Jump to content

Multisensory integration

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 208.92.44.113 (talk) at 20:24, 13 December 2010 (The double-flash illusion). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Multimodal integration, known as 'multisensory integration' is the study of how information from the different sensory modalities, such as sight, sound, touch, smell, self-motion and taste, may be integrated by the nervous system. A coherent representation of objects combining modalities enables us to have meaningful perceptual experiences. Indeed, multisensory integration is central to adaptive behavior because it allows us to perceive a world of coherent perceptual entities.[1] Multimodal integration also deals with how different sensory modalities interact with one another and alter each other’s processing.

Sensory modalities

Traditionally (since Aristotle?), humans are thought to have five separate senses: vision, hearing, touch, smell, and taste. The actual number of sensory modalities that an organism has is hard to define (e.g., Grice, 1962). Sensations of self-motion and balance for example are often referred to as being sensed by the inner ear which is misleading giving one the impression that they are related to hearing as opposed to sensations detected by the vestibular system. Further, is 'pain' a separate sense, or should it be considered a part of 'touch'? To some extent, the study of multisensory integration brings into question the idea that we have separate senses at all.

Sensory submodalities

Each sensory modality can be further sub-divided into submodalities. In vision, the processing of colour, shape, orientation, motion, and depth may occur somewhat separately, and form somewhat independent submodalities of vision. In touch (or 'somatosensation'), one can distinguish sub-modalities of vibration, light pressure, deep pressure, temperature, pain, muscle stretch, and affective touch.

The binding problem

In the visual domain, if color, motion, depth, and form, are processed independently, where does the unified coherent conscious experience of the visual world come in? This is known as the binding problem and is usually studied entirely within visual processes, however it is clear that the binding problem is central to multisensory perception.

However, considerations of how unified conscious representations are formed are not the full focus of multimodal Integration research. It is obviously important for the senses to interact in order to maximize how efficiently people interact with the environment. For perceptual experience and behavior to benefit from the simultaneous stimulation of multiple sensory modalities, integration of the information from these modalities is necessary. Some of the mechanisms mediating this phenomenon and its subsequent effects on cognitive and behavioural processes will be examined hereafter. Perception is often defined as ones conscious experience, and thereby combines inputs from all relevant senses and prior knowledge. Perception is also defined and studied in terms of feature extraction, which is several hundred milliseconds away from conscious experience. Notwithstanding the existence of Gestalt psychology schools that advocate a holistic approach to the operation of the brain, the physiological processes underlying the formation of percepts and conscious experience have been vastly understudied. Nevertheless, burgeoning neuroscience research continues to enrich our understanding of the many details of the brain, including neural structures implicated in multisensory integration such as the superior colliculus (SC) and various cortical structures such as the superior temporal gyrus (GT) and visual and auditory association areas. Although the structure and function of the SC are well known, the cortex and the relationship between its constituent parts are presently the subject of much investigation. Concurrently, the recent impetus on integration has enabled investigation into perceptual phenomena such as the ventriloquism effect, rapid localization of stimuli and the McGurk effect; culminating in a more thorough understanding of the human brain and its functions.

History

Studies of sensory processing in humans and other animals has traditionally been performed one sense at a time, and to the present day, numerous academic societies and journals are largely restricted to considering sensory modalities separately ('Vision Research', 'Hearing Research' etc.). However, while there is also a long and parallel history of multisensory research (e.g., as early as Stratton's (1897) experiments on the somatosensory effects of wearing vision-distorting prism glasses), this research field has recently gained enormously in interest and popularity.

Modularity of mind

Most psychologists interested in perceptual processes typically investigate each sense modality independently and these traditional unimodal approaches labour under a number of assumptions. Jerry Fodor (1983) popularized the notion that sense modalities are processed independently in his monograph Modularity of Mind. Fodor's modular theory of perception states that perceptual processes are simple reflex-like computations that create perceptual inferences about the environment. Importantly these computations are modular, separated into parallel processing channels that are not affected by higher level cognitive processes which Fodor labeled 'central processes'. In essence Fodor highlighted the functional independence of each of the senses and asserted that perception followed feedforward information processing scheme only.

Theories and approaches

Visual dominance

Modality appropriateness

Welch and Warren (1980) asserted that multisensory processes followed a modality appropriateness hypothesis, in which due to visual dominance of spatial tasks, also known as visual capture, one will always depend on vision over audition or tactition to solve spatial problems. Thus, auditory stimuli can not at all influence ones perception of the location of a visual stimulus. Concurrently, audition was considered dominant toward temporal tasks.

However, more recent studies have generated results that contradict this hypothesis. Alais and Burr (2004), found that following progressive degradation in the quality of a visual stimulus, participants’ perception of spatial location was determined progressively more by a simultaneous auditory cue. reached a similar finding. However, they also progressively changed the temporal uncertainty of the auditory cue; eventually concluding that it is the uncertainty of individual modalities that determine to what extent information from each modality is considered when forming a percept. This conclusion is similar in some respects to the ‘inverse effectiveness rule’. The extent to which multisensory integration occurs may vary according to the ambiguity of the relevant stimuli.

Optimal integration

Maximum likelihood estimation

Bayesian integration

The theory of bayseian integragtion is based on the fact that the brain must deal with a number of inputs, which vary in reliability[2]. In dealing with these inputs, it must construct a coherent representation of the world that corresponds to reality. The Bayseian integration view is that the brain uses a form of Bayesian inference [3]. This view has been backed up by computational modeling of such a Beysian inference from signals to coherent representation, which shows similar characteristics to integration in the brain.[3]

Principles of multisensory integration

The contributions of Barry Stein, Alex Meredith, and their colleagues (e.g., Stein & Meredith 1993) are widely considered to be the groundbreaking work in the modern field of multisensory integration. Through detailed long-term study of the neurophysiology of the superior colliculus, they distilled three general principles by which multisensory integration may best be described.

The spatial rule

The spatial rule (Meredith & Stein 1986, see also King & Palmer, 1985) states that multisensory integration is more likely or stronger when the constituent unisensory stimuli arise from approximately the same location.

The temporal rule

The temporal rule (Meredith et al., 1987, see also King & Palmer, 1985) states that multisensory integration is more likely or stronger when the constituent unisensory stimuli arise at approximately the same time.

The principle of inverse effectiveness

The principle of inverse effectiveness (Meredith & Stein, 1983) states that multisensory integration is more likely or stronger when the constituent unisensory stimuli evoke relatively weak responses when presented in isolation.

Perceptual and behavioral consequences

A unimodal approach dominated scientific literature until the beginning of this century. Although this enabled rapid progression of neural mapping, and an improved understanding of neural structures, the investigation of perception remained relatively stagnant. The recent revitalized enthusiasm into perceptual research is indicative of a substantial shift away from reductionism and toward gestalt methodologies. Gestalt theory, dominant in the late 19th and early 20th centuries espoused two general principles: the ‘principle of totality’ in which conscious experience must be considered globally, and the ‘principle of psychophysical isomorphism’ which states that perceptual phenomena are correlated with cerebral activity. These ideas are particularly relevant in the current climate and have driven researchers to investigate the behavioural benefits of multisensory integration.

Decreasing sensory uncertainty

It has been widely acknowledged that uncertainty in sensory domains results in an increased dependence of multisensory integration (Alais & Burr, 2004). Hence, it follows that cues from multiple modalities that are both temporally and spatially synchronous are viewed neurally and perceptually as emanating from the same source. The degree of synchrony that is required for this ‘binding’ to occur is currently being investigated in a variety of approaches. It should be noted here that the integrative function only occurs to a point beyond which the subject can differentiate them as two opposing stimuli. Concurrently, a significant intermediate conclusion can be drawn from the research thus far. Multisensory stimuli that are bound into a single percept, are also bound on the same receptive fields of multisensory neurons in the SC and cortex (Alais & Burr, 2004). Considering the lack of empirically based evidence for the psychophysical isomorphism principle during the period of dominance of the gestalt school, this highly nontrivial statement validates the and further emphasizes the increasing role of Gestalt psychology in modern science.

Increasing stimulus detection

Improving stimulus identification

Decreasing reaction time

Responses to multiple simultaneous sensory stimuli can be faster than responses to the same stimuli presented in isolation. Hershenson (1962) presented a light and tone simultaneously and separately, and asked human participants to respond as rapidly as possible to them. As the asynchrony between the onsets of both stimuli was varied, it was observed that for certain degrees of asynchrony, reaction times were decreased. These levels of asynchrony were quite small, perhaps reflecting the temporal window that exists in multimodal neurons of the SC. Further studies have analysed the reaction times of saccadic eye movements (Hughs et al., 1994); and more recently correlated these findings to neural phenomena (Wallace, 2004).

Redundant target effects

Multisensory illusions

The McGurk effect

It has been found that two converging bimodal stimuli can produce a perception that is not only different in magnitude than the sum of its parts, but also quite different in quality. In a classic study labeled the McGurk effect, (McGurk & MacDonald, 1976) a person’s phoneme production was dubbed with a video of that person speaking a different phoneme. The end result was the perception of a third, different phoneme. McGurk and MacDonald (1976) explained that phonemes such as ba, da, ka, ta, ga and pa can be divided into four groups, those that can be visually confused, i.e. (da, ga, ka, ta) and (ba and pa), and those that can be audibly confused. Hence, when ba – voice and ga lips are processed together, the visual modality sees ga or da, and the auditory modality hears ba or da, combining to form the percept da.

Ventriloquism

Ventriloquism has been used as the evidence for the modality appropriateness hypothesis. Ventriloquism describes the situation in which auditory location perception is shifted toward a visual cue. The original study describing this phenomenon was conducted by Howard and Templeton, (1966) after which several studies have replicated and built upon the conclusions they reached (Hairston et al., 2003;). In conditions in which the visual cue is unambiguous, visual capture reliably occurs. Thus to test the influence of sound on perceived location, the visual stimulus must be progressively degraded (Alais & Burr, 2004). Furthermore, given that auditory stimuli are more attuned to temporal changes, recent studies have tested the ability of temporal characteristics to influence the spatial location of visual stimuli. Some types of EVP - Electronic voice phenomenon, mainly the ones using sound bubles are considered a kind of modern ventriloquism technique and is played by the use of sophisticated software, computers and sound equipment.

The double-flash illusion

The double flash illusion was reported as the first illusion to show that visual stimuli can be qualitatively altered by the audio stimuli[4]. In the standard paradigm participants are presented combinations of one or more (up to four) flashes accompanied with zero or more (up to 4 beeps). They were then asked to say how many flashes they perceived. Paricipants perceived illusiory flashes when there where more flashes than beeps. fMRI studies have shown that there is crossmodal activation in early, low level visual areas, which was qualitativy similar to the perception of a real flash which suggests that the illusion reflects subjective perception of the extra flash[5]. Further, studies suggest that timing of multimodal activation in unisensory cortexes is too fast to be mediated by a higher order integration suggesting feed forward or lateral connections[6]. One study has revealed the same effect but from vision to audition, as well as fission rather than fusion effects, although the level of the auditory stimulus was reduced to make it less salient for those illusions affecting audition.[7]

The rubber hand illusion (RHI)

In the rubber hand illusion (RHI, Botvinick & Cohen, 1998), human participants view a dummy hand being stroked with a paintbrush, while they feel a series of identical brushstrokes applied to their own hand, which is hidden from view. If this visual and tactile information is applied synchronously, and if the visual appearance and position of the dummy hand is similar to one's own hand, then people may feel that the touches on their own hand are coming from the dummy hand, and even that the dummy hand is, in some way, their own hand. This is an early form of body transfer illusion. The RHI is an illusion of vision, touch, and posture (proprioception), but a similar illusion can also be induced with touch and proprioception (Ehrsson, Holmes, & Passingham, 2005). The very first report of this kind of illusion may have been as early as 1937 (Tastevin, 1937).

Body transfer illusion

Body transfer illusion involves the use of typically, virtual reality devices to induce the illusion in the subject that the body of another person or being is the subject's own body.

Neural mechanisms

Subcortical areas

Superior colliculus (SC) or Optic tectum (OT)

The superior colliculus (SC) is part of the tectum, located in the midbrain, superior to the brainstem and inferior to the thalamus. It contains seven layers of alternating white and grey matter, of which the superficial contain topographic maps of the visual field; and deeper layers contain overlapping spatial maps of the visual, auditory and somatosensory modalities (Affifi & Bergman, 2005). The structure receives afferents directly from the retina, as well as from various regions of the cortex (primarily the occipital lobe), the spinal cord and the inferior colliculus. It sends efferents to the spinal cord, cerebellum, thalamus and occipital lobe via the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). The structure contains a high proportion of multisensory neurons and plays a role in the motor control of orientation behaviours of the eyes, ears and head.[8]

Receptive fields from somatosensory, visual and auditory modalities converge in the deeper layers to form a two-dimensional multisensory map of the external world. Here, objects straight ahead are represented caudally and objects on the periphery are represented rosterally. Similarly, locations in superior sensory space are represented medially, and inferior locations are represented laterally (Stein & Meredith 1993).

However, in contrast to simple convergence, the SC integrates information to create an output that differs from the sum of its inputs. Following a phenomenon labelled the ‘spatial rule’, neurons are excited if stimuli from multiple modalities fall on the same or adjacent receptive fields, but are inhibited if the stimuli fall on disparate fields (Giard & Peronnet, 1999). Excited neurons may then proceed to innervate various muscles and neural structures to orient an individual’s behaviour and attention toward the stimulus. Neurons in the SC also adhere to the ‘temporal rule’, in which stimulation must occur within close temporal proximity to excite neurons. However, due to the varying processing time between modalities and the relatively slower speed of sound to light, it has been found the neurons may be optimally excited when stimulated some time apart (Miller & D’Esposito, 2005).

Putamen

Single neurons in the macaque putamen have been shown to have visual and somatosensory responses (Graziano & Gross, 1994/1995) closely related to those in the polysensory zone of the premotor cortex and area 7b in the parietal lobe.

Cortical areas

Multisensory neurons exist in a large number of locations, often integrated with unimodal neurons. They have recently been discovered in areas previously thought to be modality specific, such as the somatosensory cortex; as well as in clusters at the borders between the major cerebral lobes, such as the occipito-parietal space and the occipito-temporal space (Wallace, Ramachandran & Stein, 2004; Wallace, 2004).

However, in order to undergo such physiological changes, there must exist continuous connectivity between these multisensory structures. It is generally agreed that information flow within the cortex follows a hierarchical configuration (Clavagnier, Falchier & Kennedy, 2004). Hubel and Wiesel (as cited in Clavagnier et al., 2004) showed that receptive fields and thus the function of cortical structures, as one proceeds out from V1 along the visual pathways, become increasingly complex and specialized. From this it was postulated that information flowed outwards in a feed forward fashion; the complex end products eventually binding to form a percept. However, via fMRI and intracranial recording technologies, it has been observed that the activation time of successive levels of the hierarchy does not correlate with a feed forward structure. That is, late activation has been observed in the striate cortex, markedly after activation of the prefrontal cortex in response to the same stimulus (Foxe & Simpson, 2002).

Complementing this, afferent nerve fibres have been found that project to early visual areas such as the lingual gyrus from late in the dorsal (action) and ventral (perception) visual streams, as well as from the auditory association cortex (Macaluso, Frith & Driver, 2000). Feedback projections have also been observed in the opossum directly from the auditory association cortex to V1 (Clavagnier et al., 2004). This last observation currently highlights a point of controversy within the neuroscientific community. Sadato et al. (2004) concluded, in line with Bernstein et al. (2002), that the primary auditory cortex (A1) was functionally distinct from the auditory association cortex, in that it was void of any interaction with the visual modality. They hence concluded that A1 would not at all be effected by cross modal plasticity. This concurs with Jones and Powell’s (1970) contention that primary sensory areas are connected only to other areas of the same modality.

In contrast, the dorsal auditory pathway, projecting from the temporal lobe is largely concerned with processing spatial information, and contains receptive fields that are topographically organized. Fibers from this region project directly to neurons governing corresponding receptive fields in V1 (Clavagnier et al., 2004). The perceptual consequences of this have not yet been empirically acknowledged. However, it can be hypothesized that these projections may be the precursors of increased acuity and emphasis of visual stimuli in relevant areas of perceptual space. Consequently, this finding rejects Jones and Powell’s (1970) hypothesis and thus is in conflict with Sadato et al.’s (2004) findings. A resolution to this discrepancy includes the possibility that primary sensory areas can not be classified as a single group, and thus may be far more different from what was previously thought. Regardless, further research is necessary for a definitive resolution.

Frontal lobe

Area F4 in macaques

Area F5 in macaques

Polysensory zone of premotor cortex (PZ) in macaques

Occipital lobe

Primary visual cortex (V1)

Lingual gyrus in humans

Lateral occipital complex (LOC), including lateral occipital tactile visual area (LOtv)

Parietal lobe

Ventral intraparietal sulcus (VIP) in macaques

Lateral intraparietal sulcus (LIP) in macaques

Area 7b in macaques

Second somatosensory cortex (SII)

Temporal lobe

Primary auditory cortex (A1)

Superior temporal cortex (STG/STS/PT) Audio visual cross modal interactions are known to occur in the auditory association cortex which lies directly inferior to the Sylvian fissure in the temporal lobe (Sadato et al., 2004). Plasticity was observed in the superior temporal gyrus (STG) by Petitto et al. (2000). Here, it was found that the STG was more active during stimulation in native deaf signers compared to hearing non signers. Concurrently, further research has revealed differences in the activation of the Planum temporale (PT) in response to non linguistic lip movements between the hearing and deaf; as well as progressively increasing activation of the auditory association cortex as previously deaf participants gain hearing experience via a cochlear implant (Sadato et al., 2004).

Anterior ectosylvian sulus (AES) in cats

Rostral lateral suprasylvian sulcus (rLS) in cats

Cortical-subcortical interactions

The most significant interaction between these two systems (corticotectal interactions) is the connection between the anterior ectosylvian sulcus (AES), which lies at the junction of the parietal, temporal and frontal lobes, and the SC. The AES is divided into three unimodal regions with multimodal neurons at the junctions between these sections (Jiang & Stein, 2003). Neurons from the unimodal regions project to the deep layers of the SC and influence the multiplicative integration effect. That is, although they can receive inputs from all modalities as normal, the SC can not enhance or depress the effect of multimodal stimulation without input from the AES (Jiang & Stein, 2003).

Concurrently, the multisensory neurons of the AES, although also integrally connected to unimodal AES neurons, are not directly connected to the SC. This pattern of division is reflected in other areas of the cortex, resulting in the observation that cortical and tectal multisensory systems are somewhat dissociated (Wallace, Meredith & Stein, 1993). Stein, London, Wilkinson and Price (1996) analysed the perceived luminance of an LED in the context of spatially disparate auditory distracters of various types. A significant finding was that a sound increased the perceived brightness of the light, regardless of their relative spatial locations, provided the light’s image was projected onto the fovea. Here, the apparent lack of the spatial rule, further differentiates cortical and tectal multisensory neurons. Little empirical evidence exists to justify this dichotomy. Nevertheless, cortical neurons governing perception, and a separate sub cortical system governing action (orientation behavior) is synonymous with the perception action hypothesis of the visual stream (Goodale & Milner, 1995). Further investigation into this field is necessary before any substantial claims can be made.

Dual "what" and "where" multisensory routes

Research suggests the existence of two multisensory routes for "what" and "where". The "what" route identifying the identity of things involving area Brodmann area 9 in the right inferior frontal gyrus and right middle frontal gyrus, Brodmann area 13 and Brodmann area 45 in the right insula-inferior frontal gyrus area, and Brodmann area 13 bilaterally in the insula. The "where" route detecting their spatial attributes involving the Brodmann area 40 in the right and left inferior parietal lobule and the Brodmann area 7 in the right precuneus-superior parietal lobule and Brodmann area 7 in the left superior parietal lobule.[9]

Development of multimodal operations

All species equipped with multiple sensory systems, utilize them in an integrative manner to achieve action and perception (Stein & Meredith 1993). However, in most species, especially higher mammals, the ability to integrate develops in parallel with physical and cognitive maturity. Classically, two opposing views that are principally modern manifestations of the nativist/empiricist dichotomy have been put forth. The integration (empiricist) view states that at birth, sensory modalities are not at all connected. Hence, it is only through active exploration that plastic changes can occur in the nervous system to initiate holistic perceptions and actions. Conversely, the differentiation (nativist) perspective asserts that the young nervous system is highly interconnected; and that during development, modalities are gradually differentiated as relevant connections are rehearsed and the irrelevant are discarded (Lewkowicz & Kraebel, 2004).

Using the SC as a model, the nature of this dichotomy can be analysed. In the newborn cat, deep layers of the SC contain only neurons responding to the somatosensory modality. Within a week, auditory neurons begin to occur, but it is not until two weeks after birth that the first multimodal neurons appear. Further changes continue, with the arrival of visual neurons after three weeks, until the SC has achieved its fully mature structure after three to four months. Concurrently in species of monkey, newborns are endowed with a significant complement of multisensory cells; however, along with cats there is no integration effect apparent until much later (Wallace, 2004). This delay is thought to be the result of the relatively slower development of cortical structures including the AES; which as stated above, is essential for the existence of the integration effect (Jiang & Stein, 2003).

Furthermore, it was found by Wallace (2004) that cats raised in a light deprived environment had severely underdeveloped visual receptive fields in deep layers of the SC. Although, receptive field size has been shown to decrease with maturity, the above finding suggests that integration in the SC is a function of experience. Nevertheless, the existence of visual multimodal neurons, despite a complete lack of visual experience, highlights the apparent relevance of nativist viewpoints. Multimodal development in the cortex has been studied to a lesser extent, however a similar study to that presented above was performed on cats whose optic nerves had been severed. These cats displayed a marked improvement in their ability to localize stimuli through audition; and consequently also showed increased neural connectivity between V1 and the auditory cortex (Clavagnier et al., 2004). Such plasticity in early childhood allows for greater adaptability, and thus more normal development in other areas for those with a sensory deficit.

In contrast, following the initial formative period, the SC does not appear to display any neural plasticity. Despite this, habituation and sensititisation over the long term is known to exist in orientation behaviors. This apparent plasticity in function has been attributed to the adaptability of the AES. That is, although neurons in the SC have a fixed magnitude of output per unit input, and essentially operate an all or nothing response, the level of neural firing can be more finely tuned by variations in input by the AES.

Although there is evidence for either perspective of the integration/differentiation dichotomy, a significant body of evidence also exists for a combination of factors from either view. Thus, analogous to the broader nativist/empiricist argument, it is apparent that rather than a dichotomy, there exists a continuum, such that the integration and differentiation hypotheses are extremes at either end.

See also

References

  1. ^ Lewkowicz, D.J & Ghazanfar, A.A. (2009) "The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing", Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13:11, pp 470-478
  2. ^ Deneve, S. & Pouget, A. (2004) "Bayesian multisensory integration and cross-modal spatial links", Journal of Physiology-Paris, 98:1-3, pp 249-258
  3. ^ a b Pouget, A. Deneve, S. & Duhamel, JR. (2002) "A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations", Nature Reviews Neurosicence, 3, pp. 741-747
  4. ^ Shams, S., Kamitani Y. & Shimojo, S (2000) "Illusions: What you see is what you hear", Nature 408, 788
  5. ^ Watkins, l., Shams, L., Josephs, A. & Rees, G. (2007) "Activity in human V1 follows multisensory perception", NeuroImage, 37: 572–578
  6. ^ Shams, L., Iwaki, S., Chawla, A. & Bhattacharya, J. (2005) "Early modulation of visual cortex by sound: an MEG study", Neuroscience Letters, 378: 76–81
  7. ^ Tobias, S., Andersen, Tiippana, K., & Sams, M. (2004) "Factors influencing audiovisual fission and fusion illusions", Cognitive Brain Research, 21:301– 308
  8. ^ (Wallace, 2004)
  9. ^ Renier LA, Anurova I, De Volder AG, Carlson S, VanMeter J, Rauschecker JP. (2009). Multisensory integration of sounds and vibrotactile stimuli in processing streams for "what" and "where". J Neurosci. 29(35):10950-60. PMID 19726653 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0910-09.2009
  • Affifi, Bergman (2005)
  • Alais, Burr (2004)
  • Aristotle (350 B.C.)
  • Bernstein et al. (2002)
  • Botvinick, Cohen (1998)
  • Clavagnier, Falchier, Kennedy (2004)
  • Ehrsson, Holmes, Passingham, (2005)
  • Fodor J (1983)
  • Foxe, Simpson (2002)
  • Giard, Peronnet (1999)
  • Goodale, Milner (1995)
  • Graziano, Gross (1994/1995)
  • Grice (1962)
  • Hairston et al. (2003)
  • Hershenson (1962)
  • Hughs et al. (1994)
  • Jiang, Stein (2003)
  • Jones, Powell (1970)
  • King, Palmer (1985)
  • Kujala et al. (1997)
  • Lewkowicz, Kraebel (2004)
  • Macaluso, Frith, Driver (2000)
  • McGurk, MacDonald (1976)
  • Meredith, Stein (1983)
  • Meredith, Stein (1986)
  • Meredith et al. (1987)
  • Miller, D’Esposito (2005)
  • Petitto et al. (2000)
  • Sadato et al. (2004)
  • Stein, London, Wilkinson, Price (1996)
  • Stein, B. E.; Meredith, M. A. (1993). The Merging of the Senses. The MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-19331-0.
  • Stratton (1897)
  • Tastevin (1937)
  • Theoret, Pascual–Leone (2006)
  • Wallace (2004)
  • Wallace, Meredith, Stein (1993)
  • Wallace, Ramachandran, Stein (2004)
  • Welch and Warren (1980)