User:JuliaLeary/Home sign
Home sign
[edit]Home sign (or kitchen sign) is a gestural communication system, often invented spontaneously by a deaf child who lacks accessible linguistic input[1]. Home sign systems often arise in families where a deaf child is raised by hearing parents and is isolated from the Deaf community. Because the deaf child does not receive signed or spoken language input, these children are referred to as linguistic isolates .[2][3]
Because home sign systems are used regularly as the child's form of communication, they develop to become more complex than simple gestures[4]. Though not considered to be a complete language, these systems may be classified as linguistic phenomena that show similar characteristics to signed and spoken language. Home sign systems display significant degrees of internal complexity, using gestures with consistent meanings, word order, and grammatical categories. Linguists have been interested in home sign systems as insight into the the human ability to generate, acquire, and process language.[3][5]
Identifying Features
[edit]In 1987, Nancy Frishberg set out a framework for identifying and describing home-based sign systems. She states that home signs differ from sign languages in that they:[6]
- do not have a consistent meaning-symbol relationship,
- do not pass on from generation to generation,
- are not shared by one large group,
- and are not considered the same over a community of signers.
However, there are certain "resilient" properties of language whose development can proceed without guidance of a conventional language model. More recent studies of deaf children's gestural systems show systematicity and productivity[7]. Across users, these systems tend to exhibit a stable lexicon, word order tendency, complex sentence usage, and noun-verb pairs. Gesture systems have also been shown to have the property of recursion, which allows system to be generative. Deaf children may borrow spoken language gestures, but these gestures are altered to serve as linguistic markers. As the child develops, their utterances grow in size and complexity. Adult home signers use systems that mature to display more linguistic features than the simpler systems used by child home signers.[1][8]
Lexicon
[edit]Studies of home signing children and adults show consistent pairing between the form of a gesture token and its meaning. These signs are also combined in compound gestures to create new words. [1]The lack of bidirectionality in creation of home sign systems between the parent and child restricts the invention of signs with arbitrary meanings. The emergence of a conventionalized lexicon proceeds slower in a home sign system than in natural languages with a richer social network. [9][10] Study of adult home signers in Nicaragua show that home signers use gesture to communicate about number, with cardinal number and non-cardinal number markings. [11]
Morphology
[edit]Home sign systems have simple morphology. Gestures are composed of parts with a limited set of handshape forms. [8] Study of adolescent home signers show ability to express motion events, though this strategy differs from conventional sign language [12]. Morphophonological patterns in handshape production are more similar to conventionalized sign language handshapes than hearing individuals’ gestures. These handshapes are high in finger complexity for object handshapes and low in finger complexity for handling handshapes [13]. Home signers also use handshape as a productive morphological marker in predicates, displaying a distinction between nominals and predicates. [14]
Syntax
[edit]Within an individual system, home signers show consistency in a particular word order that distinguishes the subject of the utterance. Across home sign systems there is preference for action to be utterance-final. Structural dependency, words grouped based on a hierarchical structure or pattern, has been studied in Brazilian home signers who consistently produce modifiers with the noun modified. Gestural markers for negation (side to side head shake) and wh-form questions (manual flip) show consistent meaning, use, and position.[1] [15] Home signers mark grammatical subjects in sentences and are able to distinguish the subject from the topic of the sentence [16] These systems show some evidence of a prosodic system for marking phrase and utterance boundaries.[17]
Narratives
[edit]Home signing children vary greatly in how often they display narrative skills; however, their narratives show similar structural patterns[18]. This includes elaborating on basic narrative by including setting, actions, a complication, and temporal order. Hearing mothers produce co-narration with deaf children less frequently (than hearing mothers do with hearing children), and these contributions are spoken and rarely gestural.
Conditions for Emergence
[edit]The context of home sign system creation includes [1]:
- limited or no exposure to a spoken or signed language model
- isolation from Deaf children and adults
- parental choices regarding communication with the deaf child
Home sign creation is a common experience of deaf children in hearing families, as approximately 75% of hearing parents do not sign and communicate with their deaf children using a small set of gestures, speaking, and lipreading [1]. In a home with parents who are Deaf or know sign language, a child can pick up the sign language in the same way a hearing child can pick up spoken language.[4]
Home signs are a starting point for many sign languages. When a group of deaf people come together without a common sign language, they may share features of their individual home sign systems creating a village sign language that may establish itself as a complete language over time. However, home signs are rarely passed on to more than one generation, because they generally fade when the deaf child is exposed to language outside of the home. [4]
Deaf children who use home sign are distinguished from feral children who are deprived of meaningful social and linguistic interaction. Home signing children are socially integrated to an extent with lack of conventional linguistic interaction. Home sign systems have some elements of language, and children who use these systems are able to acquire a natural sign language later in school. [9]
Creator of a Home sign System
[edit]The deaf child is the creator of a home sign system. Mothers of adult home signers in Nicaragua were evaluated to determine their role in the development of their child's home sign system. The results of this study concluded that mothers comprehended spoken Spanish descriptions of events better than home sign descriptions, and native ASL signers performed better than mothers at understanding home sign productions. This suggests that mothers do not directly transmit home sign systems to their deaf children. Though caregivers' co-speech gestures may serve as an initial foundation for their child's home sign system, children surpass this input. Hearing caregivers typically do not share the same gestural communication system with the deaf child, using fewer gestures with less consistency and displaying different sentence-level patterns. A deaf child's gestural system is more likely to overlap with that of another home signer, including cross culturally.[19][20]
Social network structure influences the development of a home sign system, impacting the conventionalization of referring expressions among members. Richly connected networks, where all participants interact with one another using the communication system, show greater and faster conventionalization. Home sign systems are typically sparsely connected networks, where the home signer communicates with each member of the network but the members do not use home sign to communicate with each other.[21]
Impact of Lacking a Language Model
[edit]Recent studies by Deanna Gagne and Marie Coppola of perspective taking abilities in adult home signers, reveal that home signers do not pass experimental false belief tasks despite having visual observation of social interaction. False belief understanding, integral to the development of theory of mind, requires language experience and linguistic input. Further study of these adult home signers indicates that home signers show precursor abilities for theory of mind, such as visual perspective taking. [22][23]
Lack of conventional language for numbers has been shown to affect numerical ability. In comparison to unschooled hearing and signing deaf individuals, adult home signers do not consistently produce gestures that accurately represent cardinal values of larger sets and do not may effective use of finger counting strategies.[24] Further study indicates home signers are able to recall gestures used as nouns, verbs, and adjectives, but they show poor number recall, which worsens as number increases. [25]
Cross-cultural Comparisons
[edit]Syntactic structure is similar between groups of home signers in different cultures and geographical regions, including word order preferences and complex sentence usage. For example, home sign systems of children in Turkey and America exhibit similar patterns in sentence-level structure. [20]
Certain gestures, such as pointing, head shaking, and shrugging, share similar meanings throughout cultures. Young children shake their heads to indicate negation before they express negative meanings through language. However, most young children use the head shake as an initial marker of negation, and replace it with speech or manual signs once language is acquired. Children using a home sign system don't have exposure to a structured language, and therefore don't replace the head shake with manual signs until language is acquired[15].
Home sign systems differ across cultures in terms of gesture use by hearing caregivers. Compared to American mothers, Chinese mothers show more similarity in gesture form (handshape and motion) and syntax with systems used by their deaf children. In comparing narratives from Chinese and American deaf children, home signing children produce culturally appropriate narratives. Variability between home signers are group internal, with different individual home signers having their own set of gestures for the same type of object or predicate. [1][18]
Examples of Home sign Systems
[edit]- Rennellese Sign Language of the Solomon Islands was used by a single deaf individual and his friends and family, and ended with his death.[26]
- Following the establishment of the first deaf schools in Nicaragua in the 1970s, previously isolated deaf children quickly developed their own sign language, now known as Nicaraguan Sign Language, from the building blocks of their own diverse home sign systems[27].
- For several reported sign languages, it's not clear if they are a collection of home sign used by various families, with similarities due to the common system of gestures used by the hearing population augmented by mime, or an incipient coherent sign language. Such systems include Marajo Sign Language in Brazil, described primarily between deaf mothers and their young children[28], Maxakali Sign Language, also in Brazil, which is at the least is a very young language[29], and Mehek Sign Language in northwestern Papua New Guinea, in which signs are quite variable between families, with only a few dozen held in common, all of which are highly mimetic[30].
- Navajo family sign, distinct from the hearing Navajo way of signing, is more linguistically complex than general home sign systems where children are isolated from Deaf peers. Navajo family sign was used inter-generationally among deaf family members and were not replaced when the deaf children were exposed to standard sign language. [31][32]
- According to Anne Sullivan, Helen Keller had developed over sixty home signs long before she was taught to communicate through finger spelling.[33]
Prominent studies
[edit]It is difficult to conduct studies about home sign, because these language systems emerge when an individual has no source of traditional linguistic input, and often vanish when children adopt a conventional sign language in school. These conditions of language deprivation cannot be controlled for a study, but sometimes occur naturally. [34]
- Susan Goldin-Meadow has published a number of articles on homesign systems. She found that the homesign gestures of American deaf children are not acquired from modeling the gestures of their hearing parents — they more closely resemble the gestures of the Chinese deaf children halfway across the globe. They are structured communication systems that include gestures that function as words, which are combined to form sentences, and are used to describe situations beyond the here-and-now.
- Adam Kendon published a celebrated study of the signing system of a deaf Enga woman from the Papua New Guinea highlands, in which he investigated the notion of iconicity in language and gesture. This Enga Sign Language is used by a range of hearing and deaf individuals.
- A study conducted by Coppola & Newport in 2005 examined gesture systems developed by three isolated deaf Nicaraguans, and aimed to examine universal characteristics of gesture production.[35] This study was conducted in Nicaragua because it is difficult to find deaf adults in the US who have had no contact with any sign languages or manual codes, no interaction with a Deaf community, and no exposure to a spoken or written language. The homesigners studied in Nicaragua who had no prior linguistic input were able to develop a homesign system that consistently uses word order and spatial devices. Certain fundamental characteristics of human language systems are used in gestural communication, even if the user has never been exposed a natural language.
- A study conducted by Torigoe & Takei in 2002 analyzed pointing and oral movements in a homesign system.[36] This study focused on two deaf sisters in Japan who had no access to conventional sign languages during childhood, and no contact with a Deaf community since then. Their homesigns developed into their own communication system with devices that enable complex forms comparable to language. These signs consisted of manual and non-manual gestures. About one third of the manual signs were pointing, and the majority of non-manual markers consisted of oral movements.
I moved this here for now I dont know how to work it into the morphology paragraph without overwhelming "David". I did incorporate ability to use verbs of motion from another source. Thinking this could be shortened and added to "examples of home sign systems".
In David's homesign system, there were five handshapes (fist, O-shape, C-shape, flat palm, and pointing) that accounted for 98% of all handshapes that David produced. These hand shapes were used in two ways: to represent a hand as it manipulates an object, or to represent the object itself[7].
David also used eight different types of motions in his signs, as well as a still non-motion form, that accounted for 100% of the signs produced. These motions are defined in terms of the trajectory traced by the hand (linear path, arced path, circle), or the motions of the hand in place (revolving, open/close, bend, wiggle). These motions also vary in length of path and directionality. Most of the hand shape morphemes could be found in combination with more than one motion morpheme, and vice versa[7].
- Idioglossia
- Language Isolate
- Nonverbal communication
- Language deprivation in deaf and hard of hearing children
References
[edit]- ^ a b c d e f g Hill, Joseph C.; Lillo-Martin, Diane C.; Wood, Sandra K. (2018-12-12), "Homesign systems", Sign Languages, Routledge, pp. 117–133, ISBN 978-0-429-02087-2, retrieved 2020-03-22
- ^ Torigoe, Takashi; Takei, Wataru (2002). "A Descriptive Analysis of Pointing and Oral Movements in a Home Sign System". Sign Language Studies. 2 (3): 281–295. doi:10.1353/sls.2002.0013. ISSN 1533-6263.
- ^ a b Hoff, Erika, 1951-. Language development (Fifth edition ed.). Belmont, CA. ISBN 978-1-133-93909-2. OCLC 843489860.
{{cite book}}
:|edition=
has extra text (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c Walker, J. "Home Signs". www.signedlanguage.co.uk. Retrieved 2020-03-23.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ Begby, Endre (2016-08-17). "Language from the Ground Up: A Study of Homesign Communication". Erkenntnis. 82 (3): 693–714. doi:10.1007/s10670-016-9839-1. ISSN 0165-0106.
- ^ &NA; (1987). "Gallaudet Encyclopedia of Deaf People and Deafness (3 vols.)". Ear and Hearing. 8 (6): 355. doi:10.1097/00003446-198712000-00022. ISSN 0196-0202.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c Mylander, Carolyn; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (1991). "Home Sign Systems in Deaf Children: The Development of Morphology without a Conventional Language Model". In Siple, P.; Fischer, S. D. (eds.). Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. pp. 41–63.
- ^ a b Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2005-04-05). "The Resilience of Language". doi:10.4324/9780203943267.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ a b Begby, Endre (2016-08-17). "Language from the Ground Up: A Study of Homesign Communication". Erkenntnis. 82 (3): 693–714. doi:10.1007/s10670-016-9839-1. ISSN 0165-0106.
- ^ Richie, Russell; Yang, Charles; Coppola, Marie (2014). "Modeling the Emergence of Lexicons in Homesign Systems". Topics in Cognitive Science. 6 (1): 183–195. doi:10.1111/tops.12076. ISSN 1756-8765. PMC 3909872. PMID 24482343.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Coppola, Marie; Spaepen, Elizabet; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2013-08-01). "Communicating about quantity without a language model: Number devices in homesign grammar". Cognitive Psychology. 67 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.05.003. ISSN 0010-0285. PMC 3870334. PMID 23872365.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Morford, Jill P. (2002-01-01). "The expression of motion events in homesign". Sign Language & Linguistics. 5 (1): 55–71. doi:10.1075/sll.5.1.05mor. ISSN 1387-9316.
- ^ Brentari, Diane; Coppola, Marie; Mazzoni, Laura; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2012). "When does a system become phonological? Handshape production in gesturers, signers, and homesigners". Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. 30 (1) (published 2011): 1–31. doi:10.1007/s11049-011-9145-1. ISSN 0167-806X. PMC 3665423. PMID 23723534.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Goldin-Meadow, S.; Brentari, D.; Coppola, M.; Horton, L.; Senghas, A. (2015). "Watching language grow in the manual modality: Nominals, predicates, and handshapes". Cognition. 136: 381–395. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.029. PMC 4308574. PMID 25546342.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ a b Franklin, Amy; Giannakidou, Anastasia; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2011). "Negation, questions, and structure building in a homesign system". Cognition. 118 (3): 398–416. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.017. PMC 3658158. PMID 23630971.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Coppola, Marie; Newport, Elissa L. (2005-12-27). "Grammatical Subjects in home sign: Abstract linguistic structure in adult primary gesture systems without linguistic input". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (52): 19249–19253. doi:10.1073/pnas.0509306102. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 1315276. PMID 16357199.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Applebaum, Lauren; Coppola, Marie; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2014). "Prosody in a communication system developed without a language model". Sign language and linguistics. 17 (2): 181–212. doi:10.1075/sll.17.2.02app. ISSN 1387-9316. PMC 4285364. PMID 25574153.
- ^ a b Van Deusen-Phillips, Sarah B.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Miller, Peggy J. (2001). "Enacting Stories, Seeing Worlds: Similarities and Differences in the Cross-Cultural Narrative Development of Linguistically Isolated Deaf Children". Human Development. 44 (6): 311–336. doi:10.1159/000046153. ISSN 1423-0054.
- ^ Carrigan, Emily; Coppola, Marie (2012). "Mothers Do Not Drive Structure in Adult Homesign Systems: Evidence from Comprehension". Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. 34 (34). ISSN 1069-7977.
- ^ a b Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Namboodiripad, Savithry; Mylander, Carolyn; Özyürek, Aslı; Sancar, Burcu (2015). "The Resilience of Structure Built Around the Predicate: Homesign Gesture Systems in Turkish and American Deaf Children". Journal of Cognition and Development. 16 (1): 55–80. doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.803970. ISSN 1524-8372. PMC 4316383. PMID 25663828.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Richie, Russell; Hall, Matthew L.; Cho, Pyeong Whan; Coppola, Marie (2020). "Converging evidence: Network structure effects on conventionalization of gestural referring expressions". Language Dynamics and Change.
- ^ Gagne, Deanna L.; Coppola, Marie (2017). "Visible Social Interactions Do Not Support the Development of False Belief Understanding in the Absence of Linguistic Input: Evidence from Deaf Adult Homesigners". Frontiers in Psychology. 8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00837. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5454053. PMID 28626432.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Gagne, Deanna; Goico, Sara; Pyers, Jennie; Coppola, Marie (2019). "False Belief Understanding Requires Language Experience, but Its Precursor Abilities Do Not" (PDF). Proceedings of the 43rd Boston University Conference on Language Development. Cascadilla Press (published November 2018): 256–269.
- ^ Spaepen, Elizabet; Coppola, Marie; Spelke, Elizabeth S.; Carey, Susan E.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2011-02-22). "Number without a language model". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (8): 3163–3168. doi:10.1073/pnas.1015975108. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3044352. PMID 21300893.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Spaepen, Elizabet; Coppola, Marie; Flaherty, Molly; Spelke, Elizabeth; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2013-11-01). "Generating a lexicon without a language model: Do words for number count?". Journal of Memory and Language. 69 (4): 496–505. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.05.004. ISSN 0749-596X. PMC 3811965. PMID 24187432.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) - ^ Kuschel, Rolf. (1974). A lexicon of signs from a Polynesian outliner island : a description of 217 signs as developed and used by Kagobai, the only deaf-mute of Rennell Island. København: Universitet, Psykologisk Laboratorium (Akademisk Forlag) : eksp., DBK. ISBN 978-87-500-1506-2. OCLC 3088982.
- ^ Meir, Irit; Sandler, Wendy; Padden, Carol; Aronoff, Mark (2010). "Emerging sign languages". Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education (PDF). Vol. 2. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-539003-2. OCLC 607613491.
- ^ Carliez, Maria Luizete Sampaio Sobral; Fusellier, Ivani (2016-09-02). "Collecte des langues des signes des sourds de Soure (Île de Marajó): un parcours méthodologique (2008/2013), les enjeux sociaux et politiques de la non reconnaissance des langues des signes émergentes pratiquées par ces sourds". MOARA – Revista Eletrônica do Programa de Pós-Graduação em Letras ISSN: 0104-0944. 1 (45): 129. doi:10.18542/moara.v1i45.3712.
- ^ Stoianov, Diane|Nevins. "The phonology of handshape distribution in Maxakalí sign". lfab.14.c14. Retrieved 2020-03-23.
- ^ Hatfield, Adam (2016). "A GRAMMAR OF MEHEK" (PDF).
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ LeMaster, Barbara (1997). "Ceil Lucas (ed.), Sociolinguistics in Deaf communities. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press, 1995. Pp. x, 341. Hb $39.95". Language in Society. 26 (3): 433–436. doi:10.1017/s0047404500019576. ISSN 0047-4045.
- ^ Davis, Jeffrey Edward (2010). Hand talk : sign language among American Indian nations. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-87010-8. OCLC 535491235.
- ^ "The Story of My Life". digital.library.upenn.edu. Retrieved 2020-03-23.
- ^ Coppola, M. (2002). The emergence of grammatical categories in homesign: Evidence from family-based gesture systems in Nicaragua. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Rochester, Rochester.
- ^ Coppola, Marie; Newport, Elissa (2005). "Grammatical Subjects in home sign: Abstract linguistic structure in adult primary gesture systems without linguistic input". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102(52): 19249–19253.
- ^ Torigoe, Takashi; Takei, Wataru (2002). "A Descriptive Analysis of Pointing and Oral Movements in a Home Sign System". Sign Language Studies. 2 (3): 281–295. doi:10.1353/sls.2002.0013. ISSN 1533-6263.