase and women’s moral punishments for moral violations might increase. These norms violations could be incest, bestiality, purity violation, or even have different race, sex, religion (xenophobia) [5][10]. On the other hand, some studies argue that there is no enough evidence for women’s cyclical moral disgust [10][25].

On the whole, a recent study [10] stated that there are some methodological limitations in the study of CHP. One of the limitations are the estimation related to the cycle phase (e.g., the length or the day of the cycle) [1][35]. It is suggested that for testing CHP, ovulatory tests are necessary [10].  The other limitations are utilizing only one method for the disgust measurement (i.e., just using questionnaires and not using pictures) [10]. Additionally, the design of some studies’ method is based on between-subject design which is problematic [10]. Research design on CPH need to measure each woman’s disgust sensitivity repeatedly (between her phases) instead of comparing females with each other, or doing a single measurement [10].

Disgust measurement[edit]

The most respected and trusted methods of disgust sensitivity measuring the pathogen and moral domains are Three Domain Disgust Scale (TDDS) [5], and a measurement based on pathogenic relevant pictures (image-based) [20]. A recent study [10], in addition to disgust assessment of Pathogen and Moral domains, used Contamination Obsessions and Washing Compulsions Subscale of Padua Inventory – Washington State University Revision[36] to measure the contamination sensitivity.

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h Fessler, Daniel M.T.; Navarrete, C.David (2003-11). "Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle". Evolution and Human Behavior. 24 (6): 406–417. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(03)00054-0. ISSN 1090-5138. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  2. ^ a b Fleischman, Diana S.; Fessler, Daniel M. T. (2007-07-01). "Differences in Dietary Intake as a Function of Sexual Activity and Hormonal Contraception". Evolutionary Psychology. 5 (3): 147470490700500. doi:10.1177/147470490700500312. ISSN 1474-7049.
  3. ^ a b Fleischman, Diana S.; Fessler, Daniel M.T. (2011-02). "Progesterone's effects on the psychology of disease avoidance: Support for the compensatory behavioral prophylaxis hypothesis". Hormones and Behavior. 59 (2): 271–275. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.11.014. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  4. ^ a b c d "Genetica-Biologia Molecolare". Rivista di Neuroradiologia. 14 (4_suppl): 17–31. 2001-12. doi:10.1177/19714009010140s403. ISSN 1120-9976. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  5. ^ a b c d e Tybur, Joshua M.; Lieberman, Debra; Griskevicius, Vladas (2009). "Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 97 (1): 103–122. doi:10.1037/a0015474. ISSN 1939-1315.
  6. ^ Clancy, Kathryn B. H.; Baerwald, Angela R.; Pierson, Roger A. (2013-05-22). Hawks, John (ed.). "Systemic Inflammation Is Associated with Ovarian Follicular Dynamics during the Human Menstrual Cycle". PLoS ONE. 8 (5): e64807. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064807. ISSN 1932-6203.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  7. ^ Clancy, Kathryn B.H.; Klein, Laura D.; Ziomkiewicz, Anna; Nenko, Ilona; Jasienska, Grazyna; Bribiescas, Richard G. (2013-05). "Relationships between biomarkers of inflammation, ovarian steroids, and age at menarche in a rural polish sample: Urinary Crp and Ovarian Function in Rural Poland". American Journal of Human Biology. 25 (3): 389–398. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22386. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  8. ^ Druckmann, René; Druckmann, Marc-Alexandre (2005-12). "Progesterone and the immunology of pregnancy". The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology. 97 (5): 389–396. doi:10.1016/j.jsbmb.2005.08.010. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  9. ^ Żelaźniewicz, Agnieszka; Borkowska, Barbara; Nowak, Judyta; Pawłowski, Bogusław (2016-07). "The progesterone level, leukocyte count and disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle". Physiology & Behavior. 161: 60–65. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.04.002. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Miłkowska, Karolina; Galbarczyk, Andrzej; Klimek, Magdalena; Zabłocka-Słowińska, Katarzyna; Jasienska, Grazyna (2021-09). "Pathogen disgust, but not moral disgust, changes across the menstrual cycle". Evolution and Human Behavior. 42 (5): 402–408. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.03.002. ISSN 1090-5138. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  11. ^ Fessler, D. M., Eng, S. J., & Navarrette, C. D. (2005). Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy: Evidence supporting the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 344–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. evolhumbehav.2004.12.001.
  12. ^ a b Fleischman, D. S., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2018). Response to “hormonal correlates of pathogen disgust: Testing the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis”. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39, 468–469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. evolhumbehav.2018.03.006.
  13. ^ Brown, S. L., Fredrickson, B. L., Wirth, M. M., Poulin, M. J., Meier, E. A., Heaphy, E. D., … Schultheiss, O. C. (2009). Social closeness increases salivary progesterone in humans. Horomones and Bevaior, 56, 108–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. yhbeh.2009.03.022.
  14. ^ Wirth, M. M. (2011). Beyond the HPA Axis: Progesterone-derived neuroactive steroids in human stress and emotion. Frontiers in Endocrinology, 2, 19. https://doi.org/ 10.3389/fendo.2011.00019.
  15. ^ "Evolution: Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology. Number VII". AIBS Bulletin. 3 (5): 11–12. 1953-10-01. doi:10.1093/aibsbulletin/3.5.11-h. ISSN 0096-7645.
  16. ^ Curtis V, de Barra M (July 2018). "The structure and function of pathogen disgust". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 373 (1751): 20170208. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0208. PMC 6000136. PMID 29866921
  17. ^ Ewald, Paul W. (1994). Evolution of infectious disease. Oxford. ISBN 1-4237-3469-6. OCLC 228117631.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  18. ^ Combes, Claude (2001). Parasitism : the ecology and evolution of intimate interactions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-11445-7. OCLC 45861910.
  19. ^ Schulenburg H, Ewbank JJ (November 2007). "The genetics of pathogen avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans". Molecular Microbiology. 66 (3): 563–70. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2958.2007.05946.x. PMID 17877707. S2CID 20783253
  20. ^ a b Curtis, Val; Aunger, Robert; Rabie, Tamer (2004-05-07). "Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 271 (suppl_4). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144. ISSN 0962-8452.
  21. ^ Curtis, Valerie; de Barra, Mícheál; Aunger, Robert (2011-02-12). "Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 366 (1563): 389–401. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0117. ISSN 0962-8436.
  22. ^ Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I., & Oaten, M. J. (2009). Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 5, 363–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. evolhumbehav.2009.02.005.
  23. ^ Wicker, Bruno; Keysers, Christian; Plailly, Jane; Royet, Jean-Pierre; Gallese, Vittorio; Rizzolatti, Giacomo (2003-10). "Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  24. ^ Dillon, Kathleen M.; Brooks, Deborah (1992-02). "Unusual Cleaning Behavior in the Luteal Phase". Psychological Reports. 70 (1): 35–39. doi:10.2466/pr0.1992.70.1.35. ISSN 0033-2941. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  25. ^ a b c Jones, Benedict C.; Hahn, Amanda C.; Fisher, Claire I.; Wang, Hongyi; Kandrik, Michal; Lee, Anthony J.; Tybur, Joshua M.; DeBruine, Lisa M. (2018-03). "Hormonal correlates of pathogen disgust: testing the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis". Evolution and Human Behavior. 39 (2): 166–169. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.004. ISSN 1090-5138. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  26. ^ a b Lieberman, Debra; Tooby, John; Cosmides, Leda (2003-04-22). "Does morality have a biological basis? An empirical test of the factors governing moral sentiments relating to incest". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 270 (1517): 819–826. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2290. ISSN 0962-8452.
  27. ^ Ketelaar, Timothy (2019-12-01). "Lisa F. Barrett, Michael Lewis, and Jeannette M. Haviland Jones, eds. Handbook of Emotions, 4th ed". Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture. 3 (1): 111–114. doi:10.26613/esic.3.1.129. ISSN 2472-9876.
  28. ^ Angyal, A. (1941-07). "Disgust and related aversions". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 36 (3): 393–412. doi:10.1037/h0058254. ISSN 0096-851X. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  29. ^ Schaich Borg, Jana; Lieberman, Debra; Kiehl, Kent A. (2008-09-01). "Infection, Incest, and Iniquity: Investigating the Neural Correlates of Disgust and Morality". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 20 (9): 1529–1546. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20109. ISSN 0898-929X.
  30. ^ Haidt, Jonathan; Rozin, Paul; Mccauley, Clark; Imada, Sumio (1997-03). "Body, Psyche, and Culture: The Relationship between Disgust and Morality". Psychology and Developing Societies. 9 (1): 107–131. doi:10.1177/097133369700900105. ISSN 0971-3336. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  31. ^ Chavanne, Tara J; Gallup, Gordon G (1998-01). "Variation in Risk Taking Behavior Among Female College Students as a Function of the Menstrual Cycle". Evolution and Human Behavior. 19 (1): 27–32. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(98)00016-6. ISSN 1090-5138. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  32. ^ Petralia, Sandra M.; Gallup, Gordon G. (2002-01). "Effects of a sexual assault scenario on handgrip strength across the menstrual cycle". Evolution and Human Behavior. 23 (1): 3–10. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(01)00085-x. ISSN 1090-5138. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  33. ^ Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Güler, E., Balliet, D., & Hofmann, W. (2017). Disgust and anger relate to different aggressive responses to moral violations. Psychological Science, 28, 609–619. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617692000.
  34. ^ Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Van Lange, P. A. M., & Balliet, D. (2020). Direct and indirect punishment of norm violations in daily life. Nature Communications, 11, 3432. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17286-2.
  35. ^ Milkowska, Karolina; Galbarczyk, Andrzej; Jasienska, Grazyna (2019-03-22). "Disgust sensitivity in relation to menstrual cycle phase in women with and without an infection". American Journal of Human Biology. 31 (3). doi:10.1002/ajhb.23233. ISSN 1042-0533.
  36. ^ Burns, G.Leonard; Keortge, Susan G.; Formea, Gina M.; Sternberger, Lee G. (1996-02). "Revision of the Padua Inventory of obsessive compulsive disorder symptoms: Distinctions between worry, obsessions, and compulsions". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 34 (2): 163–173. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00035-6. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)