Echoic memory: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
"comprised of"
reformatted refs; stripped before running citation bot
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Echoic memory'''
'''Echoic memory'''
is one of the [[sensory memory]] registers; a component of [[short term memory]] (STM) that is specific to retaining auditory information. This particular sensory store is capable of storing large amounts of auditory information that is only retained for a short period of time (3-4 seconds). This echoic sound resonates in the mind and is replayed for this brief amount of time shortly after the presentation of auditory stimuli<ref name="Radvansky"> {{cite book | last1 = Radvansky | first1 = Gabriel | title = Human Memory | chapter = 4 | editors = Susan Hartman | publisher = Pearson Education Group | year = 2006 | location = Boston, Massachusetts | pages = 65-75 | url = www.ablongman.com}}</ref>.
is one of the [[sensory memory]] registers; a component of [[short term memory]] (STM) that is specific to retaining auditory information. This particular sensory store is capable of storing large amounts of auditory information that is only retained for a short period of time (3-4 seconds). This echoic sound resonates in the mind and is replayed for this brief amount of time shortly after the presentation of auditory stimuli.<ref name="Radvansky">{{cite book |last1=Radvansky |first1=Gabriel |title=Human Memory |publisher=Allyn and Bacon |year=2005 |location=Boston |pages=65-75 |isbn=978-0-205-45760-1}}</ref>


== Overview ==
== Overview ==
Shortly after [[George Sperling]]’s partial report studies of the visual sensory memory store, researchers began investigating its counterpart in the auditory domain. The term '''echoic''' memory was coined in 1967 by [[Ulric Neisser]] to describe this brief representation of acoustic information. It was initially studied using similar partial report paradigms to those utilized by Sperling; however, modern neuropsychological techniques have enabled the development of estimations of the capacity, duration, and location of the echoic memory store. Using Sperling's model as an analogue, researchers continue to apply his work to the auditory sensory store using partial and whole report experiments. They found that the echoic store has a duration of up to 4 seconds<ref name="Darwin"> {{cite journal | title = An auditory analogue of the sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage | journal = Cognitive Psychology | date = 1972 | last = Darwin | coauthors = Turvey, Crowder | volume = 3 | pages = 255-267| id = | url = http://www.haskins.yale.edu/Reprints/HL0119.pdf | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>, and in the absence of interference has been shown to last up to 20 seconds<ref> {{cite journal | title = Memory for non-attended auditory material | journal = Cognitive Psychology | date = 1970 | last = Glucksberg | coauthors = Cowan | volume = 1 | pages = 149-156| id = | url = http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6WCR-4D5X9DF-1F&_user=1067211&_coverDate=05%2F31%2F1970&_rdoc=1&_fmt=high&_orig=gateway&_origin=gateway&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_searchStrId=1673491526&_rerunOrigin=google&_acct=C000051237&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=1067211&md5=a456100a266aff089c6553655a22fb7e&searchtype=a | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>
Shortly after [[George Sperling]]’s partial report studies of the visual sensory memory store, researchers began investigating its counterpart in the auditory domain. The term '''echoic''' memory was coined in 1967 by [[Ulric Neisser]] to describe this brief representation of acoustic information. It was initially studied using similar partial report paradigms to those utilized by Sperling; however, modern neuropsychological techniques have enabled the development of estimations of the capacity, duration, and location of the echoic memory store. Using Sperling's model as an analogue, researchers continue to apply his work to the auditory sensory store using partial and whole report experiments. They found that the echoic store has a duration of up to 4 seconds,<ref name="Darwin">{{cite journal |pages=255-67 |doi=10.1016/0010-0285(72)90007-2 |url=http://www.haskins.yale.edu/Reprints/HL0119.pdf}}</ref> and in the absence of interference has been shown to last up to 20 seconds.<ref>{{cite journal |pages=149-56 |doi=10.1016/0010-0285(70)90010-1}}</ref>


=== Early Work ===
=== Early Work ===
[[Baddeley's model of working memory]] consists of the visuospatial sketchpad which is related to [[iconic memory]], and a phonological loop which attends to auditory information processing in two ways. The first is a phonological store which has the capacity to retain information for 3-4 seconds before decay, which is a much longer duration than iconic memory (which is less than 1000ms). The second is a sub-vocal rehearsal process to keep refreshing the memory trace by the using one’s "inner voice".<ref name="Baddeley"> {{cite book | last1 = Baddeley | last2 = Eysenck | last3 = Anderson | title = Memory | chapter = 2 | publisher = Psychology Press | year = 2009 | location = New York, NY | pages = 27 | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>. However, this model fails to provide a detailed description of the relationship between the initial sensory input and ensuing memory processes.
[[Baddeley's model of working memory]] consists of the visuospatial sketchpad which is related to [[iconic memory]], and a phonological loop which attends to auditory information processing in two ways. The first is a phonological store which has the capacity to retain information for 3-4 seconds before decay, which is a much longer duration than iconic memory (which is less than 1000ms). The second is a sub-vocal rehearsal process to keep refreshing the memory trace by the using one’s "inner voice".<ref name="Baddeley">{{cite book |first1=Alan D. |last1=Baddeley |first2=Michael W. |last2=Eysenck |first3=Mike |last3=Anderson |title=Memory |year=2009 |page=27 |publisher=Psychology Press |location=New York |isbn=978-1-84872-000-8}}</ref> However, this model fails to provide a detailed description of the relationship between the initial sensory input and ensuing memory processes.

A short-term memory model proposed by [[Nelson Cowan]] attempts to address this problem by describing a verbal sensory memory input and storage in more detail. It suggests a pre-attentive sensory storage system that can hold a large amount of accurate information over a short period of time and consists of an initial phase input of 200-400ms and a secondary phase that transfers the information into a more long term memory store to be integrated into working memory that starts to decay after 10-20s <ref name="Glass">{{cite journal | title = Development of auditory sensory memory from 2 to 6 years: an MMN study | journal = Journal of Neural Transmission | date = 2008 | last = Glass | coauthors = Steffi & von Suchodoletz | volume = 115 | issue = 8 | pages = 1221-1229 | id = | url = http://www.kjp.med.uni-muenchen.de/download/Glass-JNT2008.pdf | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>.
A short-term memory model proposed by [[Nelson Cowan]] attempts to address this problem by describing a verbal sensory memory input and storage in more detail. It suggests a pre-attentive sensory storage system that can hold a large amount of accurate information over a short period of time and consists of an initial phase input of 200-400ms and a secondary phase that transfers the information into a more long term memory store to be integrated into working memory that starts to decay after 10-20s.<ref name="Glass">{{cite journal |pages=1221-9 |doi=10.1007/s00702-008-0088-6 |url=http://www.kjp.med.uni-muenchen.de/download/Glass-JNT2008.pdf}}</ref>


== Methods of Testing Echoic Memory ==
== Methods of Testing Echoic Memory ==


=== Partial & Whole Report ===
=== Partial & Whole Report ===
Following Sperling's (1960) procedures on [[iconic memory ]]tasks, future researchers were interested in testing the same phenomenon for the auditory sensory store. Echoic memory is measured by behavioural tasks where participants are asked to repeat a sequence of tones, words, or syllables that were presented to them, usually requiring attention and motivation. The most famous partial report task was conducted by presenting participants with an auditory stimulus in the left, right, and both ears simultaneously<ref name="Darwin"></ref>. Then they were asked to report spatial location and category name of each stimulus. Results showed that spatial location was far easier to recall than semantic information when inhibiting information from one ear over the other. Consistent with results on iconic memory tasks, performance on the partial report conditions were far superior to the whole report condition. In addition, a decrease in performance was observed as the [[interstimulus interval]](ISI) (length of time between presentation of the stimulus and recall) increased.
Following Sperling's (1960) procedures on [[iconic memory ]]tasks, future researchers were interested in testing the same phenomenon for the auditory sensory store. Echoic memory is measured by behavioural tasks where participants are asked to repeat a sequence of tones, words, or syllables that were presented to them, usually requiring attention and motivation. The most famous partial report task was conducted by presenting participants with an auditory stimulus in the left, right, and both ears simultaneously.<ref name="Darwin"/> Then they were asked to report spatial location and category name of each stimulus. Results showed that spatial location was far easier to recall than semantic information when inhibiting information from one ear over the other. Consistent with results on iconic memory tasks, performance on the partial report conditions were far superior to the whole report condition. In addition, a decrease in performance was observed as the [[interstimulus interval]](ISI) (length of time between presentation of the stimulus and recall) increased.


=== Auditory Backward Recognition Masking ===
=== Auditory Backward Recognition Masking ===
Auditory backward recognition masking (ABRM) is the one of the most successful tasks in studying audition. It involves presenting participants with a brief target stimulus, followed by a second stimulus (the mask) after an (ISI)<ref name="Bjork"></ref>. The amount of time the auditory information is available in memory is manipulated by the length of the ISI. Performance as indicated by accuracy of target information increases as the ISI increased to 250 ms. The mask doesn’t affect the amount of information obtained from the stimulus, but it acts as interference for further processing.
Auditory backward recognition masking (ABRM) is the one of the most successful tasks in studying audition. It involves presenting participants with a brief target stimulus, followed by a second stimulus (the mask) after an (ISI).<ref name="Bjork"/> The amount of time the auditory information is available in memory is manipulated by the length of the ISI. Performance as indicated by accuracy of target information increases as the ISI increased to 250 ms. The mask doesn’t affect the amount of information obtained from the stimulus, but it acts as interference for further processing.


=== [[Mismatch Negativity]] ===
=== Mismatch Negativity ===


A more objective, independent task capable of measuring auditory sensory memory that does not require focused attention are mismatch negativity (MMN) tasks, which record changes in activation in the brain by use of [[electroencephalography]] (EEG).
A more objective, independent task capable of measuring auditory sensory memory that does not require focused attention are [[mismatch negativity]] (MMN) tasks, which record changes in activation in the brain by use of [[electroencephalography]] (EEG).
This records elements of auditory event-related potentials (ERP) of brain activity elicited 150-200ms after a stimulus. This stimulus is an unattended, infrequent, "oddball" or deviant stimulus presented among a sequence of standard stimuli, thereby comparing the deviant stimulus to a memory trace<ref> {{cite journal | title = Neural correlates of auditory sensory memory and automatic change detection | journal = NeuroImage | date = 2003 | last = Sabri | coauthors = Kareken, Dzemidzic, Lowe, Melara, | volume = 21 | pages = 69-74| id = | url = http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14741643 | accessdate = 2011-03-08}}</ref>.
This records elements of auditory event-related potentials (ERP) of brain activity elicited 150-200ms after a stimulus. This stimulus is an unattended, infrequent, "oddball" or deviant stimulus presented among a sequence of standard stimuli, thereby comparing the deviant stimulus to a memory trace.<ref>{{cite journal |pmid=14741643}}</ref>


== Neurological Basis ==
== Neurological Basis ==
Auditory sensory memory has been found to be stored in the [[primary auditory cortex]] contralateral to the ear of presentation<ref name="Alain"></ref>. This echoic memory storage involves several different brain areas, due to the different processes it is involved in. The majority of brain regions involved are located in the [[prefrontal cortex]] (PFC) as this is where the executive control is located<ref name="Bjork"> {{cite book | last1 = Bjork | last2 = Bjork | title = Memory | chapter = 1 & 3 | editors = Bjork & Bjork | publisher = Academic Press | year = 1996 | location = San Diego, California | pages = 5, 73-80 | url = http://www.apnet.com | accessdate = 2011-03-03}}</ref>,and is responsible for attentional control. The phonological store and the rehearsal system appear to be a left-hemisphere based memory system as increased brain activity has been observed in these areas<ref name="Kwon"> {{cite journal | title = Neural basis of protracted developmental changes in visuo-spatial working memory | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences in the United States of America | date = 2002 | last = Kwon | coauthors = Reiss & Menon | volume = 99 | issue = 20 | pages = 13336-13341 | id = | url = http://www.pnas.org/content/99/20/13336.full | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>. The major regions involved are the left posterior ventrolateral [[prefrontal cortex]] (VLPFC), the left [[premotor cortex]] (PMC), and the left [[posterior parietal cortex]] (PPC). Within the VLPFC, [[Broca’s area]] is the main location responsible for verbal rehearsal and the articulatory process. The dorsal PMC is used in rhythmic organization and rehearsal, and finally the PPC shows a role in localizing objects in space.
Auditory sensory memory has been found to be stored in the [[primary auditory cortex]] contralateral to the ear of presentation.<ref name="Alain"/> This echoic memory storage involves several different brain areas, due to the different processes it is involved in. The majority of brain regions involved are located in the [[prefrontal cortex]] (PFC) as this is where the executive control is located,<ref name="Bjork">{{cite book |editor1-first=Elizabeth Ligon |editor1-last=Bjork |editor2-first=Robert A. |editor2-last=Bjork |title=Memory |year=1996 |publisher=Academic Press |location=New York |isbn=978-0-12-102571-7 |pages=5, 73-80}}</ref> and is responsible for attentional control. The phonological store and the rehearsal system appear to be a left-hemisphere based memory system as increased brain activity has been observed in these areas.<ref name="Kwon">{{cite journal |pages=13336-41 |doi=10.1073/pnas.162486399}}</ref> The major regions involved are the left posterior ventrolateral [[prefrontal cortex]] (VLPFC), the left [[premotor cortex]] (PMC), and the left [[posterior parietal cortex]] (PPC). Within the VLPFC, [[Broca’s area]] is the main location responsible for verbal rehearsal and the articulatory process. The dorsal PMC is used in rhythmic organization and rehearsal, and finally the PPC shows a role in localizing objects in space.


The cortical areas in the brain believed to be involved with auditory sensory memory exhibited by MMN response have not been localized specifically. However results have shown comparative activation in the [[superior temporal gyrus]] (STG) and in the [[inferior temporal gyrus]] (ITG)<ref> {{cite journal | title = Heshl's gyrus, posterior superior temporal gyrus, and mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex have different roles in the detection of acoustic changes | journal = NeuroPhysiology| date = 2007 | last = Schonweisner | coauthors = Novitski, Pakarinen, Carlson, Tervaniemi & Naatanen| volume = 97 | issue = 3 | pages = 2075-2082 | id = | doi = 10.1152/jn.01083.2006| accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>.
The cortical areas in the brain believed to be involved with auditory sensory memory exhibited by MMN response have not been localized specifically. However results have shown comparative activation in the [[superior temporal gyrus]] (STG) and in the [[inferior temporal gyrus]] (ITG).<ref>{{cite journal |pages=2075-82 |doi=10.1152/jn.01083.2006}}</ref>


== Development ==
== Development ==
Age-related increases in activation within the neural structures responsible for echoic memory have been observed showing that with age comes increased proficiency in the processing auditory sensory information<ref name="Kwon"></ref>.
Age-related increases in activation within the neural structures responsible for echoic memory have been observed showing that with age comes increased proficiency in the processing auditory sensory information.<ref name="Kwon"/>

Findings of a (MMN) study, also suggest that the duration of auditory sensory memory increases with age, significantly between the ages of two and six years old from 500-5000ms. Children 2 years of age exhibited an MMN response in ISI between 500ms and 1000ms. Children 3 years old have a MMN response from 1 to 2 seconds, 4 year olds over 2 seconds, and 6 year old children from 3 to 5 seconds. These developmental and cognitive changes and that occur at a young age, and extends into adulthood until eventually decreasing again at old age <ref name="Glass"></ref>.
Findings of a (MMN) study, also suggest that the duration of auditory sensory memory increases with age, significantly between the ages of two and six years old from 500-5000ms. Children 2 years of age exhibited an MMN response in ISI between 500ms and 1000ms. Children 3 years old have a MMN response from 1 to 2 seconds, 4 year olds over 2 seconds, and 6 year old children from 3 to 5 seconds. These developmental and cognitive changes and that occur at a young age, and extends into adulthood until eventually decreasing again at old age.<ref name="Glass"/>
Researchers have found shortened echoic memory duration in former late talkers (LT’s), children with [[Precordial catch syndrome]] (PCS), and oral clefts, with information decaying before 2000 ms. However this reduced echoic memory is not predictive for language difficulties in adulthood<ref name="Grossheinrich"> {{cite journal | title = Auditory sensory memory and language abilities in former late talkers: A mismatch negativity study | journal = Psychophysiology | date = 2010 | last = Grossheinrich | coauthors = Kademann, Bruder, Bartling, Von Suchodoletz | volume = 47 | issue = 5 | pages = 822-830| id = | url = http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00996.x/full | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>.

Researchers have found shortened echoic memory duration in former late talkers (LT’s), children with [[Precordial catch syndrome]] (PCS), and oral clefts, with information decaying before 2000 ms. However this reduced echoic memory is not predictive for language difficulties in adulthood.<ref name="Grossheinrich">{{cite journal |pages=822-30 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00996.x}}</ref>


== Problems ==
== Problems ==
Children with deficits in auditory memory have been shown to have developmental language disorders<ref name="Sabri"> {{cite journal | title = Neural correlates of auditory sensory memory and automatic change detection | journal = NeuroImage | date = 2003 | last = Sabri | coauthors = Kareken, Dzemidzic, Lowe, Melara | volume = 21 | pages = 69-74| id = | url = http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14741643 | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>. These problems are difficult to assess since performance could be due to their inability to understand a given task, rather than a problem with their memory.
Children with deficits in auditory memory have been shown to have developmental language disorders.<ref name="Sabri">{{cite journal |pmid=14741643}}</ref> These problems are difficult to assess since performance could be due to their inability to understand a given task, rather than a problem with their memory.

People with attributed unilateral damage to the [[dorsolateral prefrontal cortex]] and temporal-parietal cortex after experiencing a stroke were measured using the a MMN test. For the control group the MMN amplitude was largest in the right hemisphere regardless if the tone was presented in the right or left ear.
People with attributed unilateral damage to the [[dorsolateral prefrontal cortex]] and temporal-parietal cortex after experiencing a stroke were measured using the a MMN test. For the control group the MMN amplitude was largest in the right hemisphere regardless if the tone was presented in the right or left ear.
MMN was greatly reduced for temporal-parietal damaged patients when the auditory stimulus was presented to the contralateral ear of the lesion side of the brain. This adheres to the theory of auditory sensory memory being stored in the contralateral auditory cortex of ear presentation<ref name="Alain"> {{cite journal | title = A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans | journal = Brain Research | date = 1998 | last = Alain | coauthors = Woods, Knight | volume = 812 | pages = 23-37| id = | url = http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9813226 | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>. Further research on stroke victims with a reduced auditory memory store has shown that listening to daily music or audio books improved their echoic memory. This shows a positive effect of music in neural rehabilitation after brain damage.<ref name="Sarkamo"> {{cite journal | title = Music and speech listening enhance the recovery of early sensory processing after stroke | journal = . Journal of cognitiveneuroscience | date = 2010 | last = Sarkamo | volume = 22 | issue = 12 | pages = 2716 -2727| id = | url = http://portal.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1865228 | accessdate = 2011-03-09}}</ref>.

MMN was greatly reduced for temporal-parietal damaged patients when the auditory stimulus was presented to the contralateral ear of the lesion side of the brain. This adheres to the theory of auditory sensory memory being stored in the contralateral auditory cortex of ear presentation.<ref name="Alain">{{cite journal |pmid=9813226}}</ref> Further research on stroke victims with a reduced auditory memory store has shown that listening to daily music or audio books improved their echoic memory. This shows a positive effect of music in neural rehabilitation after brain damage.<ref name="Sarkamo">{{cite journal |pages=2716-27 |doi=10.1162/jocn.2009.21376}}</ref>


==References==
==References==
{{reflist}}
<div class='references-small'>

<references/>
</div>
{{Memory}}
{{Memory}}
[[Category:Memory processes]]
[[Category:Memory processes]]

Revision as of 13:08, 5 May 2011

Echoic memory is one of the sensory memory registers; a component of short term memory (STM) that is specific to retaining auditory information. This particular sensory store is capable of storing large amounts of auditory information that is only retained for a short period of time (3-4 seconds). This echoic sound resonates in the mind and is replayed for this brief amount of time shortly after the presentation of auditory stimuli.[1]

Overview

Shortly after George Sperling’s partial report studies of the visual sensory memory store, researchers began investigating its counterpart in the auditory domain. The term echoic memory was coined in 1967 by Ulric Neisser to describe this brief representation of acoustic information. It was initially studied using similar partial report paradigms to those utilized by Sperling; however, modern neuropsychological techniques have enabled the development of estimations of the capacity, duration, and location of the echoic memory store. Using Sperling's model as an analogue, researchers continue to apply his work to the auditory sensory store using partial and whole report experiments. They found that the echoic store has a duration of up to 4 seconds,[2] and in the absence of interference has been shown to last up to 20 seconds.[3]

Early Work

Baddeley's model of working memory consists of the visuospatial sketchpad which is related to iconic memory, and a phonological loop which attends to auditory information processing in two ways. The first is a phonological store which has the capacity to retain information for 3-4 seconds before decay, which is a much longer duration than iconic memory (which is less than 1000ms). The second is a sub-vocal rehearsal process to keep refreshing the memory trace by the using one’s "inner voice".[4] However, this model fails to provide a detailed description of the relationship between the initial sensory input and ensuing memory processes.

A short-term memory model proposed by Nelson Cowan attempts to address this problem by describing a verbal sensory memory input and storage in more detail. It suggests a pre-attentive sensory storage system that can hold a large amount of accurate information over a short period of time and consists of an initial phase input of 200-400ms and a secondary phase that transfers the information into a more long term memory store to be integrated into working memory that starts to decay after 10-20s.[5]

Methods of Testing Echoic Memory

Partial & Whole Report

Following Sperling's (1960) procedures on iconic memory tasks, future researchers were interested in testing the same phenomenon for the auditory sensory store. Echoic memory is measured by behavioural tasks where participants are asked to repeat a sequence of tones, words, or syllables that were presented to them, usually requiring attention and motivation. The most famous partial report task was conducted by presenting participants with an auditory stimulus in the left, right, and both ears simultaneously.[2] Then they were asked to report spatial location and category name of each stimulus. Results showed that spatial location was far easier to recall than semantic information when inhibiting information from one ear over the other. Consistent with results on iconic memory tasks, performance on the partial report conditions were far superior to the whole report condition. In addition, a decrease in performance was observed as the interstimulus interval(ISI) (length of time between presentation of the stimulus and recall) increased.

Auditory Backward Recognition Masking

Auditory backward recognition masking (ABRM) is the one of the most successful tasks in studying audition. It involves presenting participants with a brief target stimulus, followed by a second stimulus (the mask) after an (ISI).[6] The amount of time the auditory information is available in memory is manipulated by the length of the ISI. Performance as indicated by accuracy of target information increases as the ISI increased to 250 ms. The mask doesn’t affect the amount of information obtained from the stimulus, but it acts as interference for further processing.

Mismatch Negativity

A more objective, independent task capable of measuring auditory sensory memory that does not require focused attention are mismatch negativity (MMN) tasks, which record changes in activation in the brain by use of electroencephalography (EEG). This records elements of auditory event-related potentials (ERP) of brain activity elicited 150-200ms after a stimulus. This stimulus is an unattended, infrequent, "oddball" or deviant stimulus presented among a sequence of standard stimuli, thereby comparing the deviant stimulus to a memory trace.[7]

Neurological Basis

Auditory sensory memory has been found to be stored in the primary auditory cortex contralateral to the ear of presentation.[8] This echoic memory storage involves several different brain areas, due to the different processes it is involved in. The majority of brain regions involved are located in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) as this is where the executive control is located,[6] and is responsible for attentional control. The phonological store and the rehearsal system appear to be a left-hemisphere based memory system as increased brain activity has been observed in these areas.[9] The major regions involved are the left posterior ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), the left premotor cortex (PMC), and the left posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Within the VLPFC, Broca’s area is the main location responsible for verbal rehearsal and the articulatory process. The dorsal PMC is used in rhythmic organization and rehearsal, and finally the PPC shows a role in localizing objects in space.

The cortical areas in the brain believed to be involved with auditory sensory memory exhibited by MMN response have not been localized specifically. However results have shown comparative activation in the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and in the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG).[10]

Development

Age-related increases in activation within the neural structures responsible for echoic memory have been observed showing that with age comes increased proficiency in the processing auditory sensory information.[9]

Findings of a (MMN) study, also suggest that the duration of auditory sensory memory increases with age, significantly between the ages of two and six years old from 500-5000ms. Children 2 years of age exhibited an MMN response in ISI between 500ms and 1000ms. Children 3 years old have a MMN response from 1 to 2 seconds, 4 year olds over 2 seconds, and 6 year old children from 3 to 5 seconds. These developmental and cognitive changes and that occur at a young age, and extends into adulthood until eventually decreasing again at old age.[5]

Researchers have found shortened echoic memory duration in former late talkers (LT’s), children with Precordial catch syndrome (PCS), and oral clefts, with information decaying before 2000 ms. However this reduced echoic memory is not predictive for language difficulties in adulthood.[11]

Problems

Children with deficits in auditory memory have been shown to have developmental language disorders.[12] These problems are difficult to assess since performance could be due to their inability to understand a given task, rather than a problem with their memory.

People with attributed unilateral damage to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and temporal-parietal cortex after experiencing a stroke were measured using the a MMN test. For the control group the MMN amplitude was largest in the right hemisphere regardless if the tone was presented in the right or left ear.

MMN was greatly reduced for temporal-parietal damaged patients when the auditory stimulus was presented to the contralateral ear of the lesion side of the brain. This adheres to the theory of auditory sensory memory being stored in the contralateral auditory cortex of ear presentation.[8] Further research on stroke victims with a reduced auditory memory store has shown that listening to daily music or audio books improved their echoic memory. This shows a positive effect of music in neural rehabilitation after brain damage.[13]

References

  1. ^ Radvansky, Gabriel (2005). Human Memory. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. pp. 65–75. ISBN 978-0-205-45760-1.
  2. ^ a b : 255–67. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(72)90007-2 http://www.haskins.yale.edu/Reprints/HL0119.pdf. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  3. ^ : 149–56. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(70)90010-1. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  4. ^ Baddeley, Alan D.; Eysenck, Michael W.; Anderson, Mike (2009). Memory. New York: Psychology Press. p. 27. ISBN 978-1-84872-000-8.
  5. ^ a b : 1221–9. doi:10.1007/s00702-008-0088-6 http://www.kjp.med.uni-muenchen.de/download/Glass-JNT2008.pdf. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  6. ^ a b Bjork, Elizabeth Ligon; Bjork, Robert A., eds. (1996). Memory. New York: Academic Press. pp. 5, 73–80. ISBN 978-0-12-102571-7.
  7. ^ . PMID 14741643. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  8. ^ a b . PMID 9813226. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  9. ^ a b : 13336–41. doi:10.1073/pnas.162486399. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  10. ^ : 2075–82. doi:10.1152/jn.01083.2006. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  11. ^ : 822–30. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00996.x. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  12. ^ . PMID 14741643. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  13. ^ : 2716–27. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21376. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)