Nonverbal autism
This article needs more reliable medical references for verification or relies too heavily on primary sources. (June 2023) |
Nonverbal autism, also called nonspeaking autism, is a subset of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) where the person does not learn how to speak.[1][failed verification]
Background
[edit]Early intervention in nonspeaking autism emphasizes the critical role of language acquisition before the age of five in predicting positive developmental outcomes; acquiring language before age five is a good indicator of positive child development, that early language development is crucial to educational achievement, employment, independence during adulthood, and social relationships.[2] The likelihood of acquiring functional language in the future past this age is minimal.[3]
The biological basis of autism is unknown. However, the most obvious signs of autism, such as atypical social and language development, and restricted or repetitive behaviors and interests, often present themselves between the ages of two and three, and most children with ASD can be diagnosed in early childhood as a result.[4]: 63 Other disorders such as epilepsy, ADHD, gastrointestinal problems, sleep disorders, depression and anxiety often accompany ASD.[4]: 67–68 [5]
Early predictors
[edit]The causes of nonspeaking autism are unknown. However, there appears to be a relationship between joint attention and verbal communication. Joint attention occurs between two individuals when one draws the other's attention to an object through gesturing (i.e. eye gazing, pointing).[6] The ability to achieve joint attention at an early age plays a significant role in language development, and studies indicate severe lapses in joint attention in autistic children.[6] Research findings indicate that young children diagnosed with autism exhibit challenges in both initiating and responding to joint attention.[7] Notably, while the difficulties in responding to joint attention may improve with development, it has been observed that autistic children who have higher IQs and lower mental ages may display distinct joint attention deficits.[8] These deficits in joint attention are observed consistently across various age groups, spanning from infancy to adolescence within the autism spectrum. Moreover, there is a concurrent relationship between joint attention skills and language development, with joint attention abilities serving as predictors for long-term gains in expressive language among individuals with autism.[9] In one study, researchers suggest that a displayed pattern of delays, absences, or a general impaired response to stimuli (hyporesponsiveness) and a fascination with intense or repetitive stimulation (sensory seeking) is more likely in nonspeaking autistic children, suggesting that both hyporesponsiveness and sensory seeking is related to poor communication outcomes in children with ASD.[10]
Potential causes
[edit]The amygdala theory
[edit]There is a growing body of tentative evidence indicating the amygdala's involvement in the development of autism. The amygdala theory of autism focuses on the importance of the amygdala in relation to social functioning and observes that autism is largely a severe impairment of social functioning. The amygdala is thought to be associated with the fight-or-flight response in animals and its activity is heavily correlated with fear in humans. Additionally, it has been heavily implicated in relation to social functioning in various animal studies. Evidence suggests an amygdala hyperactivity model may be more accurate than one comparing it to a lesion.[11]
Lesion studies have shown that amygdala damage results in severe social impairment among animal models. Vervet monkey mothers with amygdala lesions were shown to be much less caring with their young, neglecting and even abusing them.[12] Rats with amygdala ablations become much more docile.[12] Monkeys with lesions to the anterior temporal lobe develop a disorder known as Klüver–Bucy syndrome, characterized by loss of fear, hypersexuality, hyperorality, and an inability to recognize visual objects (often, but not always).[12]
Evidence shows the amygdala accounts for the emotional, oral, and sexual abnormalities listed above.[12] These abnormalities coincide with several characteristics of the diagnostic guidelines for autism, at least passably for an animal model.
Post-mortem analysis of humans shows an increased neuronal density in the amygdala in autism compared to controls, indicating a potential linkage and supporting the hyperactivity model.[12]
Several studies presented subjects with ASD photographs of human eyes and had them report the emotional state of the person in the picture. A smaller amygdala was associated with increased response time but not decreased accuracy.[12] There was also significantly less amygdaloid activation in the brains of those with ASD than controls. Subjects compensated for this lack of amygdaloid activity with increased activation in the temporal lobe, and are associated with verbally labeling images.[11] This activity is thought to imply less usage of emotional/social cues to identify objects and rather more objective, factually based processing. One may extrapolate from this model that patients with autism may learn that a specific facial configuration represents an emotional state and what that emotional state implies socially, but they may not come to truly understand how that person feels. This supports a theory of mind deficit.
The left amygdala[13] is critical in the involvement of processing mental state and emotional information from complex visual stimuli, particularly the eye region. However, individuals with autism do not seem to engage the amygdala during tasks when processing the emotions of others. Instead, they exhibit greater reliance on temporal lobe structures, specialized for verbally labeling complex visual stimuli and processing faces and eyes. This shift in processing may serve as compensation for potential amygdala abnormalities in individuals with autism.[13]
Studies conducted specifically on nonspeaking autistic individuals[which?] provide similar evidence. Brain studies have shown several amygdaloid impairments among those with ASD. The amygdala in those with nonspeaking autism have less volume compared to controls, contain a higher density of neurons suggesting hyperconnection, and show a negative correlation between amygdala size and impairment severity among subjects.[11]
While infantile autism is actually associated with an oversized amygdala, there are developmental theories as to how this may occur. Research on major depressive disorder has shown that excessive activation such as stress or fear leads to allostasis, or degeneration of the neurons involved in creating the phenomenon. Initial hypertrophy results in atrophy and reduction of brain size in the given region.[11] Over time, this occurs in patients with severe depression, and they develop a decreased amygdala size. Some scientists[who?] theorize that this is happening early during infancy in the autistic brain, accounting for the initial overgrowth and later observed size reduction.[11]
When eye tracking software is employed to record where subjects focus their visual attention on images of human faces, small amygdala volume is associated with decreased eye fixation.[11] Eyes are considered to be especially important for establishing human connection and conveying emotion, thus fixation on them is considered to be a crucial part of identifying people and emotions in a social setting.
In addition to a negative correlation to eye fixation studies showed a smaller amygdala was associated with impairment in nonverbal communication skills as well.[11] This suggests that the amygdala is critical in developing all types of communicative abilities, not just verbal. This suggests the amygdala may play a crucial role in relating to other humans in a way that allows for behavioral mimicry.
Among nonspeaking autistic patients, researchers could predict symptom severity based on amygdala activity. Those with the least amygdala activity had the most impaired nonspeaking communication abilities, those with the most activity had the strongest communication abilities.[11]
The development of language, similar to the development of most physical skills, relies heavily on mimicry of other humans.[11] ASDs are known to impair one's ability to focus on and relate with people possibly as a result of a damaged amygdala. Nonspeaking autistics will often be able to learn more basic communicative skills such as pointing to objects or selecting a picture from a list. These skills are far more simple and do not require the degree of personal connection needed for language development.
Language outcomes
[edit]When considering language abilities in autism, two main theories come into play: the theory of mind and the procedural deficit hypothesis (PDH). Both theories share a common approach, aiming to understand the connections between language and non-language domains and explore whether similar behavioral profiles can be elucidated by common neurocognitive substrates.[14] The theory of mind in autism provides an explanation for pragmatic impairments in language and communication, attributing them to social deficits and their underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. In contrast, the PDH suggests that grammatical impairments, encompassing syntax, morphology, and phonology, in individuals with autism can be predominantly attributed to abnormalities in the procedural memory system. Meanwhile, lexical knowledge, which relies on the declarative memory system, remains relatively unaffected.[15]
For nonspeaking autistic children and adolescents, augmentative and alternative communication and interventions with speech-language pathologists may be implemented to enhance language and communication outcomes. They are often given dedicated speech-generating devices that allow them to select icons to indicate requests or needs.[16] Continued interventions can be used to teach them more advanced operations on these devices (i.e. turning on the device, scrolling through pages) or add additional vocabulary, helping them enhance their communicative abilities independently.[16]
Nonspeaking autistic adolescents and adults may transition away from dedicated speech-generating devices, instead opting to use apps on their personal mobile devices, like Spoken or Avaz.
The Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) is a form of spontaneous communication for children with autism in which an individual selects a picture indicating a request.[17] PECS can be utilized in educational settings and at the child's home. Longitudinal study suggest PECS can have long-term positive outcomes for school-aged children with nonspeaking autism, specifically their social-communicative skills, such as higher frequencies of joint attention and initiation, and duration of cooperative play,[17] which are all important roles in improving language outcomes.
It has been suggested that a significant stage in acquiring verbal language is learning how to identify and reproduce syllables of words. One study found that nonspeaking and minimally speaking autistic children are capable of enhancing their oral production and vocalizing written words by isolating each syllable of a word one at a time.[3] The process of breaking down a syllable at a time and having it visually displayed and audibly available to the child can prompt them to imitate and create nonrandom and meaningful utterances.[3]
Most of these studies contain small sample sizes and were pilot studies, making additional research significant to assess whether these findings can be generalized to all age groups of the same population. Furthermore, most studies on nonspeaking autism and communication with speech-generating devices were based on more basic skills, such as naming pictures and making requests for stimuli, while studies in advanced communication are limited.[18]
See also
[edit]- Augmentative and alternative communication
- Facilitated communication: a controversial AAC technique that purports to help nonverbal people communicate
- Low-functioning autism: verbal abilities may be lacking
- Language delay and speech delay
- Late talkers: sometimes mistaken as nonverbal autistics
- Nonverbal communication
References
[edit]- ^ Baghdadli, Amaria; Assouline, Brigitte; Sonié, Sandrine; Pernon, Eric; Darrou, Céline; Michelon, Cécile; Picot, Marie-Christine; Aussilloux, Charles; Pry, René (2012-07-01). "Developmental Trajectories of Adaptive Behaviors from Early Childhood to Adolescence in a Cohort of 152 Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 42 (7): 1314–1325. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1357-z. ISSN 1573-3432. PMID 21928042. Retrieved 2024-03-24.
- ^ Mayo, Jessica; Chlebowski, Colby; Fein, Deborah A.; Eigsti, Inge-Marie (February 2013). "Age of First Words Predicts Cognitive Ability and Adaptive Skills in Children with ASD". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 43 (2): 253–264. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1558-0. ISSN 0162-3257. PMC 4386060. PMID 22673858.
- ^ a b c Vernay, Frédérique; Kahina, Harma; Thierry, Marrone; Jean-Yves, Roussey (2017). "Self-paced segmentation of written words on a touchscreen tablet promotes the oral production of nonverbal and minimally verbal autistic children". Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs. 17 (4): 265–73. doi:10.1111/1471-3802.12384.
- ^ a b American Psychiatric Association (2022). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787. ISBN 978-0-89042-575-6. S2CID 249488050.
- ^ Furfaro, Hannah (2018-07-25). "Conditions that accompany autism, explained". Spectrum. Simons Foundation. doi:10.53053/PCFG3602. S2CID 252300878.
- ^ a b Paparella, Tanya; Goods, Kelly Stickles; Freeman, Stephanny; Kasari, Connie (2011). "The emergence of nonverbal joint attention and requesting skills in young autistic children". Journal of Communication Disorders. 44 (6): 569–83. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.08.002. PMID 21907346.
- ^ Chiang, Chung-Hsin; Soong, Wei-Tsuen; Lin, Tzu-Ling; Rogers, Sally (20 May 2008). "Nonverbal Communication Skills in Young Children with Autism". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 38 (10): 1898–1906. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0586-2. PMC 4951089. PMID 18491223.
- ^ Chiang, Chung-Hsin; Soong, Wei-Tsuen; Lin, Tzu-Ling; Rogers, Sally (20 May 2008). "Nonverbal Communication Skills in Young Children with Autism". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 38 (10): 1898–1906. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0586-2. PMC 4951089. PMID 18491223.
- ^ Chiang, Chung-Hsin; Soong, Wei-Tsuen; Lin, Tzu-Ling; Rogers, Sally (20 May 2008). "Nonverbal Communication Skills in Young Children with Autism". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 38 (10): 1898–1906. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0586-2. PMC 4951089. PMID 18491223.
- ^ Patten, Elena; Ausderau, Karla K; Watson, Linda R; Baranek, Grace T (2013). "Sensory Response Patterns in Nonverbal Children with ASD". Autism Research and Treatment. 2013: 436286. doi:10.1155/2013/436286. PMC 3727194. PMID 23956859.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Nacewicz, Brendon M; Dalton, Kim M; Johnstone, Tom; Long, Micah T; McAuliff, Emelia M; Oakes, Terrence R; Alexander, Andrew L; Davidson, Richard J (2006). "Amygdala Volume and Nonverbal Social Impairment in Adolescent and Adult Males with Autism". Archives of General Psychiatry. 63 (12): 1417–1428. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1417. PMC 4767012. PMID 17146016.
- ^ a b c d e f Baron-Cohen, S; Ring, H.A; Bullmore, E.T; Wheelwright, S; Ashwin, C; Williams, S.C.R (2000). "The amygdala theory of autism". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 24 (3): 355–64. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00011-7. PMID 10781695. S2CID 7455984.
- ^ a b Baron-Cohen, S; Ring, H.A.; Bullmore, E.T.; Wheelwright, S; Ashwin, C; Williams, S.C.R. (May 2000). "The amygdala theory of autism". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 24 (3): 355–364. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00011-7. PMID 10781695. S2CID 7455984.
- ^ Walenski, Matthew; Tager-Flusberg, Helen; Ullman, Michael (2008). "Language in Autism". APA PsycNet.
- ^ Walenski, Matthew; Tager-Flusberg, Helen; Ullman, Michael (2008). "Language in Autism". APA PsycNet.
- ^ a b Achmadi, Donna; Kagohara, Debora M; Van Der Meer, Larah; o'Reilly, Mark F; Lancioni, Giulio E; Sutherland, Dean; Lang, Russell; Marschik, Peter B; Green, Vanessa A; Sigafoos, Jeff (2012). "Teaching advanced operation of an iPod-based speech-generating device to two students with autism spectrum disorders". Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 6 (4): 1258–64. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.05.005.
- ^ a b Lerna, Anna; Esposito, Dalila; Conson, Massimiliano; Massagli, Angelo (2014). "Long-term effects of PECS on social-communicative skills of autistic children: A follow-up study". International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 49 (4): 478–85. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12079. PMID 24655345.
- ^ Kagohara, Debora M; Van Der Meer, Larah; Ramdoss, Sathiyaprakash; O'Reilly, Mark F; Lancioni, Giulio E; Davis, Tonya N; Rispoli, Mandy; Lang, Russell; Marschik, Peter B; Sutherland, Dean; Green, Vanessa A; Sigafoos, Jeff (2013). "Using iPods® and iPads® in teaching programs for individuals with developmental disabilities: A systematic review". Research in Developmental Disabilities. 34 (1): 147–56. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.07.027. PMID 22940168.