Anosognosia
Anosognosia | |
---|---|
Pronunciation | |
Specialty | Psychiatry, Neurology |
Anosognosia is a condition in which a person with a disability is cognitively unaware of having it due to an underlying physical or psychological (e.g., PTSD, Stockholm syndrome, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, dementia) condition. Anosognosia can result from physiological damage to brain structures, typically to the parietal lobe or a diffuse lesion on the fronto-temporal-parietal area in the right hemisphere,[1][2][3] and is thus a neuropsychiatric disorder. A deficit of self-awareness, it was first named by the neurologist Joseph Babinski in 1914.[4] Phenomenologically, anosognosia has similarities to denial, which is a psychological defense mechanism; attempts have been made at a unified explanation.[5] Anosognosia is sometimes accompanied by asomatognosia, a form of neglect in which patients deny ownership of body parts such as their limbs. The term is from Ancient Greek ἀ- a-, 'without', νόσος nosos, 'disease' and γνῶσις gnōsis, 'knowledge'. It is also considered a disorder that makes the treatment of the patient more difficult, since it may affect negatively the therapeutic relationship.[6]
Causes
Relatively little has been discovered about the cause of the condition since its initial identification. Recent empirical studies tend to consider anosognosia a multi-componential syndrome or multi-faceted phenomenon. That is it can be manifested by failure to be aware of a number of specific deficits, including motor (hemiplegia), sensory (hemianaesthesia, hemianopia), spatial (unilateral neglect), memory (dementia), and language (receptive aphasia) due to impairment of anatomo-functionally discrete monitoring systems.[1][2]
Anosognosia is relatively common following different causes of brain injury, such as stroke and traumatic brain injury; for example, anosognosia for hemiparesis (weakness of one side of the body) with onset of acute stroke is estimated at between 10% and 18%.[7] However, it can appear to occur in conjunction with virtually any neurological impairment. It is more frequent in the acute than in the chronic phase and more prominent for assessment in the cases with right hemispheric lesions than with the left.[8] Anosognosia is not related to global mental confusion, cognitive flexibility, other major intellectual disturbances, or mere sensory/perceptual deficits.[citation needed]
The condition does not seem to be directly related to sensory loss but is thought to be caused by damage to higher level neurocognitive processes that are involved in integrating sensory information with processes that support spatial or bodily representations (including the somatosensory system). Anosognosia is thought to be related to unilateral neglect, a condition often found after damage to the non-dominant (usually the right[9]) hemisphere of the cerebral cortex in which people seem unable to attend to, or sometimes comprehend, anything on a certain side of their body[10] (usually the left).
Anosognosia can be selective in that an affected person with multiple impairments may seem unaware of only one handicap, while appearing to be fully aware of any others.[11] This is consistent with the idea that the source of the problem relates to spatial representation of the body. For example, anosognosia for hemiplegia may occur with or without intact awareness of visuo-spatial unilateral neglect. This phenomenon of double dissociation can be an indicator of domain-specific disorders of awareness modules, meaning that in anosognosia, brain damage can selectively impact the self-monitoring process of one specific physical or cognitive function rather than a spatial location of the body.[1][2][12]
There are also studies showing that the maneuver of vestibular stimulation could temporarily improve both the syndrome of spatial unilateral neglect and of anosognosia for left hemiplegia. Combining the findings of hemispheric asymmetry to the right, association with spatial unilateral neglect, and the temporal improvement on both syndromes, it is suggested there can be a spatial component underlying the mechanism of anosognosia for motor weakness and that neural processes could be modulated similarly.[2] There were some cases of anosognosia for right hemiplegia after left hemisphere damage, but the frequency of this type of anosognosia has not been estimated.[1]
Anosognosia may occur as part of receptive aphasia, a language disorder that causes poor comprehension of speech and the production of fluent but incomprehensible sentences. A patient with receptive aphasia cannot correct his own phonetics errors and shows "anger and disappointment with the person with whom s/he is speaking because that person fails to understand her/him". This may be a result of brain damage to the posterior portion of the superior temporal gyrus, believed to contain representations of word sounds. With those representations significantly distorted, patients with receptive aphasia are unable to monitor their mistakes.[4] Other patients with receptive aphasia are fully aware of their condition and speech inhibitions, but cannot monitor their condition, which is not the same as anosognosia and therefore cannot explain the occurrence of neologistic jargon.[13]
Psychiatry
Although largely used to describe unawareness of impairment after brain injury or stroke, the term "anosognosia" is occasionally used to describe the lack of insight shown by some people with anorexia nervosa.[14] They do not seem to recognize that they have a mental illness. There is evidence that anosognosia related to schizophrenia may be the result of frontal lobe damage.[15] E. Fuller Torrey, a psychiatrist and schizophrenia researcher, has stated that among those with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, anosognosia is the most prevalent reason for not taking medications.[16]
Diagnosis
Clinically, anosognosia is often assessed by giving patients an anosognosia questionnaire in order to assess their metacognitive knowledge of deficits. However, neither of the existing questionnaires applied in the clinics are designed thoroughly for evaluating the multidimensional nature of this clinical phenomenon; nor are the responses obtained via offline questionnaire capable of revealing the discrepancy of awareness observed from their online task performance.[8][17] The discrepancy is noticed when patients showed no awareness of their deficits from the offline responses to the questionnaire but demonstrated reluctance or verbal circumlocution when asked to perform an online task. For example, patients with anosognosia for hemiplegia may find excuses not to perform a bimanual task even though they do not admit it is because of their paralyzed arms.[17]
A similar situation can happen to patients with anosognosia for cognitive deficits after traumatic brain injury when monitoring their errors during the tasks regarding their memory and attention (online emergent awareness) and when predicting their performance right before the same tasks (online anticipatory awareness).[18] It can also occur among patients with dementia and anosognosia for memory deficit when prompted with dementia-related words, showing possible pre-attentive processing and implicit knowledge of their memory problems.[19] Patients with anosognosia may also overestimate their performance when asked in first-person formed questions but not from a third-person perspective when the questions referring to others.[1][3][17]
When assessing the causes of anosognosia within stroke patients, CT scans have been used to assess where the greatest amount of damage is found within the various areas of the brain. Stroke patients with mild and severe levels of anosognosia (determined by response to an anosognosia questionnaire) have been linked to lesions within the temporoparietal and thalamic regions, when compared to those who experience moderate anosognosia, or none at all.[20] In contrast, after a stroke, people with moderate anosognosia have a higher frequency of lesions involving the basal ganglia, compared to those with mild or severe anosognosia.[20]
Treatment
In regard to anosognosia for neurological patients, no long-term treatments exist. As with unilateral neglect, caloric reflex testing (squirting ice cold water into the left ear) is known to temporarily ameliorate unawareness of impairment. It is not entirely clear how this works, although it is thought that the unconscious shift of attention or focus caused by the intense stimulation of the vestibular system temporarily influences awareness. Most cases of anosognosia appear to simply disappear over time, while other cases can last indefinitely. Normally, long-term cases are treated with cognitive therapy to train patients to adjust for their inoperable limbs (though it is believed that these patients still are not "aware" of their disability). Another commonly used method is the use of feedback – comparing clients' self-predicted performance with their actual performance on a task in an attempt to improve insight.[21]
Neurorehabilitation is difficult because, as anosognosia impairs the patient's desire to seek medical aid, it may also impair their ability to seek rehabilitation.[22] A lack of awareness of the deficit makes cooperative, mindful work with a therapist difficult. In the acute phase, very little can be done to improve their awareness, but during this time, it is important for the therapist to build a therapeutic alliance with patients by entering their phenomenological field and reducing their frustration and confusion. Since severity changes over time, no single method of treatment or rehabilitation has emerged or will likely emerge.[23]
In regard to psychiatric patients, empirical studies verify that, for individuals with severe mental illnesses, lack of awareness of illness is significantly associated with both medication non-compliance and re-hospitalization.[24] Fifteen percent of individuals with severe mental illnesses who refuse to take medication voluntarily under any circumstances may require some form of coercion to remain compliant because of anosognosia.[25] Coercive psychiatric treatment is a delicate and complex legal and ethical issue, but under certain circumstances, such as for individuals with schizophrenia and anosognosia, it can be justified.[26]
One study of voluntary and involuntary inpatients confirmed that committed patients require coercive treatment because they fail to recognize their need for care.[27] The patients committed to the hospital had significantly lower measures of insight than the voluntary patients.[citation needed]
Anosognosia is also closely related to other cognitive dysfunctions that may impair the capacity of an individual to continuously participate in treatment.[27] Other research has suggested that attitudes toward treatment can improve after involuntary treatment and that previously committed patients tend later to seek voluntary treatment.[28]
See also
References
- ^ a b c d e Moro V, Pernigo S, Zapparoli P, Cordioli Z, Aglioti SM (November 2011). "Phenomenology and neural correlates of implicit and emergent motor awareness in patients with anosognosia for hemiplegia". Behavioural Brain Research. 225 (1): 259–269. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.010. PMID 21777624. S2CID 8389272.
- ^ a b c d Vallar G, Ronchi R (2006). "Anosognosia for motor and sensory deficits after unilateral brain damage: a review". Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 24 (4–6): 247–257. PMID 17119302.
- ^ a b Vuilleumier P (February 2004). "Anosognosia: the neurology of beliefs and uncertainties". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 40 (1): 9–17. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70918-3. PMID 15070000. S2CID 4482597.
- ^ a b Prigatano, George P.; Schacter, Daniel L (1991). Awareness of deficit after brain injury: clinical and theoretical issues. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. pp. 53–55. ISBN 978-0-19-505941-0.
- ^ Ramachandran, V. S.; Blakeslee, Sandra (1999). Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind. New York: Quill. pp. 113–157. ISBN 978-0-688-17217-6.
- ^ Castillero O (21 October 2016). "Anosognosia". Psicología y Mente (in Spanish). Archived from the original on 2020-04-17.
- ^ Baier B, Karnath HO (March 2005). "Incidence and diagnosis of anosognosia for hemiparesis revisited". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 76 (3): 358–361. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2004.036731. PMC 1739568. PMID 15716526.
- ^ a b Orfei MD, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G (2009). "The evaluation of anosognosia in stroke patients". Cerebrovascular Diseases. 27 (3): 280–289. doi:10.1159/000199466. PMID 19202333.
- ^ Breier JI, Adair JC, Gold M, Fennell EB, Gilmore RL, Heilman KM (January 1995). "Dissociation of anosognosia for hemiplegia and aphasia during left-hemisphere anesthesia". Neurology. 45 (1): 65–67. doi:10.1212/WNL.45.1.65. PMID 7824138. S2CID 46383489. INIST 3452304.
- ^ Heilman KM, Barrett AM, Adair JC (November 1998). "Possible mechanisms of anosognosia: a defect in self-awareness". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 353 (1377): 1903–1909. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0342. PMC 1692420. PMID 9854262.
- ^ Hirstein W (2005). Brain fiction: self-deception and the riddle of confabulation. MIT Press. p. 148. ISBN 978-0-262-08338-6.
- ^ Spinazzola L, Pia L, Folegatti A, Marchetti C, Berti A (February 2008). "Modular structure of awareness for sensorimotor disorders: evidence from anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianaesthesia". Neuropsychologia. 46 (3): 915–926. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.569.2766. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.015. PMID 18281065. S2CID 2436977.
- ^ Ellis AW, Miller D, Sin G (December 1983). "Wernicke's aphasia and normal language processing: a case study in cognitive neuropsychology". Cognition. 15 (1–3): 111–144. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90036-7. PMID 6686505. S2CID 29284758.
- ^ "Anosognosia/anosognosic - Eating Disorders Glossary". glossary.feast-ed.org. Retrieved 2015-06-23.
- ^ Pia L, Tamietto M (October 2006). "Unawareness in schizophrenia: neuropsychological and neuroanatomical findings". Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 60 (5): 531–537. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01576.x. hdl:2318/8242. PMID 16958934. S2CID 42043399.
- ^ Johnson RS (12 August 2014). "Anosognosia and Getting a "Borderline" into Therapy". BPDFamily.com. Retrieved 25 October 2014.
- ^ a b c Marcel AJ, Tegnér R, Nimmo-Smith I (February 2004). "Anosognosia for plegia: specificity, extension, partiality and disunity of bodily unawareness". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 40 (1): 19–40. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70919-5. PMID 15070001. S2CID 4484058.
- ^ O'Keeffe F, Dockree P, Moloney P, Carton S, Robertson IH (January 2007). "Awareness of deficits in traumatic brain injury: a multidimensional approach to assessing metacognitive knowledge and online-awareness". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 13 (1): 38–49. doi:10.1017/S1355617707070075. hdl:2262/35786. PMID 17166302. S2CID 8466337.
- ^ Martyr A, Clare L, Nelis SM, Roberts JL, Robinson JU, Roth I, et al. (January 2011). "Dissociation between implicit and explicit manifestations of awareness in early stage dementia: evidence from the emotional Stroop effect for dementia-related words". International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 26 (1): 92–99. doi:10.1002/gps.2495. PMID 21157854. S2CID 34463285.
- ^ a b Starkstein SE, Fedoroff JP, Price TR, Leiguarda R, Robinson RG (October 1992). "Anosognosia in patients with cerebrovascular lesions. A study of causative factors". Stroke. 23 (10): 1446–1453. doi:10.1161/01.STR.23.10.1446. PMID 1412582.
- ^ Chapman S, Colvin LE, Vuorre M, Cocchini G, Metcalfe J, Huey ED, Cosentino S (April 2018). "Cross domain self-monitoring in anosognosia for memory loss in Alzheimer's disease". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 101: 221–233. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.019. PMC 5877321. PMID 29518705.
- ^ Prigatano, George P.; Schacter, Daniel L. (1991). Awareness of deficit after brain injury: clinical and theoretical issues. New York, New York: Oxford University Press. OCLC 496306119.
- ^ Prigatano GP (2005). "Disturbances of self-awareness and rehabilitation of patients with traumatic brain injury: a 20-year perspective". The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation. 20 (1): 19–29. doi:10.1097/00001199-200501000-00004. PMID 15668568. S2CID 27815630.
- ^ McEvoy J. (1998). "The Relationship Between Insight in Psychosis and Compliance With Medications". In Xavier F. Amador; Anthony S. David (eds.). Insight and Psychosis. Oxford University Press US. p. 299. ISBN 978-0-19-508497-9.
- ^ David, Anthony S.; Amador, Xavier Francisco (2004). Insight and psychosis: awareness of illness in schizophrenia and related disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 293. ISBN 978-0-19-852568-4.
- ^ Marley J (December 2007). "Anosognosia and schizophrenia: the ethical intersection of insight, treatment and coercion". BMC Psychiatry. 7 (S1): S20. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-7-S1-S20. ISSN 1471-244X. PMC 3332836.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ a b McEvoy JP, Applebaum PS, Apperson LJ, Geller JL, Freter S (1989). "Why must some schizophrenic patients be involuntarily committed? The role of insight". Comprehensive Psychiatry. 30 (1): 13–17. doi:10.1016/0010-440X(89)90113-2. PMID 2564330.
- ^ Kane JM, Quitkin F, Rifkin A, Wegner J, Rosenberg G, Borenstein M (April 1983). "Attitudinal changes of involuntarily committed patients following treatment". Archives of General Psychiatry. 40 (4): 374–377. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790040028004. PMID 6838317.
Further reading
- Amador, Xavier Francisco (2000). I am not sick, I don't need help! Helping the seriously mentally ill accept treatment. A practical guide for families and therapists. Vida Press. ISBN 978-0-9677189-0-3.
- Berti A, Bottini G, Gandola M, Pia L, Smania N, Stracciari A, et al. (July 2005). "Shared cortical anatomy for motor awareness and motor control". Science. 309 (5733): 488–491. Bibcode:2005Sci...309..488B. doi:10.1126/science.1110625. PMID 16020740. S2CID 8423262.
- Clare L, Halligan P (2006). "Neuropsychological Rehabilitation.". Pathologies of Awareness: Bridging the Gap between Theory and Practice. Taylor & Francis(Psychology Press). ISBN 978-1-84169-810-6.
- Lysaker P, Bell M, Milstein R, Bryson G, Beam-Goulet J (November 1994). "Insight and psychosocial treatment compliance in schizophrenia". Psychiatry. 57 (4): 307–315. doi:10.1080/00332747.1994.11024695. PMID 7899525.
- Pia L, Neppi-Modona M, Ricci R, Berti A (April 2004). "The anatomy of anosognosia for hemiplegia: a meta-analysis". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 40 (2): 367–377. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70131-X. PMID 15156794. S2CID 4479704.
- Ramachandran, V. S.; Blakeslee, Sandra (1999). Phantoms in the brain: probing the mysteries of the human mind. New York: Quill. ISBN 978-0-688-17217-6.
- Torrey, E. Fuller (2012). The Insanity Offense. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. pp. 111–122. ISBN 978-0-393-34137-9.
- Vuilleumier P (February 2004). "Anosognosia: the neurology of beliefs and uncertainties". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 40 (1): 9–17. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70918-3. PMID 15070000. S2CID 4482597.