User:SJK/ASD bibliography
Appearance
< User:SJK
Bibliography of interesting sources on the topic of autism spectrum disorder. I hope that I might be able to insert some of these references into one or more articles (although that depends on whether other editors let me do it).
Allen Frances quotes
[edit]- Allen Frances [@AllenFrancesMD] (Jul 6, 2019). "I very much regret adding #Aspergers to DSM-IV because it was wildly over-used & stigmatized millions of people. The DSM-5 #Autism spectrum is even worse for the same reasons. But the genie is now out of the bottle & I doubt we will ever go back to more conservative diagnosis" (Tweet) – via Twitter.
- @AllenFrancesMD (Aug 22, 2019). "I very much regret our including #Aspergers in DSM-IV. New study confirms #Autism has since been very over-diagnosed. Terrible consequences: anti-vax hysteria/measles epidemics/stigmatization/needless worry/medicalizing normal diversity" (Tweet) – via Twitter.
Validity
[edit]Validity of DSM/ICD diagnoses in general
[edit]- Weinberger, Daniel R.; Glick, Ira D.; Klein, Donald F. (2015-12-01). "Whither Research Domain Criteria (RDoC)?: The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly". JAMA Psychiatry. 72 (12): 1161. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1743. ISSN 2168-622X.
- "That DSM-5 is a less than ideal approach to clinical diagnosis is evident. It is purely phenomenological and largely arbitrary, and not based on valid etiological concepts or mechanisms of illness or genetic predispositions"
- "Diagnostic system lacks validity" (PDF). Council for Evidence-Based Psychiatry. 15 March 2014.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link)- "Psychiatric diagnostic manuals such as the DSM and ICD (chapter 5) are not works of objective science, but rather works of culture since they have largely been developed through clinical consensus and voting. Their validity and clinical utility is therefore highly questionable..."
- Frances, Allen J.; Egger, Helen Link (April 1999). "Whither Psychiatric Diagnosis". Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 33 (2): 161–165. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1614.1999.00534.x. ISSN 0004-8674.
- ""An effective clinician... must not worship at the altar of diagnosis, because in real life patients do not fit neatly into diagnostic categories"
- Hyman, Steven E. (March 2010). "The Diagnosis of Mental Disorders: The Problem of Reification". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 6 (1): 155–179. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091532. ISSN 1548-5943.
- Wakefield, J. C. (June 2015). "DSM-5, psychiatric epidemiology and the false positives problem". Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences. 24 (3): 188–196. doi:10.1017/S2045796015000116. ISSN 2045-7960.
- "The revision effort leading to the publication of the fifth edition of the American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) was flawed in process, goals and outcome. The revision process suffered from lack of an adequate public record of the rationale for changes, thus shortchanging future scholarship. The goals, such as dimensionalising diagnosis, incorporating biomarkers and separating impairment from diagnosis, were ill-considered and mostly abandoned. However, DSM-5's greatest problem, and the target of the most vigorous and sustained criticism, was its failure to take seriously the false positives problem. By expanding diagnosis beyond plausible boundaries in ways inconsistent with DSM-5's own definition of disorder, DSM-5 threatened the validity of psychiatric research, including especially psychiatric epidemiology..."
- Khoury, Bassam; Langer, Ellen J.; Pagnini, Francesco (2014-06-17). "The DSM: mindful science or mindless power? A critical review". Frontiers in Psychology. 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00602. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 4060802. PMID 24987385.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
Validity of ASD in particular
[edit]- Waterhouse, Lynn; London, Eric; Gillberg, Christopher (2016-12-01). "ASD Validity". Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 3 (4): 302–329. doi:10.1007/s40489-016-0085-x. ISSN 2195-7185.
- Müller, Ralph-Axel; Amaral, David G. (January 2017). "Editorial: Time to give up on Autism Spectrum Disorder?: Time to give up on Autism Spectrum Disorder?". Autism Research. 10 (1): 10–14. doi:10.1002/aur.1746.
- "Waterhouse et al. [2016] do not claim to present a meta‐analysis, and indeed the empirical evidence reviewed is highly selective and in many respects overly simplified. However, these shortcomings are not crucial, as the cited evidence sufficiently demonstrates inconsistencies in empirical findings that the authors suggest indicate a lack of neurobiological validity of the construct of ‘autism spectrum disorder’. The attempt to raise awareness is laudable, but while the conclusions are radical, the underlying insight is not particularly novel..."
- Waterhouse, Lynn; London, Eric; Gillberg, Christopher (July 2017). "The ASD diagnosis has blocked the discovery of valid biological variation in neurodevelopmental social impairment". Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research. 10 (7): 1182. doi:10.1002/aur.1832. ISSN 1939-3806. PMID 28714261.
- Müller, Ralph-Axel (July 2017). "Abandoning ASD? A response to Waterhouse, London, and Gillberg". Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research. 10 (7): 1183. doi:10.1002/aur.1833. ISSN 1939-3806. PMID 28714260.
- Müller, Ralph-Axel; Amaral, David G. (January 2017). "Editorial: Time to give up on Autism Spectrum Disorder?: Time to give up on Autism Spectrum Disorder?". Autism Research. 10 (1): 10–14. doi:10.1002/aur.1746.
- Timimi, Sami (9 May 2011). "Autism is not a scientifically valid or clinically useful diagnosis". BMJ.
- Timimi, Sami. (2011). The myth of autism : medicalising men's and boys' social and emotional competence. Gardner, Neil., McCabe, Brian. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. ISBN 978-0-230-54526-7. OCLC 685065512.
- Reviews:
- Melling, Richard (2011). "The Myth of Autism: medicalising men's and boys' social and emotional competence – By Sami Timimi, Neil Gardner and Brian McCabe". British Journal of Special Education. 38 (3): 154–155. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8578.2011.00511.x. ISSN 1467-8578.
- Cross, Merry (2012-06-01). "The myth of autism: medicalising men's and boys' social and emotional competence". Disability & Society. 27 (4): 587–588. doi:10.1080/09687599.2012.674766. ISSN 0968-7599.
- Reviews:
- Runswick-Cole, Katherine; Mallett, Rebecca; Timimi, Sami, eds. (2016). Re-thinking autism : diagnosis, identity and equality. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. ISBN 1-78450-027-5. OCLC 948780403.
- London, Eric B. (2014-12-01). "Categorical diagnosis: a fatal flaw for autism research?". Trends in Neurosciences. 37 (12): 683–686. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2014.10.003. ISSN 0166-2236. PMID 25465942.
- "The DSM-3 provided reliability of diagnosis, but without attention to etiology or pathophysiology. The DSM-3 was created by utilizing the ‘wise professor model’, with one influential physician describing and classifying his patients and, from that, creating categorical diagnoses. Efforts to validate these diagnoses have been disappointing."
- "Nosological constructs such as phenotype, diagnosis, and syndromes are manmade tools that can be useful for various tasks but should not be thought of as absolute truth... DSM diagnoses frequently tell us little about etiology, lack biological markers, have widely varying and poorly predictable prognoses, have multiple comorbidities (often outweighing the clinical significance of the original diagnosis), merge into other disorders or into neurotypicality and therefore lack clear boundaries between disease and health or between disorders, lack rigor in their description of symptoms, and are not predictably responsive to treatments."
- "Perhaps more pernicious is the widespread reification (making something real, absent of evidence) of these diagnoses, creating premature closure of debate on the quality of the diagnosis and facilitating false premises on which research is conducted..."
- Waterhouse, Lynn H. (2013). Rethinking Autism : Variation and Complexity (1st ed.). London: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-391413-2. OCLC 809249108.
Philosophy of psychiatry
[edit]- Verhoeff, Berend (2013-11-08). "The autism puzzle: challenging a mechanistic model on conceptual and historical grounds". Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine : PEHM. 8: 17. doi:10.1186/1747-5341-8-17. ISSN 1747-5341. PMC 4226210. PMID 24207065.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)- Berend Verhoeff is a Psychiatrist and Philosopher of Science from the Netherlands
- See also his 2015 PhD dissertation "Autism's anatomy: A dissection of the structure and development of a psychiatric concept":
- Verhoeff, Berend, 1977-. Autism's anatomy : a dissection of the structure and development of a psychiatric concept. ISBN 978-90-367-8159-6. OCLC 925462892.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link) - Verhoeff, Berend (2015). Autism's anatomy: A dissection of the structure and development of a psychiatric concept (Thesis). University of Groningen.
- Verhoeff, Berend, 1977-. Autism's anatomy : a dissection of the structure and development of a psychiatric concept. ISBN 978-90-367-8159-6. OCLC 925462892.
Fractionablity of the autism triad/dyad
[edit]- Happé, Francesca; Ronald, Angelica (December 2008). "The 'Fractionable Autism Triad': A Review of Evidence from Behavioural, Genetic, Cognitive and Neural Research" (PDF). Neuropsychology Review. 18 (4): 287–304. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9076-8. ISSN 1040-7308.
- "The suggestion that the different aspects of the ASD triad have fractionable causes, at the genetic, neurological, and cognitive levels, is sometimes taken as an attack on the validity of the diagnosis of autism (see Mandy and Skuse 2008 for discussion). However, it is quite compatible to assert that ASD results when a number of independent impairments co-occur, and to assert that the resulting mix has a special quality, distinct prognosis and response to intervention, and is therefore worthy of a distinct diagnostic label."
- Mandy, William P.L.; Skuse, David H. (August 2008). "Research Review: What is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities?". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 49 (8): 795–808. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01911.x.
- "Secondly, although the current literature suggests only a moderate correlation between social-commun- ication and nonsocial behaviours, this is not the same as there being no association at all, and we should not throw the baby out with the bath water. Ronald and colleagues suggest that this correlation (r) lies somewhere in the region of .3. Leaving aside methodological concerns, it is the case that a correlation in psychology of this magnitude is often taken to be meaningful. For example, correlations between goal conflicts and ill health (Emmons & King, 1988), trait positive affectivity and job satisfaction (Agho, Mueller, & Price, cited in Warr, 1996) and use of primitive defence mechanisms and anti-social per- sonality traits (Chabrol & Leichsenring, 2006) have all been reported to be in this range. That RIBAs are not universal in people with social communication disorders should not obscure the fact that these two sets of behaviours are moderately, and perhaps meaningfully, related."
- (Question: how does the correlation between autism domains compare to the correlation between autism and frequently co-occurring non-autism symptoms, e.g. ADHD symptoms?)
- Whitten, Allison; Unruh, Kathryn E.; Shafer, Robin L.; Bodfish, James W. (June 2018). "Subgrouping Autism Based on Symptom Severity Leads to Differences in the Degree of Convergence Between Core Feature Domains". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 48 (6): 1908–1919. doi:10.1007/s10803-017-3451-3. ISSN 0162-3257. PMID 29307039.
- "Existing models of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) disagree as to whether the core features should be conceptualized as convergent (related) or divergent (unrelated), and the few previous studies addressing this question have found conflicting results. We examined standardized parent ratings of symptoms from three domains (social, communication, repetitive behaviors) in large samples of typically developing children, children with ASD, and ASD subgroups. Our results suggest that the most evidence for divergence lies in typically developing children and lower severity ASD cases, while more evidence for convergence is found in a subset of cases with more severe impairment on any core feature. These results highlight the importance of subgrouping ASD given the degree of phenotypic heterogeneity present across the autism spectrum."
Overdiagnosis
[edit]- Frances, Allen, 1942- (2013). Saving normal : an insider's revolt against out-of-control psychiatric diagnosis, DSM-5, big pharma, and the medicalization of ordinary life (1st ed ed.). New York: William Morrow. ISBN 978-0-06-222927-4. OCLC 843126734.
{{cite book}}
:|edition=
has extra text (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)- "...despite our best efforts to tame excessive diagnostic exuberance, DSM-IV had since been misused to blow up the diagnostic bublle. Even though we had been boringly modest in our goals, obsessively meticulous in our methods, and rigidly conservative in our product, we failed to predict or prevent three new false epidemics of mental disorder in children–autism, attention deficit, and childhood bipolar disorder."
- Kirkey, Sharon (2019-08-22). "Is the 'autism epidemic' real? Researchers say over-diagnosis to blame for spike in number of cases". National Post. Retrieved 2020-02-16.
{{cite news}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link)- Allen Frances quotes: "Probably the biggest mistake we made in DSM-IV was including Asperger’s, a much milder form of autistic disorder with unclear boundaries to normal diversity, eccentricity and giftedness... Careless diagnosis, often related to requirements for extra school services, resulted in a fake epidemic — a 50-fold increase in the past 25 years... "
- About this journal article: Rødgaard, Eya-Mist; Jensen, Kristian; Vergnes, Jean-Noël; Soulières, Isabelle; Mottron, Laurent (2019-11-01). "Temporal Changes in Effect Sizes of Studies Comparing Individuals With and Without Autism: A Meta-analysis". JAMA Psychiatry. 76 (11): 1124. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1956. ISSN 2168-622X. PMC 6704749. PMID 31433441.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - Same journal article also discussed by "Is it autism? The line is getting increasingly blurry". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 2020-02-16.
- Overdiagnosis and diagnosis of increasingly mild cases may result in a reduction of focus on those with the most severe symptoms, to the later's detriment – "Out of Sight, Out of Mind: The Erasure of Severe Autism — NCSA". National Council on Severe Autism. Retrieved 2020-02-16.
- Parry, Peter. "The difficulties doctors face in diagnosing autism". The Conversation. Retrieved 2020-02-20.
- "In my experience as a child and adolescent psychiatrist, it is apparent there is an over-diagnosis problem. In addition, there are still children, particularly girls, whose ASD diagnosis is tragically missed until late in their development and schooling. Many child psychiatrist and paediatrician colleagues offer similar observations."
- Basu, Soumya; Parry, Peter (2013-12). "The autism spectrum disorder 'epidemic': Need for biopsychosocial formulation". Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 47 (12): 1116–1118. doi:10.1177/0004867413509694. ISSN 0004-8674.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help)
Role of social networks in diagnosis
[edit]- Liu, Kayuet; Bearman, Peter S. (May 2015). "Focal Points, Endogenous Processes, and Exogenous Shocks in the Autism Epidemic". Sociological Methods & Research. 44 (2): 272–305. doi:10.1177/0049124112460369. ISSN 0049-1241.
- Liu, Ka‐Yuet; King, Marissa; Bearman, Peter S. (2010-03-01). "Social Influence and the Autism Epidemic". American Journal of Sociology. 115 (5): 1387–1434. doi:10.1086/651448. ISSN 0002-9602.
{{cite journal}}
: no-break space character in|first3=
at position 6 (help)
Boundary with other conditions
[edit]Some argue that ASD as a diagnosis has unclear and variable boundaries with other conditions which have overlapping symptoms – ADHD, OCD, Tourette's, schizophrenia, IDD
with ADHD
[edit]- Ashinoff, Brandon K.; Abu-Akel, Ahmad (2019-09-20). "Hyperfocus: the forgotten frontier of attention". Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-019-01245-8. ISSN 1430-2772.
- Table 1 is particularly interesting – "hyperfocus" is recognised as a symptom of ADHD, ASD, and schizophrenia, and yet (contradicting that) some authors present it as a differential symptom which is present in ASD but not in ADHD
- Grzadzinski, Rebecca; Dick, Catherine; Lord, Catherine; Bishop, Somer (2016-01-19). "Parent-reported and clinician-observed autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptoms in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): implications for practice under DSM-5". Molecular Autism. 7 (1): 7. doi:10.1186/s13229-016-0072-1. ISSN 2040-2392. PMC 4717584. PMID 26788284.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)- "Of the ADHD sample, 21 % met ASD cut-offs on the ADOS and 30 % met ASD cut-offs on all domains of the ADI-R". (Note the ADHD sample is children diagnosed with ADHD but for whom ASD has been clinically excluded.)
- Kushki, Azadeh; Anagnostou, Evdokia; Hammill, Christopher; Duez, Pierre; Brian, Jessica; Iaboni, Alana; Schachar, Russell; Crosbie, Jennifer; Arnold, Paul; Lerch, Jason P. (December 2019). "Examining overlap and homogeneity in ASD, ADHD, and OCD: a data-driven, diagnosis-agnostic approach". Translational Psychiatry. 9 (1): 318. doi:10.1038/s41398-019-0631-2. ISSN 2158-3188. PMC 6880188. PMID 31772171.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)- "The validity of diagnostic labels of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) is an open question given the mounting evidence that these categories may not correspond to conditions with distinct etiologies, biologies, or phenotypes"
- "Our results did not support the validity of existing diagnostic labels of ASD, ADHD, and OCD as distinct entities with respect to phenotype and cortical morphology"
Accuracy of diagnostic instruments
[edit]- Evers, Kris; Maljaars, Jarymke; Carrington, Sarah J.; Carter, Alice S.; Happé, Francesca; Steyaert, Jean; Leekam, Susan R.; Noens, Ilse (2020-02-19). "How well are DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for ASD represented in standardized diagnostic instruments?". European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. doi:10.1007/s00787-020-01481-z. ISSN 1018-8827.
- Examines how well diagnostic instruments (ADOS-2, DISCO-11, 3di) align with DSM-5 criteria
- "Our analyses showed that the three instruments do not cover all ASD symptoms to the same extent and that their diagnostic classification procedures are not always in line with the DSM-5 ASD criteria. Furthermore, the interpretation of the DSM-5 behavioral A (‘Deficits in social communication and interac- tions’) and B (‘Restricted and repetitive behavior, interests, and activities’) criteria is sometimes ambiguous and the other criteria (C—‘Early onset’, D—‘Significant impact on daily life functioning’, and E—‘Not better explained by other developmental diagnosis’) are not clearly defined."
ADOS/-2
[edit]- Kamp-Becker, I.; Albertowski, K.; Becker, J.; Ghahreman, M.; Langmann, A.; Mingebach, T.; Poustka, L.; Weber, L.; Schmidt, H.; Smidt, J.; Stehr, T. (September 2018). "Diagnostic accuracy of the ADOS and ADOS-2 in clinical practice". European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 27 (9): 1193–1207. doi:10.1007/s00787-018-1143-y. ISSN 1018-8827.
- "High variance was found in the codings. The accuracy of the coding depends on the experience of the coder with the ADOS as well as on characteristics of the cases and the quality of the administration of the ADOS"
- "There is an obvious symptom overlap between ASD and emotional and anxiety disorders documented by several studies [24, 52–56]. Both disorders involve profound social interaction and communication deficits, problems in emotion recognition, insistence on sameness, inflexible adherence to routines, or ritualized patterns or verbal/non-verbal behaviour. 30% of children with an anxiety disorder (but no known diagnosis of ASD) were above the cutoff of the ADI-R in at least one domain [57]. Other disorders are likewise associated with “autistic traits” of profound amount, such as ADHD, conduct disorders, intellectual impairment, and language disorders (see above). The degree of overlap between the above-mentioned disorders and ASD may result in misinterpretation of symptoms and in high scores in the ADOS/-2, respectively. In clinical samples, the specificity of the ADOS/-2 is lower than in research settings [32, 46, 58, 59], with high levels of false-positive diagnoses. In our study, the clinicians made many more false-positive than false-negative ADOS diagnoses."
- "...examiners and entire clinical teams tend to ‘drift’ with their ADOS codings. Thus, it is important to be involved in calibration with different teams and organisations"
- Randall, Melinda; Egberts, Kristine J; Samtani, Aarti; Scholten, Rob JPM; Hooft, Lotty; Livingstone, Nuala; Sterling-Levis, Katy; Woolfenden, Susan; Williams, Katrina (2018-07-24). Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group (ed.). "Diagnostic tests for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in preschool children". Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD009044.pub2. PMC 6513463. PMID 30075057.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
- A 2018 Cochrane systematic review included 12 studies of ADOS diagnostic accuracy in pre-school children (Modules 1 and 2). The summary sensitivity was 0.94 (95% CI 0.89 to 0.97), with sensitivity in individual studies ranging from 0.76 to 0.98. The summary specificity was 0.80 (95% CI 0.68 to 0.88), with specificity in individual studies ranging from 0.20 to 1.00. The studies were evaluated for bias using the QUADAS-2 framework; of the 12 included studies, 8 were evaluated as having a high risk of bias, while for the remaining four there was insufficient information available for the risk of bias to be properly evaluated. The authors could not identify any studies for the ADOS-2; the scope of the review was limited to preschool age children (mean age under 6 years), which excluded studies of Modules 3 and 4 from the meta-analysis. One included study examined the additive sensitivity and specificity of the ADOS used in combination with the ADI-R; that study found an 11% improvement in specificity (compared to ADOS alone) at the cost of a 14% reduction in sensitivity; however, due to overlapping confidence intervals, that result could not be considered statistically significant.
- T. Hutchins, H. Morris and S. Habermehl, Diagnostic Accuracy of the ADOS-2, Poster Presentation 29391, INSAR 2019 Annual Meeting.
- "Overall accuracy across modules was 70.4% (sensitivity = 90.9%; specificity = 66.0%) with a high rate of false positives (27.9%). Overall accuracy tended to decrease as module number increased (module 1 = 90.9%; module 2 = 93.9%; module 3 = 62.5%; module 4 = 58.8%). The most common non-spectrum diagnosis for children classified as ASD by the ADOS-2 for modules 2 – 4 (approximately 88%) was ADHD and anxiety. "
Theory of mind
[edit]- Gernsbacher, Morton Ann; Yergeau, Melanie (2019-12-09). "Empirical failures of the claim that autistic people lack a theory of mind". Archives of Scientific Psychology. 7 (1): 102–118. doi:10.1037/arc0000067. ISSN 2169-3269. PMC 6959478. PMID 31938672.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
Cognitive rigidity
[edit]While cognitive rigidity (low levels of cognitive flexibility) is often associated with ASD (as a symptom in the RRB domain), it is not a unique symptom to ASD and also occurs in other disorders and "typical" individuals:
- Wilson, Cristina G.; Nusbaum, Amy T.; Whitney, Paul; Hinson, John M. (2018-09-27). "Trait anxiety impairs cognitive flexibility when overcoming a task acquired response and a preexisting bias". PLoS ONE. 13 (9). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0204694. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 6160151. PMID 30261023.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - Lima, Isabela M.M.; Peckham, Andrew D.; Johnson, Sheri L. (February 2018). "Cognitive deficits in bipolar disorders: Implications for emotion". Clinical psychology review. 59: 126–136. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2017.11.006. ISSN 0272-7358. PMC 6404979. PMID 29195773.
- Waltz, James A. (2017-03-14). "The Neural Underpinnings of Cognitive Flexibility and their Disruption in Psychotic Illness". Neuroscience. 345: 203–217. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.06.005. ISSN 0306-4522. PMC 5143214. PMID 27282085.
Social motivation
[edit]- Jaswal, Vikram K.; Akhtar, Nameera (2019). "Being versus appearing socially uninterested: Challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 42: e82. doi:10.1017/S0140525X18001826. ISSN 0140-525X.
- Levy, Yonata (2019). ""Autistic people"? Who do you mean?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 42: e97. doi:10.1017/S0140525X18002261. ISSN 0140-525X.
Miscellaneous
[edit]- Marks, Deb. "Autism Spectrum Disorders: Aka Pervasive Developmental Disorders".
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - Hodge, Nick (2005-05-01). "Reflections on diagnosing autism spectrum disorders". Disability & Society. 20 (3): 345–349. doi:10.1080/09687590500060810. ISSN 0968-7599.
- "Recently, however, I have begun to question my total allegiance to the diktat that a diagnosis of ASD is helpful"
- "I am currently undertaking a doctoral study into the impact on parents when, unexpectedly, their child is given a diagnosis of ASD. Mostly, the parents have been looking for practical support in developing their child’s language skills and have not anticipated ASD. In three cases I have been researching in depth, the diagnosis of ASD has had a largely devastating impact on the parents, disempowering them by causing them to question their ability to interact with and provide for their child without specialist training, even though they were doing an excellent job up to the time of diagnosis. The term ‘Autistic Spectrum Disorder’ causes them to rethink completely their child’s potential future, making them fearful of what lies ahead. Through talking with others and through their reading, the parents become unsure of how their child will be in the future; one family who had a very cooperative young son began to become highly anxious that he would, in the future, develop severely non-cooperative behaviours like children they read about in textbooks. I have seen that the label becomes more significant than the nature of the child"
- "parents report that they feel ‘blackmailed’ into diagnosis"
- "I became increasingly uncomfortable with the extent to which giving diagnostic labels reinforces a system of provision that is about recycling the potency of professionals and the impotency of parents in the lives of vulnerable children"
- Hodge, Nicholas Stuart (2006-01-31). Disabling families: How parents experience the process of diagnosing autism spectrum disorders (doctoral thesis). Sheffield Hallam University.
- Hodge, Nick; Runswick‐Cole, Katherine (2008-10-01). "Problematising parent–professional partnerships in education". Disability & Society. 23 (6): 637–647. doi:10.1080/09687590802328543. ISSN 0968-7599.
- Runswick-Cole, Katherine (2007). Parents as advocates : the experience of parents who register an appeal with the Special Educational Needs and Disability Tribunal (SENDisT) (phd thesis). University of Sheffield.