User:Sburton17/signature whistle
Signature Whistle
[edit]A signature whistle is a learned, individually distinctive whistle type in a Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) acoustic repertoire that gives the identity of the whistle owner.[1] The whistles are identified and studied in the wild or in captivity by researchers using hydrophones.[2] Vocal learning strongly influences the development of signature whistles,[3] which can remain stable for up to at least 12 years.[4] More often than not, calves develop signature whistles by the sounds they hear while making sure that they differ from close associates.[1] However, male calves tend to learn signature whistles that resemble their mother's.[1] They are believed to be most frequently used in communication of captive bottlenose dolphins,[4] along with having specialized functions and properties.[5] Researchers define it as a whistle with a unique frequency curve that dominates in the repertoire of a dolphin.[6] Each dolphin has a distinct signature whistle that other members of its social group use to individually identify the whistler.[4] They are typically used for localisation purposes, however they also provide dolphins with behavioural context.[7][8] Signature whistles also play an important role in group cohesion and social interaction.[3]
Identification of signature whistles
[edit]Identification of signature whistle calls from wild, unrestrained bottlenose dolphin's is essential in order to understand how they are used in their natural environment.[1]
Signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins can be recorded in the wild or in captivity.[6][3] Hydrophones are used in both cases, but the number of devices may vary based on the researcher's preference and methodology. Using multiple hydrophones allows the researchers to better identify which dolphin emitted which whistle.[8] Although multiple hydrophones are used, signature identification uses a single hydrophone and allows better identification.[2] Variation between signature whistles can be tested by examining the whistle contours frequency modulation pattern.[1] A frequency modulation pattern differs between dolphins, which is usually identified by using standard acoustic parameter measurements, such as beginning, end, maximum or minimum frequency.[1] Combining the measurements of the frequencies will show the modulation pattern of the individuals signature whistle.
Signature whistles are difficult to identify because bottlenose dolphins emit non-signature whistles as well. However, there is a distinct amount of time in between each signature whistle that helps researchers distinguish them from the rest of dolphin noises.[2] Signature whistles are emitted within 1 to 10 milliseconds of each other, while non-signature whistles occur with longer or shorter intervals between each whistle.[2] This distinction can be used to identify signature whistles in hydrophone recordings.
Development
[edit]A dolphin's signature whistle development is influenced by auditory experience,[1] and usually fully develops within the first year of life, rarely changing throughout adulthood.[9] Stable whistle call structure for at least a decade is necessary for long term call recognition between bottlenose dolphin's.[1] A calf develops its own signature whistle based on the sounds they hear and most calves develop signature whistles that differ from their close associates.[1] Calves tend to model their signature whistles after those of adult dolphins who they do not spend much time with.[9] Due to the fact that signature whistles are unique, a calf's signature whistle never matches a single whistle but instead has similarities to multiple whistles.[9] However, male calves tend to develop signature whistles that resemble the signature whistles of their mother's.[1] The male calf does not copy the whistle, instead it uses it as a model.[9]
Vocal learning is essential in a dolphin calf's signature whistle development.[10] It plays an important role in development because it helps to build an individualized whistle with a unique frequency modulation pattern.[1] It is an animal's ability to imitate vocalizations from other animals of the same species, and eventually produces its own sounds.[11] Social interaction plays a significant role in vocal learning.[9] Although vocal learning is often associated with aggressive behavior in some animals, this is not the case in dolphins.[10] Rather, vocal learning strengthens social bonds, such as those between mother-calf pairs and alliance partners.[10]
Apart from signature whistles being developed socially there are other behavioural, ecological, genetic and modification factors that can also influence the development of a dolphin's whistle.[1] For example, signature whistles may vary from a behavioural factor and the temporal production of whistles can be affected by stress or other psychological influences.[1] Determining the contribution of different influences on signature whistle variation, aside from social influence, is difficult to achieve due to minimal populations. It is best examined in multiple populations using recordings collected from various behavioural contexts.[1]
Group interaction
[edit]Between groups
[edit]Dolphins also use signature whistles to provide identity information when meeting new groups of dolphins.[8][12] When groups of dolphin's first join whistle exchanges are a necessary component of the social interaction.[12] Signature whistles between groups are the primary source in providing identity information due to the fact that only 10 percent of joins occurred without whistles.[12] Signature whistle emission rates are nine times higher when two groups encounter each other than in peak whistle rates during general socialization within pods; and they repeat them multiple times during the exchange.[12] The repetition of signature whistle's increases the chances of correct identification.[12]
Whistle matching may occur when two or more groups of dolphins encounter each other, as well.[8] Bottlenose dolphin's have low rates of aggression towards close associates and other members of the species.[10] It is observed when a dolphin copies another individual's whistle and repeats it back.[8] Matching occurs in order for one dolphin to acknowledge another individual's presence, which can occur when the animals are up to 600 meters apart.[8] Signature whistles may play an important role in interactions between groups.
References
[edit]- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n Kriesell, Hannah Joy; Elwen, Simon Harvey; Nastasi, Aurora; Gridley, Tess (2014-09-09). "Identification and Characteristics of Signature Whistles in Wild Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from Namibia". PLOS ONE. 9 (9): e106317. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0106317. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4159226. PMID 25203814.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ a b c d Janik, V; King, S (2013). "Identifying signature whistles from recordings of groups of unrestrained bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". Marine Mammal Science. 29: 109–122. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2011.00549.x.
- ^ a b c Janik, V; Sayigh L; Wells R (2006). "Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins". PNAS. 103 (21): 8293–8297. doi:10.1073/pnas.0509918103. PMC 1472465. PMID 16698937.
- ^ a b c Janik, Vincent M.; Todt, Dietmar; Dehnhardt, G. (1994-10-01). "Signature whistle variations in a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 35 (4): 243–248. doi:10.1007/BF00170704. ISSN 1432-0762.
- ^ Janik, V (2009). Acoustic communication in delphinids. Vol. 40. pp. 123–157. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(09)40004-4. ISBN 9780123744753.
{{cite book}}
:|journal=
ignored (help) - ^ a b Agafonov, A; Panova, E (2012). "Individual patterns of tonal (whistling) signals of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncates) kept in relative isolation". Zoology. 39 (5): 430–440. doi:10.1134/S1062359012050020.
- ^ Lopez, B (2011). "Whistle characteristics in free-ranging bottlenose dolphins in the Mediterranean Sea: influence of behavior". Mammalian Biology. 76 (2): 180–189. doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2010.06.006.
- ^ a b c d e f Janik, V (2000). "Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins". Science. 289 (5483): 1355–1357. doi:10.1126/science.289.5483.1355.
- ^ a b c d e Fripp, D; Owen C; Quintana-Rizzo E; Shapiro A; Buckstaff K; Jankowski K; Wells R; Tyack P (2005). "Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncates) calves appear to model their signature whistles on the signature whistles of community members". Animal Cognition. 8 (1): 17–26. doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0225-z. PMID 15221637.
- ^ a b c d King, S; Sayigh L; Wells R; Fellner W; Janik V (2013). "Vocal copying of individually distinctive signature whistes in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 280 (1757): 20130053. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0053. PMC 3619487. PMID 23427174.
- ^ Nottebohm, F (1972). "The origins of vocal learning". The American Naturalist. 106 (947): 116–140. doi:10.1086/282756.
- ^ a b c d e Quick, N; Janik V (2012). "Bottlenose dolphins exchange signature whistles when meeting at sea". Proceedings of the Royal Society. 279 (1738): 2539–2545. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2537. PMC 3350692. PMID 22378804.