Jump to content

Marilyn vos Savant

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Jodamiller (talk | contribs) at 17:23, 21 March 2015. The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Marilyn vos Savant
Born
Marilyn Mach

(1946-08-11) August 11, 1946 (age 78)
St. Louis, Missouri, United States
OccupationAuthor
Known forMagazine column; Guinness Records highest IQ
SpouseRobert Jarvik (1987–present)
Websitewww.marilynvossavant.com

Marilyn vos Savant (/ˌvɒs səˈvɑːnt/; born August 11, 1946) is an American magazine columnist, author, lecturer, and playwright who rose to fame through her former listing in the Guinness Book of World Records under "Highest IQ."[1] Since 1986 she has written "Ask Marilyn," a Parade magazine Sunday column where she solves puzzles and answers questions on various subjects.

Biography

She was born Marilyn Mach in St. Louis, Missouri. Her parents—Joseph Mach and Marina vos Savant—were German and Italian respectively. She says one should keep premarital surnames, with sons taking their fathers’ and daughters their mothers’.[2] The word savant, meaning someone of learning, appears twice in her family: her grandmother’s name was Savant; her grandfather’s, vos Savant. She is of Italian, Czechoslovakian,[3] German,[4] and Austrian ancestry, being descended from physicist and philosopher Ernst Mach.[5]

Teenage Savant worked in her father’s general store and wrote for local newspapers using pseudonyms. She married at 16 and divorced ten years later. Her second marriage ended when she was 35.

She went to Meramec Community College and studied philosophy at Washington University in St. Louis but quit two years later to help with a family investment business. Having sought financial freedom to launch a career in writing, Savant moved to New York City in the 1980s. Prior to starting “Ask Marilyn,” she wrote the Omni I.Q. Quiz Contest for Omni, which included IQ quizzes and expositions on intelligence and its testing.

Savant married Robert Jarvik (one developer of the Jarvik-7 artificial heart) on August 23, 1987 and was made Chief Financial Officer of Jarvik Heart, Inc. She has served on the Board of Directors of the National Council on Economic Education, on the advisory boards of the National Association for Gifted Children and the National Women's History Museum,[6] and as a fellow of the Committee for Skeptical Inquiry.[7] Toastmasters International named her one of “Five Outstanding Speakers of 1999,” and in 2003 she was awarded an honorary Doctor of Letters from The College of New Jersey.

Rise to fame and IQ score

Savant was listed in the Guinness Book of World Records under "Highest IQ" from 1986 to 1989[8] and entered the Guinness Book of World Records Hall of Fame in 1988.[8][9] Guinness retired the "Highest IQ" category in 1990 after concluding IQ tests were too unreliable to designate a single record holder.[8] The listing drew nationwide attention.[10]

Guinness cited her performance on two intelligence tests, the Stanford-Binet and the Mega Test. She took the 1937 Stanford-Binet, Second Revision test at age ten.[4] She claims her first test was in September 1956 and measured her mental age at 22 years and 10 months, yielding a 228 score.[4] This figure was listed in the Guinness Book of World Records; it is also listed in her books’ biographical sections and was given by her in interviews.

Ronald K. Hoeflin looked back at this claim and calculated her IQ at 218 by using 10 years 6 months for her chronological age and 22 years 11 months for her mental age for the test scoring tables.[4] A 10 years and 6 months chronological age corresponds to neither the age in accounts by Savant nor the school records cited by Baumgold, nor does it appear directly in the scoring tables for the 1937 Stanford-Binet test.[11] Savant has commented on reports mentioning varying IQ scores she was said to have obtained.[12]

Alan S. Kaufman, a psychology professor and author of IQ tests, writes in IQ Testing 101 that "Miss Savant was given an old version of the Stanford-Binet (Terman & Merrill 1937), which did, indeed, use the antiquated formula of MA/CA × 100. But in the test manual's norms, the Binet does not permit IQs to rise above 170 at any age, child or adult. And the authors of the old Binet stated: 'Beyond fifteen the mental ages are entirely artificial and are to be thought of as simply numerical scores.' (Terman & Merrill 1937). ...the psychologist who came up with an IQ of 228 committed an extrapolation of a misconception, thereby violating almost every rule imaginable concerning the meaning of IQs."[13]

The second test reported by Guinness was Hoeflin's Mega Test, taken in the mid-1980s. The Mega Test yields IQ standard scores obtained by multiplying the subject's normalized z-score, or the rarity of the raw test score, by a constant standard deviation, and adding the product to 100, with Savant's raw score reported by Hoeflin to be 46 out of a possible 48, with a 5.4 z-score, and a standard deviation of 16, arriving at a 186 IQ. The Mega Test has been criticized by professional psychologists as improperly designed and scored, "nothing short of number pulverization."[14]

Savant sees IQ tests as measurements of a variety of mental abilities and thinks intelligence entails so many factors that "attempts to measure it are useless."[15] She has held memberships with the high-IQ societies Mensa International and the Mega Society.[16]

"Ask Marilyn"

Following her listing in the 1986 Guinness Book of World Records, Parade ran a profile of her along with a selection of questions from Parade readers and her answers. Parade continued to get questions, so “Ask Marilyn” was made.

She uses her column to answer questions on many chiefly academic subjects; solve logical, mathematical or vocabulary puzzles posed by readers; answer requests for advice with logic; and give self-devised quizzes and puzzles. Aside from the weekly printed column, “Ask Marilyn” is a daily online column which adds to the printed version by resolving controversial answers, correcting mistakes, expanding answers, reposting previous answers, and solving additional questions.

Three of her books (Ask Marilyn, More Marilyn, and Of Course, I’m for Monogamy) are compilations of questions and answers from “Ask Marilyn”. The Power of Logical Thinking includes many questions and answers from the column.

Fermat’s last theorem

A few months after Andrew Wiles said he had proved Fermat’s Last Theorem, Savant published The World’s Most Famous Math Problem (October 1993),[17] which surveys the history of Fermat’s last theorem as well as other mathematical problems. Controversy came from its criticism of Wiles’ proof; she was said to misunderstand mathematical induction, proof by contradiction, and imaginary numbers.[18]

Especially contested was her statement that Wiles’ proof should be rejected for its use of non-Euclidean geometry. She said that because “the chain of proof is based in hyperbolic (Lobachevskian) geometry,” and because squaring the circle is seen as a “famous impossibility” despite being possible in hyperbolic geometry, then “if we reject a hyperbolic method of squaring the circle, we should also reject a hyperbolic proof of Fermat’s last theorem.”

Specialists flagged discrepancies between the two cases, distinguishing the use of hyperbolic geometry as a “tool” for proving Fermat’s last theorem and from its use as a “setting” for squaring the circle: squaring the circle in hyperbolic geometry is a different problem from that of squaring it in Euclidean geometry. Savant was criticized for rejecting hyperbolic geometry as a satisfactory basis for Wiles’ proof, with critics pointing out that axiomatic set theory (rather than Euclidean geometry) is now the accepted foundation of mathematical proofs and that set theory is sufficiently robust to encompass both Euclidean and non-Euclidean geometry as well as geometry and adding numbers.

Savant retracted the argument in a July 1995 addendum, saying she saw the theorem as "an intellectual challenge – 'to find a proof with Fermat’s tools.'" Fermat claimed to have a proof he could not fit in the margins where he wrote his theorem. If he really had a proof, it would presumably be Euclidean. Therefore, Wiles may have proven the theorem but Fermat’s proof remains undiscovered, if it ever really existed. She now agrees there are no restrictions on what kind of tools may be used.

Famous columns

The Monty Hall problem

Savant was asked the following question in her September 9, 1990 column:

Suppose you’re on a game show, and you’re given the choice of three doors. Behind one door is a car, behind the others, goats. You pick a door, say #1, and the host, who knows what’s behind the doors, opens another door, say #3, which has a goat. He says to you, “Do you want to pick door #2?” Is it to your advantage to switch your choice of doors?

— Craig F. Whitaker Columbia, Maryland, [19]

This question is called the Monty Hall problem due to its resembling scenarios on the game show Let's Make a Deal; its answer existed before it was used in “Ask Marilyn”. She said the selection should be switched to door #2 because it has a 2/3 chance of success, while door #1 has just 1/3. To summarize, 2/3 of the time the opened door #3 will indicate the location of the door with the car (the door you had not picked and the one not opened by the host). Only 1/3 of the time will the opened door #3 mislead you into changing from the winning door to a losing door. These probabilities assume you change your choice each time door #3 is opened, and that the host always opens a door with a goat. This response provoked letters from thousands of readers, nearly all arguing doors #1 and #2 each have an equal chance of success. A follow-up column reaffirming her position served only to intensify the debate and soon became a feature article on the front page of The New York Times. Parade received around 10,000 letters from readers who thought her wrong.[20]

Under the “standard” version of the problem, the host always opens a losing door and offers a switch. In the standard version, Savant’s answer is correct. However, the statement of the problem as posed in her column is ambiguous.[21] The answer depends on what strategy the host is following. If the host operates under a strategy of only offering a switch if the initial guess is correct, it would clearly be disadvantageous to accept the offer. If the host merely selects a door at random, the question is likewise very different from the standard version. Savant addressed these issues by writing the following in Parade Magazine, “the original answer defines certain conditions, the most significant of which is that the host always opens a losing door on purpose. Anything else is a different question.”[22]

She expounded on her reasoning in a second follow-up and called on school teachers to show the problem to classes. In her final column on the problem, she gave the results of more than 1,000 school experiments. Nearly 100% found it pays to switch. Of the readers who wrote computer simulations of the problem, 97% reached the same conclusion. Most respondents now agree with her original solution, with half of the published letters declaring their authors had changed their minds.[23]

"Two boys" problem

Like the Monty Hall problem, the "two boys" or "second-sibling" problem predates Ask Marilyn, but generated controversy in the column,[24] first appearing there in 1991–92 in the context of baby beagles:

A shopkeeper says she has two new baby beagles to show you, but she doesn't know whether they're male, female, or a pair. You tell her that you want only a male, and she telephones the fellow who's giving them a bath. "Is at least one a male?" she asks him. "Yes!" she informs you with a smile. What is the probability that the other one is a male?

— Stephen I. Geller, Pasadena, California

When Savant replied "one out of three," readers[citation needed] wrote the odds were 50–50. In a follow-up, she defended her answer, saying that "If we could shake a pair of puppies out of a cup the way we do dice, there are four ways they could land", in three of which at least one is male, but in only one of which both are male.

The confusion arises here because the bather is not asked if the puppy he is holding is a male, but rather if either is a male. If the puppies are labeled (A and B), each has a 50% chance of being male independently. This independence is restricted when at least A or B is male. Now, if A is not male, B must be male, and vice-versa. This restriction is introduced by the way the question is structured and is easily overlooked – misleading people to the erroneous answer of 50%. See Boy or Girl paradox for solution details.

The problem re-emerged in 1996–97 with two cases juxtaposed:

Say that a woman and a man (who are unrelated) each have two children. We know that at least one of the woman’s children is a boy and that the man’s oldest child is a boy. Can you explain why the chances that the woman has two boys do not equal the chances that the man has two boys? My algebra teacher insists that the probability is greater that the man has two boys, but I think the chances may be the same. What do you think?

Savant agreed with the teacher, saying the chances were only 1 out of 3 that the woman had two boys, but 1 out of 2 the man had two boys. Readers argued for 1 out of 2 in both cases, prompting follow-ups. Finally she began a survey, asking female readers with exactly two children, at least one of them male, to give the sex of both children. Of the 17,946 women who responded, 35.9%, about 1 in 3, had two boys.[25]

Woman has
young boy, older girl young girl, older boy 2 boys 2 girls
Probability: 1/3 1/3 1/3 0


Man has
young boy, older girl young girl, older boy 2 boys 2 girls
Probability: 0 1/2 1/2 0

Publications

  • 1985 – Omni I.Q. Quiz Contest
  • 1990 – Brain Building: Exercising Yourself Smarter (co-written with Leonore Fleischer)
  • 1992 – Ask Marilyn: Answers to America's Most Frequently Asked Questions
  • 1993 – The World's Most Famous Math Problem: The Proof of Fermat's Last Theorem and Other Mathematical Mysteries
  • 1994 – More Marilyn: Some Like It Bright!
  • 1994 – "I've Forgotten Everything I Learned in School!": A Refresher Course to Help You Reclaim Your Education
  • 1996 – Of Course I'm for Monogamy: I'm Also for Everlasting Peace and an End to Taxes
  • 1996 – The Power of Logical Thinking: Easy Lessons in the Art of Reasoning…and Hard Facts about Its Absence in Our Lives
  • 2000 – The Art of Spelling: The Madness and the Method
  • 2002 – Growing Up: A Classic American Childhood

References

  1. ^ "The Time Everyone “Corrected” the World’s Smartest Woman". Priceonomics, Feb 19, 2015
  2. ^ vos Savant, Marilyn (25 November 2001). "Ask Marilyn". Parade.
  3. ^ vos Savant, Marilyn (4 May 2013). "Ask Marilyn: The 'First Sandwich Generation': True Trend or Marketing Invention?". Parade. Retrieved 2013-08-15.
  4. ^ a b c d Baumgold, Julie (6 February 1989). "In the Kingdom of the Brain". New York Magazine. New York Media, LLC.
  5. ^ Vitez, Michael (12 October 1988). "Two of a Kind". The Chicago Tribune.
  6. ^ National Women’s History Museum
  7. ^ "CSI Fellows and Staff". Center for Inquiry. Retrieved 20 June 2012.
  8. ^ a b c Knight, Sam (10 April 2009). "Is a high IQ a burden as much as a blessing?". Financial Times. Financial Times Ltd. Retrieved 7 October 2013. {{cite news}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  9. ^ http://www.parade.com/askmarilyn
  10. ^ Knight, Sam (10 April 2009). "Is a high IQ a burden as much as a blessing?". Financial Times. Financial Times Ltd. Castles, Elaine E. (6 June 2012). Inventing Intelligence. ABC-CLIO. p. 3. ISBN 978-1-4408-0338-3. Retrieved 31 August 2013. And what is that makes Marilyn vos Savant so uniquely qualified to answer such questions? There is only one reason: she is listed in the Guinness Book of World Records as having the highest IQ ever recorded. Never mind that this record is based on a nonstandardized test put out by an obscure group known as Mega, supposedly the world's most selective organization of geniuses. Ignore the fact that test scores at the extreme ends of any distribution are notoriously unreliable. . . . None of this is meant to downplay her very real accomplishments; by all accounts, vos Savant is a sensible and grounded woman, and she has won several awards for her work in the fields of education and communications. But her fame came, in the words of journalist Julie Baumgold, 'only because of the glory of that number.' (citing New York magazine 22 (1989):36–42) {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help)
  11. ^ Terman, Lewis Madison; Merrill, Maude A. (1937). Measuring intelligence: A guide to the administration of the new revised Stanford-Binet tests of intelligence. Riverside textbooks in education. Boston (MA): Houghton Mifflin. p. 450 (last page of scoring tables). OCLC 964301.
  12. ^ vos Savant, Marilyn (12 June 2001). "Ask Marilyn: Are adult IQ tests more accurate than child IQ tests?". Parade. Retrieved 2008-11-15.[dead link]
  13. ^ Kaufman, Alan S. (2009). IQ Testing 101. New York: Springer Publishing. p. 104. ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2.
  14. ^ Carlson, Roger D. (1991). Keyser, Daniel J.; Sweetland, Richard C. (eds.). Test Critiques (Volume VIII ed.). PRO-ED. pp. 431–435. ISBN 0-89079-254-2. Although the approach that Hoeflin takes is interesting, it violates good psychometric principles by overinterpreting the weak data of a self-selected sample. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  15. ^ vos Savant, Marilyn (17 July 2005). "Ask Marilyn: Are Men Smarter Than Women?". Parade. Retrieved 2008-02-25.[dead link]
  16. ^ Thompson, D. (5 July 1986). "Marilyn's Most Vital Statistic". The Courier-Mail. {{cite news}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help)
  17. ^ Fermat’s Last Theorem and Wiles’ proof were discussed in her Parade column of November 21, 1993, which introduced the book.
  18. ^ Boston, Nigel; Granville, Andrew (May 1995). "Review of The World's Most Famous Math Problem" (.PDF). American Mathematical Monthly. 102 (5). The American Mathematical Monthly, Vol. 102, No. 5: 470–473. doi:10.2307/2975048. JSTOR 2975048. Retrieved 2008-02-25.
  19. ^ vos Savant, Marilyn. "Game Show Problem". marilynvossavant.com. Retrieved 2010-08-07.
  20. ^ Tierney, John (21 July 1991). "Behind Monty Hall's Doors: Puzzle, Debate and Answer?". The New York Times. Retrieved 2008-08-07.
  21. ^ Krauss, Stefan and Wang, X. T. (2003). "The Psychology of the Monty Hall Problem: Discovering Psychological Mechanisms for Solving a Tenacious Brain Teaser", Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132(1). Retrieved from http://www.usd.edu/~xtwang/Papers/MontyHallPaper.pdf 30 March 2008.
  22. ^ "Game Show Problem". marilynvossavant.com. Retrieved 2008-06-02.
  23. ^ vos Savant, Marilyn (1992). "Ask Marilyn". Parade. {{cite news}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help)
  24. ^ The problem appeared in Ask Marilyn on October 13, 1991 with a follow-up on January 5, 1992 (initially involving two baby beagles instead of two children), and then on May 26, 1996 with follow-ups on December 1, 1996, March 30, 1997, July 20, 1997, and October 19, 1997.
  25. ^ Stansfield, William D.; Carlton, Matthew A. (February 2009). "The Most Widely Publicized Gender Problem in Human Genetics". Human Biology. 81 (1). Retrieved 2013-04-07. Some readers doubted her 1/3 solution, so she asked for data from her women readers "with two children (no more), at least one of which is a boy (either child or both of them)." She got 17,946 responses by letters and e-mails. Without reporting the sex ratio in the sample, she says about 35.9% of respondents ("about 1 in 3") said they have two boys.

Template:Persondata