Pattern recognition (psychology): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
Tag: section blanking
Recdep (talk | contribs)
added references (WikiProject Unreferenced articles); copyedit (lead section); replaced Unreferenced with Refimprove; categories
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Other uses|Pattern recognition (disambiguation)}}
{{Other uses|Pattern recognition (disambiguation)}}
{{Unreferenced|date=October 2006}}
{{Refimprove|date=November 2014}}


[[File:Mirror-Induced-Behavior-in-the-Magpie-(Pica-pica)-Evidence-of-Self-Recognition-pbio.0060202.sv001.ogv|thumb|[[Mirror]] induced behavior in the [[magpie]]]]
[[File:Mirror-Induced-Behavior-in-the-Magpie-(Pica-pica)-Evidence-of-Self-Recognition-pbio.0060202.sv001.ogv|thumb|[[Mirror]] induced behavior in the [[magpie]]]]
In [[psychology]] and [[cognitive neuroscience]], '''pattern recognition''' describes a [[cognitive]] process that matches information from a [[Stimulus (psychology)|stimulus]] with information [[Recall (memory)|retrieved]] from [[memory]].{{Sfn|Eysenck & Keane|2003|pp=83–117}} Among others, the recognized ''patterns'' can be those perceived in [[facial feature]]s,{{Sfn|Chambon et al.|2007|p=2}} units of [[music]],{{Sfn|Krumhansl|2001|pp=3–8}} components of [[language]]{{Sfn|Margolis|1996|pp=56–57}} or [[Character (symbol)|characters]] and other [[symbol]]s.{{Sfn|Eysenck & Keane|2003|pp=83–117}} One [[theory]] understands patterns as a set of characteristic ''features'' extracted from the stimulus, but it does not comprehensively describe the process or the role of ''context'' and there is a multitude of other theories with different approaches.{{Efn|{{Harvnb|Eysenck & Keane|2003|pp=83–117}}, see also ''context'' as described by {{Harvnb|Krumhansl|2001|pp=3–8}}}} Pattern recognition does not occur instantly, although it does happen automatically and spontaneously.{{cn|date=November 2014}} Pattern recognition is an innate ability of animals.{{cn|date=November 2014}}
'''Pattern recognition''' involves identification of faces, objects, words, melodies, etc. The visual system does more than just interpret forms, contours and colors. Pattern recognition refers to the process of recognizing a set of [[Stimulus (psychology)|stimuli]] arranged in a certain pattern that is characteristic of that set of stimuli. Pattern recognition does not occur instantly, although it does happen automatically and spontaneously. Pattern recognition is an innate ability of animals.


==Theories of pattern recognition==
==Theories of pattern recognition==
Line 44: Line 44:
*[[Pareidolia]]
*[[Pareidolia]]
*[[Thin-slicing]]
*[[Thin-slicing]]

==Notes==
{{Notelist}}

==Citations==
{{Reflist|30em}}

==References==
{{Refbegin}}
*{{Cite book
| last =Chambon
| first =Valérian
| last2 =Vernet
| first2 =Mathilde
| last3 =Martin
| first3 =Flavie
| last4 =Baudouin
| first4 =Jean-Yves
| last5 =Tiberghien
| first5 =Guy
| last6 =Franck
| first6 =Nicolas
| display-authors =1
| editor-last =Corrigan
| editor-first =Marsha S.
| publication-date =2007
| title =Pattern Recognition in Biology
| chapter =Visual Pattern Recognition: What Makes Faces so Special?
| publisher =Nova Science Publishers
| publication-place =New York
| isbn =9781600217166
| oclc =123962949
| url =http://books.google.com/books?vid=ISBN9781600217166
| accessdate =27 November 2014
| ref ={{harvid|Chambon et al.|2007}}
}}
*{{Cite book
| last =Eysenck
| first =Michael W.
| last2 =Keane
| first2 =Mark T.
| publication-date =2003
| title =Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook
| edition ={{Ordinal|4|sup=yes}}
| publisher =Taylor & Francis
| publication-place =Hove; Philadelphia; New York
| isbn =9780863775512
| oclc =894210185
| url =http://books.google.com/books?vid=ISBN9780863775512
| accessdate =27 November 2014
| ref ={{harvid|Eysenck & Keane|2003}}
}}
*{{Cite book
| last =Krumhansl
| first =Carol L.
| publication-date =2001
| origyear =1990
| title =Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch
| edition ={{Ordinal|2|sup=yes}}
| series =Oxford Psychology Series No. 17
| publisher =Oxford University Press
| publication-place =New York
| isbn =9780198022152
| issn =1362-9972
| oclc =62386986
| url =http://books.google.com/books?vid=ISBN9780198022152
| accessdate =27 November 2014
| ref ={{harvid|Krumhansl|2001}}
}}
*{{Cite book
| last =Margolis
| first =Howard
| publication-date =1996
| origyear =1987
| title =Patterns, Thinking, and Cognition: A Theory of Judgement
| edition ={{Ordinal|3|sup=yes}}
| publisher =University of Chicago Press
| publication-place =Chicago; London
| isbn =9780226505282
| oclc =15792013
| url =http://books.google.com/books?vid=ISBN9780226505282
| accessdate =27 November 2014
| ref ={{harvid|Margolis|1996}}
}}
{{Refend}}


==External links==
==External links==
Line 50: Line 135:
{{Hidden messages}}
{{Hidden messages}}


[[Category:Visual perception]]
[[Category:Perception]]
[[Category:Cognition]]
[[Category:Cognitive psychology]]

Revision as of 03:37, 27 November 2014

Mirror induced behavior in the magpie

In psychology and cognitive neuroscience, pattern recognition describes a cognitive process that matches information from a stimulus with information retrieved from memory.[1] Among others, the recognized patterns can be those perceived in facial features,[2] units of music,[3] components of language[4] or characters and other symbols.[1] One theory understands patterns as a set of characteristic features extracted from the stimulus, but it does not comprehensively describe the process or the role of context and there is a multitude of other theories with different approaches.[a] Pattern recognition does not occur instantly, although it does happen automatically and spontaneously.[citation needed] Pattern recognition is an innate ability of animals.[citation needed]

Theories of pattern recognition

Template matching

The incoming sensory information is compared directly to copies (templates) stored in the long term memory. These copies are stored in the process of our past experiences and learning.

E.g. A A A are all recognized as the letter A but not B.

Note: This does not allow for variation in letters unless there are templates for each variation.

Prototype matching

Prototype means a concept of average characteristics of a particular subject. It can be found throughout the world. For instance a concept of small animal with feathers, beak, two wings that can fly is a prototype concept of a crow, sparrow, hen, eagle, etc. Prototype matching, unlike template matching, does not emphasize a perfect match between the incoming stimuli and the stored concept in the brain

Feature analysis

According to this theory, the sensory system breaks down the incoming stimuli into its features and processes the information. Some features may be more important for recognition than others. All stimuli have a set of distinctive features. Feature analysis proceeds through 4 stages.[citation needed]

  1. Detection
  2. Pattern dissection
  3. Feature comparison in memory
  4. Recognition

Multiple discrimination scaling

Template and feature analysis approaches to recognition of objects (and situations) have been merged / reconciled / overtaken by multiple discrimination theory. This states that the amounts in a test stimulus of each salient feature of a template are recognized in any perceptual judgment as being at a distance in the universal unit of 50% discrimination (the objective performance 'JND': Torgerson, 1958) from the amount of that feature in the template (Booth & Freeman, 1993, Acta Psychologica).

False pattern recognition

Shark or submarine?

The human tendency to see patterns that do not actually exist is called apophenia. Examples of apophenia include the Man in the Moon, faces or figures in shadows, clouds and in patterns with no deliberate design, such as the swirls on a baked confection, and the perception of causal relationships between events which are, in fact, unrelated. Apophenia figures prominently in conspiracy theories, gambling, misinterpretation of statistics and scientific data, and some kinds of religious and paranormal experiences. Misperception of patterns in random data is called pareidolia.

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Eysenck & Keane 2003, pp. 83–117, see also context as described by Krumhansl 2001, pp. 3–8

Citations

  1. ^ a b Eysenck & Keane 2003, pp. 83–117.
  2. ^ Chambon et al. 2007, p. 2.
  3. ^ Krumhansl 2001, pp. 3–8.
  4. ^ Margolis 1996, pp. 56–57.

References

  • Chambon, Valérian; et al. (2007). "Visual Pattern Recognition: What Makes Faces so Special?". In Corrigan, Marsha S. (ed.). Pattern Recognition in Biology. New York: Nova Science Publishers. ISBN 9781600217166. OCLC 123962949. Retrieved 27 November 2014.
  • Eysenck, Michael W.; Keane, Mark T. (2003). Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook (4th ed.). Hove; Philadelphia; New York: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9780863775512. OCLC 894210185. Retrieved 27 November 2014.
  • Krumhansl, Carol L. (2001) [1990]. Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch. Oxford Psychology Series No. 17 (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780198022152. ISSN 1362-9972. OCLC 62386986. Retrieved 27 November 2014.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: ref duplicates default (link)
  • Margolis, Howard (1996) [1987]. Patterns, Thinking, and Cognition: A Theory of Judgement (3rd ed.). Chicago; London: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 9780226505282. OCLC 15792013. Retrieved 27 November 2014.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: ref duplicates default (link)

External links

Media related to Visual pattern recognition at Wikimedia Commons