Jump to content

Paternal care: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Journal cites, templated 10 journal cites using AWB (10971)
m Journal cites, Added 9 dois to journal cites using AWB (10911)
Line 4: Line 4:
In [[biology]], '''paternal care''' is [[parental investment]] provided by a [[male]] to his own [[offspring]].
In [[biology]], '''paternal care''' is [[parental investment]] provided by a [[male]] to his own [[offspring]].


Parental care, by males or females, is presumed to increase [[Human development (biology)|growth]] rates, quality, and/or [[Survivorship curve|survival]] of young, and hence ultimately increase the [[inclusive fitness]] of parents.<ref>Lack, L. 1968. Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. Methuen, London.</ref><ref>Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pages 136-179 in Campbell, B., Ed., Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871–1971. Aldine Pub, Chicago.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Westneat | first1 = D. F. | last2 = Sherman | first2 = P. W. | year = 1993 | title = Parentage and the evolution of parental behavior | url = | journal = Behavioral Ecology | volume = 4 | issue = | pages = 66–77 }}</ref> In a variety of vertebrate species (e.g., ~80 of birds<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Cockburn | first1 = A | year = 2006 | title = Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds | url = | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society B | volume = 273 | issue = | pages = 1375–1383 }}</ref> and ~6% of mammals,<ref>Kleiman, D. G., & Malcolm, J. R., 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pages 347-387 in D. J. Gubernick and P. H. Klopfer, Eds., Parental care in mammals. Plenum Press, New York.</ref> both males and females invest heavily in their offspring. Many of these biparental species are socially [[Monogamy|monogamous]], so individuals remain with their mate for at least one breeding season.
Parental care, by males or females, is presumed to increase [[Human development (biology)|growth]] rates, quality, and/or [[Survivorship curve|survival]] of young, and hence ultimately increase the [[inclusive fitness]] of parents.<ref>Lack, L. 1968. Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. Methuen, London.</ref><ref>Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pages 136-179 in Campbell, B., Ed., Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871–1971. Aldine Pub, Chicago.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Westneat | first1 = D. F. | last2 = Sherman | first2 = P. W. | year = 1993 | title = Parentage and the evolution of parental behavior | url = | journal = Behavioral Ecology | volume = 4 | issue = | pages = 66–77 | doi=10.1093/beheco/4.1.66}}</ref> In a variety of vertebrate species (e.g., ~80 of birds<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Cockburn | first1 = A | year = 2006 | title = Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds | url = | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society B | volume = 273 | issue = | pages = 1375–1383 | doi=10.1098/rspb.2005.3458}}</ref> and ~6% of mammals,<ref>Kleiman, D. G., & Malcolm, J. R., 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pages 347-387 in D. J. Gubernick and P. H. Klopfer, Eds., Parental care in mammals. Plenum Press, New York.</ref> both males and females invest heavily in their offspring. Many of these biparental species are socially [[Monogamy|monogamous]], so individuals remain with their mate for at least one breeding season.


Exclusive paternal care has evolved multiple times in a variety of organisms, including invertebrates, fishes, and amphibians.<ref>Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental care. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.</ref><ref>Reynolds, J. D., Goodwin, N. B., & Freckleton, R. P. 2002. Evolutionary transitions in parental care and live bearing in vertebrates. ''Phil Trans R Soc Lond B'' 357:269-281.</ref><ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal | last1 = Wesołowski | first1 = T | year = 2004 | title = The origin of parental care in birds: a reassessment | url = | journal = Behavioral Ecology | volume = 15 | issue = | pages = 520–523 }}</ref>
Exclusive paternal care has evolved multiple times in a variety of organisms, including invertebrates, fishes, and amphibians.<ref>Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental care. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.</ref><ref>Reynolds, J. D., Goodwin, N. B., & Freckleton, R. P. 2002. Evolutionary transitions in parental care and live bearing in vertebrates. ''Phil Trans R Soc Lond B'' 357:269-281.</ref><ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal | last1 = Wesołowski | first1 = T | year = 2004 | title = The origin of parental care in birds: a reassessment | url = | journal = Behavioral Ecology | volume = 15 | issue = | pages = 520–523 | doi=10.1093/beheco/arh039}}</ref>


== Mammals ==
== Mammals ==


[[Female]] [[mammals]] invest heavily in [[reproduction]], in large part due to [[lactation]]. Because these costs are obvious, the physiological costs of female reproductive effort (defined as that proportion of the total energy budget of an organism that is devoted to reproductive processes<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hirshfield | first1 = M. F. | authorlink2 = Donald W. Tinkle | year = 1975 | title = Natural selection and the evolution of reproductive effort | url = | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 72 | issue = | pages = 2227–2231 }}</ref>), especially care of [[offspring]], have been studied extensively in mammals. These costs have been found to include reductions in [[longevity]] and in such processes as [[thermogenesis]], [[physical activity]], and [[immune function]], apparently reflecting the high [[energy|energetic]] and [[nutritional]] demands of [[parenting]].<ref>Jasienska G. 2009. Reproduction and lifespan: trade-offs, overall energy budgets, intergenerational costs, and costs neglected by research. ''Am J Hum Biol'' 21:524-432.</ref><ref>Speakman JR. 2008. The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. ''Phil Trans R Soc B'' 363:375-398.</ref>
[[Female]] [[mammals]] invest heavily in [[reproduction]], in large part due to [[lactation]]. Because these costs are obvious, the physiological costs of female reproductive effort (defined as that proportion of the total energy budget of an organism that is devoted to reproductive processes<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hirshfield | first1 = M. F. | authorlink2 = Donald W. Tinkle | year = 1975 | title = Natural selection and the evolution of reproductive effort | url = | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 72 | issue = | pages = 2227–2231 | doi=10.1073/pnas.72.6.2227}}</ref>), especially care of [[offspring]], have been studied extensively in mammals. These costs have been found to include reductions in [[longevity]] and in such processes as [[thermogenesis]], [[physical activity]], and [[immune function]], apparently reflecting the high [[energy|energetic]] and [[nutritional]] demands of [[parenting]].<ref>Jasienska G. 2009. Reproduction and lifespan: trade-offs, overall energy budgets, intergenerational costs, and costs neglected by research. ''Am J Hum Biol'' 21:524-432.</ref><ref>Speakman JR. 2008. The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. ''Phil Trans R Soc B'' 363:375-398.</ref>


Male mammals may also invest heavily in reproduction through efforts to enhance [[reproductive success]] (e.g., [[courtship]] displays, [[Mate choice|intrasexual combat]]) or to provide paternal care. However, the costs of paternal care have rarely been studied in mammals, in large part because only 5-10% of mammals exhibit such care.<ref>Kleiman DG, Malcolm JR. 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pp 347-387 in Parental Care in Mammals, Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH, eds. Plenum Press, New York.</ref><ref>Woodroffe R, Vincent A. 1994. Mother’s little helpers: patterns of male care in mammals. Trends Ecol Evol 9:294-297.</ref> Nonetheless, in those species in which males do provide extensive care for their offspring (i.e., biparental species, including [[humans]]), indirect evidence suggests that its costs can be substantial. For example, mammalian [[fathers]] that care for their young may undergo systematic changes in [[body mass]] and in circulating or excreted concentrations of a number of [[hormones]] (e.g., [[androgens]], [[glucocorticoids]], [[leptin]]) as a function of reproductive status,<ref>Campbell JC, Laugero KD, Van Westerhuyzen JA, Hostetler CM, Cohen JD, Bales KL. 2009. Costs of pair-bonding and paternal care in male prairie voles (''Microtus ochrogaster''). Physiol Behav 98:367-373.
Male mammals may also invest heavily in reproduction through efforts to enhance [[reproductive success]] (e.g., [[courtship]] displays, [[Mate choice|intrasexual combat]]) or to provide paternal care. However, the costs of paternal care have rarely been studied in mammals, in large part because only 5-10% of mammals exhibit such care.<ref>Kleiman DG, Malcolm JR. 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pp 347-387 in Parental Care in Mammals, Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH, eds. Plenum Press, New York.</ref><ref>Woodroffe R, Vincent A. 1994. Mother’s little helpers: patterns of male care in mammals. Trends Ecol Evol 9:294-297.</ref> Nonetheless, in those species in which males do provide extensive care for their offspring (i.e., biparental species, including [[humans]]), indirect evidence suggests that its costs can be substantial. For example, mammalian [[fathers]] that care for their young may undergo systematic changes in [[body mass]] and in circulating or excreted concentrations of a number of [[hormones]] (e.g., [[androgens]], [[glucocorticoids]], [[leptin]]) as a function of reproductive status,<ref>Campbell JC, Laugero KD, Van Westerhuyzen JA, Hostetler CM, Cohen JD, Bales KL. 2009. Costs of pair-bonding and paternal care in male prairie voles (''Microtus ochrogaster''). Physiol Behav 98:367-373.
Line 22: Line 22:
One study of humans has found evidence suggesting a possible evolutionary trade-off between mating success and parenting involvement; specifically, [[fathers]] with smaller [[testes]] tend to be more involved in care of their [[children]].<ref>http://www.nature.com/news/better-fathers-have-smaller-testicles-1.13701</ref>
One study of humans has found evidence suggesting a possible evolutionary trade-off between mating success and parenting involvement; specifically, [[fathers]] with smaller [[testes]] tend to be more involved in care of their [[children]].<ref>http://www.nature.com/news/better-fathers-have-smaller-testicles-1.13701</ref>


Research on the effects of paternal care on human happiness have yielded conflicting results. However, one recent study concluded that fathers generally report higher levels of happiness, positive emotion, and meaning in life as compared with non-fathers.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Nelson | first1 = S. K. | last2 = Kushlev | first2 = K. | last3 = English | first3 = T. | last4 = Dunn | first4 = E. W. | last5 = Lyubomirsky | first5 = S. | year = 2013 | title = In Defense of Parenthood: Children Are Associated With More Joy Than Misery | url = | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 24 | issue = | pages = 3–10 }}</ref>
Research on the effects of paternal care on human happiness have yielded conflicting results. However, one recent study concluded that fathers generally report higher levels of happiness, positive emotion, and meaning in life as compared with non-fathers.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Nelson | first1 = S. K. | last2 = Kushlev | first2 = K. | last3 = English | first3 = T. | last4 = Dunn | first4 = E. W. | last5 = Lyubomirsky | first5 = S. | year = 2013 | title = In Defense of Parenthood: Children Are Associated With More Joy Than Misery | url = | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 24 | issue = | pages = 3–10 | doi=10.1177/0956797612447798}}</ref>


According to the United States Census Bureau, approximately one third of children in the U.S. grow up without their biological father in their home. Numerous studies have documented negative consequences of being raised in a home that lacks a father, including increased likelihood of living in poverty, having behavioral problems, committing crimes, spending time in prison, abusing drugs or alcohol, becoming obese, and dropping out of school.<ref>http://blog.fatherhood.org/bid/190202/The-Father-Absence-Crisis-in-America-Infographic Accessed 17 December 2013</ref>
According to the United States Census Bureau, approximately one third of children in the U.S. grow up without their biological father in their home. Numerous studies have documented negative consequences of being raised in a home that lacks a father, including increased likelihood of living in poverty, having behavioral problems, committing crimes, spending time in prison, abusing drugs or alcohol, becoming obese, and dropping out of school.<ref>http://blog.fatherhood.org/bid/190202/The-Father-Absence-Crisis-in-America-Infographic Accessed 17 December 2013</ref>
Line 34: Line 34:
[[Image:California mouse 5 Peromyscus californicus.jpg|thumb|left|California mice (''[[Peromyscus californicus]]'') are very well known among [[Mammalogy|mammalogists]] for their strongly [[monogamous]] breeding system. Unlike most small mammals, male California mice have intensive and sustained paternal behavior, and are not very [[promiscuous]]. Because of this unusual breeding and [[mating system]], California mice have been studied extensively.]]
[[Image:California mouse 5 Peromyscus californicus.jpg|thumb|left|California mice (''[[Peromyscus californicus]]'') are very well known among [[Mammalogy|mammalogists]] for their strongly [[monogamous]] breeding system. Unlike most small mammals, male California mice have intensive and sustained paternal behavior, and are not very [[promiscuous]]. Because of this unusual breeding and [[mating system]], California mice have been studied extensively.]]


Several species of rodents have been studied as models of paternal care, including [[Prairie vole]]s (''Microtus ochrogaster''), [[Campbell's dwarf hamster]], the [[Mongolian gerbil]], and the [[Rhabdomys|African striped mouse]]. The [[California mouse]] (''Peromyscus californicus'') is a [[Monogamy|monogamous]] rodent that exhibits extensive and essential paternal care, and hence has been studied as a [[model organism]] for this phenomenon.<ref>Trainor, B. C., Pride, M. C., Villalon Landeros, R., Knoblauch, N. W., Takahashi, E. Y., Silva, A. L. & Crean, K. K. 2011. Sex differences in social interaction behavior following social defeat stress in the monogamous California mouse. PLoS One, e17405.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = De Jong | first1 = T.R. | last2 = Korosi | first2 = A. | last3 = Harris | first3 = B.N. | last4 = Perea-Rodrigues | first4 = J.P. | last5 = Saltzman | first5 = W. | year = 2012 | title = Individual variation in paternal responses of virgin male California Mice (''Peromyscus californicus''): Behavioral and physiological correlates | url = | journal = Physiological and Biochemical Zoology | volume = 85 | issue = | pages = 740–751 }}</ref>
Several species of rodents have been studied as models of paternal care, including [[Prairie vole]]s (''Microtus ochrogaster''), [[Campbell's dwarf hamster]], the [[Mongolian gerbil]], and the [[Rhabdomys|African striped mouse]]. The [[California mouse]] (''Peromyscus californicus'') is a [[Monogamy|monogamous]] rodent that exhibits extensive and essential paternal care, and hence has been studied as a [[model organism]] for this phenomenon.<ref>Trainor, B. C., Pride, M. C., Villalon Landeros, R., Knoblauch, N. W., Takahashi, E. Y., Silva, A. L. & Crean, K. K. 2011. Sex differences in social interaction behavior following social defeat stress in the monogamous California mouse. PLoS One, e17405.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = De Jong | first1 = T.R. | last2 = Korosi | first2 = A. | last3 = Harris | first3 = B.N. | last4 = Perea-Rodrigues | first4 = J.P. | last5 = Saltzman | first5 = W. | year = 2012 | title = Individual variation in paternal responses of virgin male California Mice (''Peromyscus californicus''): Behavioral and physiological correlates | url = | journal = Physiological and Biochemical Zoology | volume = 85 | issue = | pages = 740–751 | doi=10.1086/665831}}</ref>


== Birds ==
== Birds ==
Line 47: Line 47:


Although some maternal care and considerable [[Social behavior|sociality]] (especially in [[lizards]]<ref>Fox, S. H., J. K. McCoy & T. A. Baird, editors. 2003. Lizard Social Behavior. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore.</ref>
Although some maternal care and considerable [[Social behavior|sociality]] (especially in [[lizards]]<ref>Fox, S. H., J. K. McCoy & T. A. Baird, editors. 2003. Lizard Social Behavior. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore.</ref>
<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Davis | first1 = A. R. | last2 = Corl | first2 = A. | last3 = Surget-Groba | first3 = Y. | last4 = Sinervo | first4 = B. | year = 2010 | title = Convergent evolution of kin-based sociality in a lizard | url = | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society B | volume = 278 | issue = | pages = 1507–1514 }}</ref>) occurs in [[squamates]], paternal care per se apparently has not been reported.
<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Davis | first1 = A. R. | last2 = Corl | first2 = A. | last3 = Surget-Groba | first3 = Y. | last4 = Sinervo | first4 = B. | year = 2010 | title = Convergent evolution of kin-based sociality in a lizard | url = | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society B | volume = 278 | issue = | pages = 1507–1514 | doi=10.1098/rspb.2010.1703}}</ref>) occurs in [[squamates]], paternal care per se apparently has not been reported.


== Fish ==
== Fish ==
Line 57: Line 57:
== Arthropods ==
== Arthropods ==


Paternal care is rare in [[arthropods]],<ref>Wong, J. W. Y., J. Meunier & M. Kolliker. 2013. The evolution of parental care in insects: the roles of ecology, life history and the social environment.</ref> but occurs in some [[species]], including the [[giant water bug]]<ref>Smith, R.L. 1997. Evolution of paternal care in the giant water bugs (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae). Pages 116–149 in The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids (ed. by J. C. Choe and B. J. Crespi). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.</ref><ref>Ohba, S.-Y., Hidaka, K. & Sasaki, M. 2006. Notes on paternal care and sibling cannibalism in the giant water bug, ''Lethocerus deyrolli'' (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae). Entomological Science 9:1–5.</ref> and the [[arachnid]] ''Iporangaia pustulosa'', a [[Opiliones|harvestman]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Requena | first1 = G.S. | last2 = Buzatto | first2 = B.A. | last3 = Martins | first3 = E.G. | last4 = Machado | first4 = G. | year = 2012 | title = Paternal care decreases foraging activity and body condition, but does not impose survival costs to caring males in a Neotropical arachnid | url = | journal = PLoS ONE | volume = 7 | issue = | page = e46701 }}</ref> In several species of [[crustaceans]], males provide care of offspring by building and defending burrows or other nest sites.<ref>Duffy, S. E. 2010. Crustacean social evolution, in: Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior. Elsevier, pp. 421–429.</ref> Exclusive paternal care, where males provide the sole investment after egg-laying, is the rarest form, and is known in only 13 taxa: giant water bugs, [[sea spider]]s, two genera of [[leaf-footed bugs]], two genera of [[Reduviidae|assassin bugs]], three genera of [[Phlaeothripidae|phlaeothripid thrips]], three genera of harvestmen, and in [[millipedes]] of the family [[Andrognathidae]].<ref name=Tallamy2001>{{cite journal|last=Tallamy|first=Douglas W.|title=Evolution of exclusive paternal care in arthopods|journal=Annual Review of Entomology|year=2001|volume=46|issue=1|pages=139–165|doi=10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.139}}</ref>
Paternal care is rare in [[arthropods]],<ref>Wong, J. W. Y., J. Meunier & M. Kolliker. 2013. The evolution of parental care in insects: the roles of ecology, life history and the social environment.</ref> but occurs in some [[species]], including the [[giant water bug]]<ref>Smith, R.L. 1997. Evolution of paternal care in the giant water bugs (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae). Pages 116–149 in The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids (ed. by J. C. Choe and B. J. Crespi). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.</ref><ref>Ohba, S.-Y., Hidaka, K. & Sasaki, M. 2006. Notes on paternal care and sibling cannibalism in the giant water bug, ''Lethocerus deyrolli'' (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae). Entomological Science 9:1–5.</ref> and the [[arachnid]] ''Iporangaia pustulosa'', a [[Opiliones|harvestman]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Requena | first1 = G.S. | last2 = Buzatto | first2 = B.A. | last3 = Martins | first3 = E.G. | last4 = Machado | first4 = G. | year = 2012 | title = Paternal care decreases foraging activity and body condition, but does not impose survival costs to caring males in a Neotropical arachnid | url = | journal = PLoS ONE | volume = 7 | issue = | page = e46701 | doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0046701}}</ref> In several species of [[crustaceans]], males provide care of offspring by building and defending burrows or other nest sites.<ref>Duffy, S. E. 2010. Crustacean social evolution, in: Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior. Elsevier, pp. 421–429.</ref> Exclusive paternal care, where males provide the sole investment after egg-laying, is the rarest form, and is known in only 13 taxa: giant water bugs, [[sea spider]]s, two genera of [[leaf-footed bugs]], two genera of [[Reduviidae|assassin bugs]], three genera of [[Phlaeothripidae|phlaeothripid thrips]], three genera of harvestmen, and in [[millipedes]] of the family [[Andrognathidae]].<ref name=Tallamy2001>{{cite journal|last=Tallamy|first=Douglas W.|title=Evolution of exclusive paternal care in arthopods|journal=Annual Review of Entomology|year=2001|volume=46|issue=1|pages=139–165|doi=10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.139}}</ref>


== Theoretical models of the evolution of paternal care ==
== Theoretical models of the evolution of paternal care ==
Line 77: Line 77:
Paternal males do not undergo these same hormonal changes and so the proximate causes of the onset of parental behaviors must differ from those in females. There is little consensus regarding the processes by which mammalian males begin to express parental behaviors.<ref>Wynne-Edwards, K. E., & Timonin, M. E. 2007. Paternal care in rodents: weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care. Hormones and Behavior 52:114–121.</ref> In humans, evidence ties [[oxytocin]] to sensitive care-giving in both women and men, and with [[affectionate]] infant contact in women and [[Stimulate|stimulatory]] [[infant]] contact in men. In contrast, testosterone decreases in men who become involved fathers and testosterone may interfere with aspects of paternal care.<ref>Rilling, K. K. 2013. The neural and hormonal bases of human parental care. ''Neuropsychologia'' 51:)731–747.</ref>
Paternal males do not undergo these same hormonal changes and so the proximate causes of the onset of parental behaviors must differ from those in females. There is little consensus regarding the processes by which mammalian males begin to express parental behaviors.<ref>Wynne-Edwards, K. E., & Timonin, M. E. 2007. Paternal care in rodents: weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care. Hormones and Behavior 52:114–121.</ref> In humans, evidence ties [[oxytocin]] to sensitive care-giving in both women and men, and with [[affectionate]] infant contact in women and [[Stimulate|stimulatory]] [[infant]] contact in men. In contrast, testosterone decreases in men who become involved fathers and testosterone may interfere with aspects of paternal care.<ref>Rilling, K. K. 2013. The neural and hormonal bases of human parental care. ''Neuropsychologia'' 51:)731–747.</ref>


[[Placentophagia]] (the [[behavior]] of [[ingesting]] the [[afterbirth]] after [[parturition]]) has been proposed to have physiological consequences that could facilitate a male’s responsiveness to offspring<ref>Gregg, J. K., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. 2005. Placentophagia in naïve adults, new fathers, and new mothers in the biparental dwarf hamster, ''Phodopus campbelli''. ''Developmental Psychobiology'' 47:179–188.</ref><ref>Gregg, J. K., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. 2006. In uniparental ''Phodopus sungorus'', new mothers, and fathers present during the birth of their offspring, are the only hamsters that readily consume fresh placenta. ''Developmental Psychobiology'' 48:528–536.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Lévy | first1 = F. | last2 = Keller | first2 = M. | year = 2009 | title = Olfactory mediation of maternal behavior in selected mammalian species | url = | journal = Behavioural Brain Research | volume = 200 | issue = | pages = 336–345 }}</ref><ref>Lévy, F., Keller, M., & Poindron, P. 2004. Olfactory regulation of maternal behavior in mammals. ''Hormones and Behavior'' 46:284–302.</ref> Non-[[genomic]] transmission of paternal behavior from fathers to their sons has been reported to occur in laboratory studies of the biparental [[California mouse]], but whether this involves ([[epigenetic]]) modifications or other mechanisms is not yet known.<ref>Gleason, E. D., & Marler, C. A. 2013. Non-genomic transmission of paternal behaviour between fathers and sons in the monogamous and biparental California mouse. ''Proc R Soc B'' 280: 20130824. doi 10.1098/rspb.2013.0824</ref>
[[Placentophagia]] (the [[behavior]] of [[ingesting]] the [[afterbirth]] after [[parturition]]) has been proposed to have physiological consequences that could facilitate a male’s responsiveness to offspring<ref>Gregg, J. K., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. 2005. Placentophagia in naïve adults, new fathers, and new mothers in the biparental dwarf hamster, ''Phodopus campbelli''. ''Developmental Psychobiology'' 47:179–188.</ref><ref>Gregg, J. K., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. 2006. In uniparental ''Phodopus sungorus'', new mothers, and fathers present during the birth of their offspring, are the only hamsters that readily consume fresh placenta. ''Developmental Psychobiology'' 48:528–536.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Lévy | first1 = F. | last2 = Keller | first2 = M. | year = 2009 | title = Olfactory mediation of maternal behavior in selected mammalian species | url = | journal = Behavioural Brain Research | volume = 200 | issue = | pages = 336–345 | doi=10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.017}}</ref><ref>Lévy, F., Keller, M., & Poindron, P. 2004. Olfactory regulation of maternal behavior in mammals. ''Hormones and Behavior'' 46:284–302.</ref> Non-[[genomic]] transmission of paternal behavior from fathers to their sons has been reported to occur in laboratory studies of the biparental [[California mouse]], but whether this involves ([[epigenetic]]) modifications or other mechanisms is not yet known.<ref>Gleason, E. D., & Marler, C. A. 2013. Non-genomic transmission of paternal behaviour between fathers and sons in the monogamous and biparental California mouse. ''Proc R Soc B'' 280: 20130824. doi 10.1098/rspb.2013.0824</ref>


== See also ==
== See also ==

Revision as of 11:20, 23 May 2015

In biology, paternal care is parental investment provided by a male to his own offspring.

Parental care, by males or females, is presumed to increase growth rates, quality, and/or survival of young, and hence ultimately increase the inclusive fitness of parents.[1][2][3] In a variety of vertebrate species (e.g., ~80 of birds[4] and ~6% of mammals,[5] both males and females invest heavily in their offspring. Many of these biparental species are socially monogamous, so individuals remain with their mate for at least one breeding season.

Exclusive paternal care has evolved multiple times in a variety of organisms, including invertebrates, fishes, and amphibians.[6][7][8]

Mammals

Female mammals invest heavily in reproduction, in large part due to lactation. Because these costs are obvious, the physiological costs of female reproductive effort (defined as that proportion of the total energy budget of an organism that is devoted to reproductive processes[9]), especially care of offspring, have been studied extensively in mammals. These costs have been found to include reductions in longevity and in such processes as thermogenesis, physical activity, and immune function, apparently reflecting the high energetic and nutritional demands of parenting.[10][11]

Male mammals may also invest heavily in reproduction through efforts to enhance reproductive success (e.g., courtship displays, intrasexual combat) or to provide paternal care. However, the costs of paternal care have rarely been studied in mammals, in large part because only 5-10% of mammals exhibit such care.[12][13] Nonetheless, in those species in which males do provide extensive care for their offspring (i.e., biparental species, including humans), indirect evidence suggests that its costs can be substantial. For example, mammalian fathers that care for their young may undergo systematic changes in body mass and in circulating or excreted concentrations of a number of hormones (e.g., androgens, glucocorticoids, leptin) as a function of reproductive status,[14][15][16] and several of these hormones have important effects on body composition, metabolism, and organismal performance.[17][18] Nonetheless, the energetic and performance consequences of male parental investment have not been investigated directly in mammals.

In mammals, paternal care is found most commonly in primates, rodents, and canids.

Humans

Human cultures and societies vary widely in the expression of paternal care. Some cultures recognize paternal care via celebration of Father's Day. According to CARTA [1], human paternal care is a derived characteristic (evolved in humans or our recent ancestors) and one of the defining characteristics of Homo sapiens. Different aspects of human paternal care (direct, indirect, fostering social or moral development) may have evolved at different points in our history, and together they form a unique suite of behaviors as compared with the Great Apes. One study of humans has found evidence suggesting a possible evolutionary trade-off between mating success and parenting involvement; specifically, fathers with smaller testes tend to be more involved in care of their children.[19]

Research on the effects of paternal care on human happiness have yielded conflicting results. However, one recent study concluded that fathers generally report higher levels of happiness, positive emotion, and meaning in life as compared with non-fathers.[20]

According to the United States Census Bureau, approximately one third of children in the U.S. grow up without their biological father in their home. Numerous studies have documented negative consequences of being raised in a home that lacks a father, including increased likelihood of living in poverty, having behavioral problems, committing crimes, spending time in prison, abusing drugs or alcohol, becoming obese, and dropping out of school.[21]

Non-human primates

Paternal care is rare in non-human primates.[22]

Rodents

California mice (Peromyscus californicus) are very well known among mammalogists for their strongly monogamous breeding system. Unlike most small mammals, male California mice have intensive and sustained paternal behavior, and are not very promiscuous. Because of this unusual breeding and mating system, California mice have been studied extensively.

Several species of rodents have been studied as models of paternal care, including Prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), Campbell's dwarf hamster, the Mongolian gerbil, and the African striped mouse. The California mouse (Peromyscus californicus) is a monogamous rodent that exhibits extensive and essential paternal care, and hence has been studied as a model organism for this phenomenon.[23][24]

Birds

Fathers contribute to the care of offspring in as many as 90% of bird species, sometimes including incubating the eggs. Most paternal care is associated with biparental care in socially monogamous mating systems (about 81% of species), but in approximately 1% of species, fathers provide all care after eggs are laid.[25] The unusually high incidence of paternal care in birds compared to other vertebrate taxa is often assumed to stem from the extensive resource requirements for production of flight-capable offspring. By contrast, in bats (the other extant flying vertebrate lineage), care of offspring is provided by females (although males may help guard pups in some species[26]). In contrast to the large clutch sizes found in many bird species with biparental care, bats typically produce single offspring, which may be a limitation related to lack of male help. It has been suggested, though not without controversy, that paternal care is the ancestral form of parental care in birds.[8]

Crocodilians

Although female crocodilians typically provide some protection for eggs and hatchlings, paternal care has not been reported in this lineage of vertebrates.[27]

Squamates (lizards and snakes)

Although some maternal care and considerable sociality (especially in lizards[28] [29]) occurs in squamates, paternal care per se apparently has not been reported.

Fish

Paternal care occurs in perhaps as many as half of the known species of certain families of teleost fish. One well-known example of paternal care is in seahorses, where males brood the eggs in a pouch until they are ready to hatch.

In jawfish, the female lays the eggs and the male then takes them in his mouth. A male can have up to 400 eggs in his mouth at one time. The male can't feed while he hosts the young, but as the young get older, they spend more time out of the mouth.[30]

Arthropods

Paternal care is rare in arthropods,[31] but occurs in some species, including the giant water bug[32][33] and the arachnid Iporangaia pustulosa, a harvestman.[34] In several species of crustaceans, males provide care of offspring by building and defending burrows or other nest sites.[35] Exclusive paternal care, where males provide the sole investment after egg-laying, is the rarest form, and is known in only 13 taxa: giant water bugs, sea spiders, two genera of leaf-footed bugs, two genera of assassin bugs, three genera of phlaeothripid thrips, three genera of harvestmen, and in millipedes of the family Andrognathidae.[36]

Theoretical models of the evolution of paternal care

Mathematical models related to the Prisoner's Dilemma suggest that when female reproductive costs are higher than male reproductive costs, males cooperate with females even when they do not reciprocate. In this view, paternal care is an evolutionary achievement that compensates for the higher energy demands that reproduction typically involves for mothers.[37]

Other models suggest that basic life-history differences between males and females are adequate to explain the evolutionary origins of maternal, paternal, and bi-parental care. Specifically, paternal care is more likely if male adult mortality is high, and maternal care is more likely to evolve if female adult mortality is high.[38] Basic life-history differences between the sexes can also cause evolutionary transitions among different sex-specific patterns of parental care.[39]

Consequences of paternal care for offspring survival and development

Care by fathers can have important consequences for survival and development of offspring in both humans[40] and other species. A study discovered an extended duration of paternal care in the Gentoo penguin, when compared to other Pygoscelis species. It was found that their fledging period, the time between a chick’s first trip to sea and its absolute independence from the group, was longer than other penguins of the same genus. They hypothesized that this was because it allowed chicks to better develop their foraging skills before becoming completely independent from their parents. By doing so, a chick may have a higher chance of survival and increase the population’s overall fitness.[41]

Proximate mechanisms of paternal care

The proximate mechanisms of paternal care are not well understood for any organism. In vertebrates, at the level of hormonal control, vasopressin apparently underlies the neurochemical basis of paternal care; prolactin and testosterone may also be involved. As with other behaviors that affect Darwinian fitness, reward pathways[42] in the brain may reinforce the expression of paternal care and may be involved in the formation of attachment bonds.

The mechanisms that underlie the onset of parental behaviors in female mammals have been characterized in a variety of species. In mammals, females undergo endocrine changes during gestation and lactation that "prime" mothers to respond maternally towards their offspring.[43][44]

Paternal males do not undergo these same hormonal changes and so the proximate causes of the onset of parental behaviors must differ from those in females. There is little consensus regarding the processes by which mammalian males begin to express parental behaviors.[45] In humans, evidence ties oxytocin to sensitive care-giving in both women and men, and with affectionate infant contact in women and stimulatory infant contact in men. In contrast, testosterone decreases in men who become involved fathers and testosterone may interfere with aspects of paternal care.[46]

Placentophagia (the behavior of ingesting the afterbirth after parturition) has been proposed to have physiological consequences that could facilitate a male’s responsiveness to offspring[47][48][49][50] Non-genomic transmission of paternal behavior from fathers to their sons has been reported to occur in laboratory studies of the biparental California mouse, but whether this involves (epigenetic) modifications or other mechanisms is not yet known.[51]

See also

3

References

  1. ^ Lack, L. 1968. Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. Methuen, London.
  2. ^ Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pages 136-179 in Campbell, B., Ed., Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871–1971. Aldine Pub, Chicago.
  3. ^ Westneat, D. F.; Sherman, P. W. (1993). "Parentage and the evolution of parental behavior". Behavioral Ecology. 4: 66–77. doi:10.1093/beheco/4.1.66.
  4. ^ Cockburn, A (2006). "Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 273: 1375–1383. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3458.
  5. ^ Kleiman, D. G., & Malcolm, J. R., 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pages 347-387 in D. J. Gubernick and P. H. Klopfer, Eds., Parental care in mammals. Plenum Press, New York.
  6. ^ Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental care. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  7. ^ Reynolds, J. D., Goodwin, N. B., & Freckleton, R. P. 2002. Evolutionary transitions in parental care and live bearing in vertebrates. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 357:269-281.
  8. ^ a b Wesołowski, T (2004). "The origin of parental care in birds: a reassessment". Behavioral Ecology. 15: 520–523. doi:10.1093/beheco/arh039.
  9. ^ Hirshfield, M. F. (1975). "Natural selection and the evolution of reproductive effort". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 72: 2227–2231. doi:10.1073/pnas.72.6.2227.
  10. ^ Jasienska G. 2009. Reproduction and lifespan: trade-offs, overall energy budgets, intergenerational costs, and costs neglected by research. Am J Hum Biol 21:524-432.
  11. ^ Speakman JR. 2008. The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. Phil Trans R Soc B 363:375-398.
  12. ^ Kleiman DG, Malcolm JR. 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pp 347-387 in Parental Care in Mammals, Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH, eds. Plenum Press, New York.
  13. ^ Woodroffe R, Vincent A. 1994. Mother’s little helpers: patterns of male care in mammals. Trends Ecol Evol 9:294-297.
  14. ^ Campbell JC, Laugero KD, Van Westerhuyzen JA, Hostetler CM, Cohen JD, Bales KL. 2009. Costs of pair-bonding and paternal care in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Physiol Behav 98:367-373.
  15. ^ Wynne-Edwards KE, Timonin ME. 2007. Paternal care in rodents: weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care. Horm Behav 52:114-121.
  16. ^ Ziegler TE, Prudom SL, Schultz-Darken NJ, Kurian AV, Snowdon CT. 2006. Pregnancy weight gain: marmoset and tamarin dads show it too. Biol Lett 2:181-183.
  17. ^ Arnold, S. J. (1983). "Morphology, performance and fitness" (PDF). American Zoologist. 23: 347–361. doi:10.1093/icb/23.2.347.
  18. ^ Careau, V. C.; T. Garland, Jr. (2012). "Performance, personality, and energetics: correlation, causation, and mechanism" (PDF). Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 85 (6): 543–571. doi:10.1086/666970. PMID 23099454.
  19. ^ http://www.nature.com/news/better-fathers-have-smaller-testicles-1.13701
  20. ^ Nelson, S. K.; Kushlev, K.; English, T.; Dunn, E. W.; Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). "In Defense of Parenthood: Children Are Associated With More Joy Than Misery". Psychological Science. 24: 3–10. doi:10.1177/0956797612447798.
  21. ^ http://blog.fatherhood.org/bid/190202/The-Father-Absence-Crisis-in-America-Infographic Accessed 17 December 2013
  22. ^ Fernandez-Duque, Eduardo; Valeggia, Claudia R.; Mendoza, Sally P. (2009). "The Biology of Paternal Care in Human and Nonhuman Primates". Annual Review of Anthropology. 38 (1): 115–130. doi:10.1146/annurev-anthro-091908-164334.
  23. ^ Trainor, B. C., Pride, M. C., Villalon Landeros, R., Knoblauch, N. W., Takahashi, E. Y., Silva, A. L. & Crean, K. K. 2011. Sex differences in social interaction behavior following social defeat stress in the monogamous California mouse. PLoS One, e17405.
  24. ^ De Jong, T.R.; Korosi, A.; Harris, B.N.; Perea-Rodrigues, J.P.; Saltzman, W. (2012). "Individual variation in paternal responses of virgin male California Mice (Peromyscus californicus): Behavioral and physiological correlates". Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 85: 740–751. doi:10.1086/665831.
  25. ^ Cockburn, A., 2006. Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 273 , 1375-1383
  26. ^ Kunz, T.H. & W.R. Hood. 2000. Parental care and postnatal growth in the Chiroptera. pp 416-510 in Reproductive Biology of Bats; E. G. Chrichton, & P. H. Krutzsch, Eds. Academic Press
  27. ^ http://physiologizing.blogspot.com/2013/01/parental-care-in-crocodilians-nothing.html
  28. ^ Fox, S. H., J. K. McCoy & T. A. Baird, editors. 2003. Lizard Social Behavior. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore.
  29. ^ Davis, A. R.; Corl, A.; Surget-Groba, Y.; Sinervo, B. (2010). "Convergent evolution of kin-based sociality in a lizard". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 278: 1507–1514. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1703.
  30. ^ Taylor, A-L. (2012). "Shells, trees and bottoms: Strange places fish live". BBC. Retrieved February 26, 2015.
  31. ^ Wong, J. W. Y., J. Meunier & M. Kolliker. 2013. The evolution of parental care in insects: the roles of ecology, life history and the social environment.
  32. ^ Smith, R.L. 1997. Evolution of paternal care in the giant water bugs (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae). Pages 116–149 in The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids (ed. by J. C. Choe and B. J. Crespi). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.
  33. ^ Ohba, S.-Y., Hidaka, K. & Sasaki, M. 2006. Notes on paternal care and sibling cannibalism in the giant water bug, Lethocerus deyrolli (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae). Entomological Science 9:1–5.
  34. ^ Requena, G.S.; Buzatto, B.A.; Martins, E.G.; Machado, G. (2012). "Paternal care decreases foraging activity and body condition, but does not impose survival costs to caring males in a Neotropical arachnid". PLoS ONE. 7: e46701. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  35. ^ Duffy, S. E. 2010. Crustacean social evolution, in: Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior. Elsevier, pp. 421–429.
  36. ^ Tallamy, Douglas W. (2001). "Evolution of exclusive paternal care in arthopods". Annual Review of Entomology. 46 (1): 139–165. doi:10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.139.
  37. ^ Salgado, M. 2013. The Evolution of Paternal Care. Pages 1-10 in A.M. Greenberg, W.G. Kennedy, and N.D. Bos (Eds.): SBP 2013, LNCS 7812. Springer-Verlag: Berlin, Heidelberg.
  38. ^ Klug, H., M. B. Bonsall & S. H. Alonzo. 2013. The origin of parental care in relation to male and female life history. Ecology and Evolution 3:779-991. doi:10.1002/ece3.493
  39. ^ Klug, H., M. B. Bonsall & S. H. Alonzo. 2013. Sex differences in life history drive evolutionary transitions among maternal, paternal, and bi-parental care. Ecology and Evolution 3:792-806. doi:10.1002/ece3.494
  40. ^ http://www.zerotothree.org/child-development/early-development/how-men-and-children-affect.html
  41. ^ Polito, Michael J., and Wayne Z. Trivelpiece. "Transition to Independence and Evidence of Extended Parental Care in the Gentoo Penguin (Pygoscelis Papua)." Marine Biology (2008): n. pag. WorldCat@OSU. Web. Sept. 2014.
  42. ^ http://learn.genetics.utah.edu/content/addiction/reward/
  43. ^ Brunton, P., Russell, J., & Douglas, A. 2008. Adaptive responses of the maternal hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis during pregnancy and lactation. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 20:764-776.
  44. ^ Numan, M., & Insel, T. R. 2003. The Neurobiology of Parental Behavior. Springer, New York.
  45. ^ Wynne-Edwards, K. E., & Timonin, M. E. 2007. Paternal care in rodents: weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care. Hormones and Behavior 52:114–121.
  46. ^ Rilling, K. K. 2013. The neural and hormonal bases of human parental care. Neuropsychologia 51:)731–747.
  47. ^ Gregg, J. K., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. 2005. Placentophagia in naïve adults, new fathers, and new mothers in the biparental dwarf hamster, Phodopus campbelli. Developmental Psychobiology 47:179–188.
  48. ^ Gregg, J. K., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. 2006. In uniparental Phodopus sungorus, new mothers, and fathers present during the birth of their offspring, are the only hamsters that readily consume fresh placenta. Developmental Psychobiology 48:528–536.
  49. ^ Lévy, F.; Keller, M. (2009). "Olfactory mediation of maternal behavior in selected mammalian species". Behavioural Brain Research. 200: 336–345. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.017.
  50. ^ Lévy, F., Keller, M., & Poindron, P. 2004. Olfactory regulation of maternal behavior in mammals. Hormones and Behavior 46:284–302.
  51. ^ Gleason, E. D., & Marler, C. A. 2013. Non-genomic transmission of paternal behaviour between fathers and sons in the monogamous and biparental California mouse. Proc R Soc B 280: 20130824. doi 10.1098/rspb.2013.0824

Further reading

  • Gray, P. B., & Anderson, K. G. 2010. Fatherhood: Evolution and human paternal behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Gray, P. B., & Garcia, J. R. 2013. Evolution and human sexual behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Malinowski, B. 1938. The sexual life of savages. Boston: Beacon Press.
  • Muller, M. N., & Emery Thompson, M. 2012. Mating, parenting, and male reproductive strategies. In. J. C. Mitani, J. Call, P. M. Kappeler, R. A. Palombi, & J. B. Silk (Eds.), The evolution of primate societies (pp. 387–411). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  • Salgado, M. 2013. The Evolution of Paternal Care. Pages 1–10 in A.M. Greenberg, W.G. Kennedy, and N.D. Bos (Eds.): SBP 2013, LNCS 7812. Springer-Verlag: Berlin, Heidelberg.
  • Smuts, B. B., & Gubernick, D. J. 1992. Male-infant relationships in nonhuman primates: Paternal investment or mating effort? In B. S. Hewlett (Ed.), Father-child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts, (pp. 1–30). New York: Aldine.

External links