Neville Moray
Neville Moray (May 27, 1935 – 15 December 2017) was a British/Canadian academic and Professor at the Department of Psychology of the University of Surrey,[1] known from his 1959 research of the cocktail party effect.
Biography
Moray started studying medicine at Worcester College, Oxford in 1953.[2] He received his BA in Philosophy, Psychology and Physiology in 1957, his MA in Psychology in 1959, and his D.Phil. in 1960 all from Oxford University in the United Kingdom.[1]
Moray started his academic career as Assistant Lecturer in Psychology at the University of Hull in 1959. The next year he moved to the University of Sheffield and became lecturer in Psychology, and senior lecturer in 1966. In 1970 he moved to Canada where he became Associate Professor at the University of Toronto, and Professor of Psychology in 1972. In 1974 he moved back to Scotland to become Professor of Psychology at the University of Stirling and Chairman of Department of Psychology since 1977. In 1981 back at University of Toronto he was appointed Professor of Industrial Engineering, and member of its Institute of Nuclear Engineering since 1984. In 1988 he moved to the United States to the University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, where until 1995 he had a joint appointment as Professor at the Departments of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering; the Department of Psychology; and the Institute of Aviation. From 1995 to 1997 he was a Professor at the University of Valenciennes in France, and from 1997 until his retirement in 2001 he was a Professor of Psychology at University of Surrey in the UK.[2]
Moray was elected Fellow of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society in 1991; Fellow of the Ergonomics Society in 1998; Fellow of the Institute of Ergonomics and Human Factors; and Fellow of the International Ergonomics Association. The International Ergonomics Association awarded him the Ergonomics Development Award and the President's Award in 2000. In 2000, Stirling University inaugurated the annual prize for best Combined Honours Student, to be named the "Neville Moray Prize."[1][2]
He died on 15 December 2017 at the age of 82.[3]
Work
Cocktail party phenomenon
Moray became known for his scientific contributions to the cocktail party effect, which became his major research interest for about two decades.[2] This effect concerns the phenomenon of being able to focus one's auditory attention on a particular stimulus while filtering out a range of other stimuli. The effect was first defined and named "the cocktail party problem" by Colin Cherry in 1953.[4] Cherry found that participants were able to detect their name from the unattended channel, the channel they were not shadowing.[5] Moray build his research using Cherry's shadowing task. He was able to conclude that almost none of the rejected messages were able to penetrate the block set up, except subjectively "important" messages.[5]
Neville Moray used Cherry's shadowed dichotic listening task in his 1959 research and was able to conclude that almost none of the rejected messages were able to penetrate the block set up, except subjectively "important" messages.[6] Personal names, taboo language, and backward language are the "subjectively" important messages that have been found to date. Moray's 1959 study found a 33% detection rate for personal names, which revealed that participants sometimes notice their name in an ignored auditory channel.[7] This ability to selectively attend to one's own name has been found in infants as young as five months of age and appears to be fully developed by thirteen months of age.
Rochelle S. Newman in a 2005 study found that five-month-old infants listened longer to their names when the target voice was 10 dB, but not 5 dB more intense than the background noise. Nine-month-olds also failed at 5 dB, but thirteen-month-olds succeeded.[8] This success in recognizing one's own name in the unattended channel can be explained using Cherry's initial report on dichotic shadowing. Cherry found that the verbal content of the message in the unattended channel was completely blocked, so that the words were treated as merely sounds.[9] This allows the subject to know that something has stimulated the ear whose message is rejected. It may be thought of as a general warning signal, that a sound has occurred to which the subject might need to respond.[6]
Selected publications
- Neville Moray, Cybernetics: Machines with Intelligence, Burns Oates, London, 1963.
- Neville Moray, Attention: Selective processes in vision and hearing. New York: Academic Press, 1970.
- Neville Moray (ed.) Mental workload: Its theory and measurement. Vol. 8. Plenum Publishing Corporation, 1979.
- Neville Moray, Ergonomics: The history and scope of human factors, Routledge, 2005.
Articles, a selection:
- Moray, Neville (1959). "Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions" (PDF). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 11 (1): 56–60. doi:10.1080/17470215908416289.[dead link]
- Stassen, Henk G.; Johannsen, Gunnar; Moray, Neville (1990). "Internal representation, internal model, human performance model and mental workload". Automatica. 26 (4): 811–820. doi:10.1016/0005-1098(90)90057-o.
- Lee, John; Moray, Neville (1992). "Trust, control strategies and allocation of function in human-machine systems" (PDF). Ergonomics. 35 (10): 1243–1270. doi:10.1080/00140139208967392. PMID 1516577.
References
- ^ a b c Neville Moray Curriculum vitae Archived 2014-03-12 at the Wayback Machine at moraysatmagagnosc.com. Accessed March 12, 2014.
- ^ a b c d Neville Moray Archived 2015-09-24 at the Wayback Machine Fellow Profile at hfes.org. Accessed March 12, 2014.
- ^ Moray
- ^ Cherry, E. Colin (1953). "Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 25 (5): 975–79. Bibcode:1953ASAJ...25..975C. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. ISSN 0001-4966.
- ^ a b Moray, Neville (1959). "Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions" (PDF). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 11 (1): 56–60. doi:10.1080/17470215908416289. ISSN 0033-555X.[dead link]
- ^ a b Moray, Neville (1959). "Attention in Dichotic Listening: Affective Cues and the Influence of Instructions". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 11: 1156–11560. doi:10.1080/17470215908416289.
- ^ Wood, Noelle; Cowan, Nelson (1995). "The Cocktail Party Phenomenon Revisited: How Frequent Are Attention Shifts to One's Name in an Irrelevant Auditory Channel?". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 21 (1): 255–260. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.255. PMID 7876773.
- ^ Newman, Rochelle S (2005). "The Cocktail Party Effect in Infants Revisited: Listening to One's Name in Noise". Developmental Psychology. 41 (2): 352–362. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.352. PMID 15769191.
- ^ Cherry, Colin E (1953). "Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 25 (5): 975–979. Bibcode:1953ASAJ...25..975C. doi:10.1121/1.1907229.
External links
- Google Scholar Profile
- Neville Moray homepage
- Neville Moray Fellow Profile at hfes.org