Jump to content

Koinophilia: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Added {{COI}} tag to article (TW)
rearranged and rephrased as per suggestions on Talk page
Line 3: Line 3:
{{evolutionary biology}}
{{evolutionary biology}}


'''Koinophilia''' occurs when [[Mating|mate]]-seeking animals preferentially choose individuals that do not have any unusual features. The term was first used by the evolutionary biologist Johan Koeslag.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990">Koeslag, J.H. (1990). "Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour." ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''144''', 15–35</ref>
'''Koinophilia''' occurs when [[Mating|mate]]-seeking animals preferentially choose individuals with a minimum of unusual features.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990">Koeslag, J.H. (1990). "Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour." ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''144''', 15–35</ref><ref name=miller>{{cite book|author1-last=Miller|author1-first =W.B.|title=In: The Microcosm within: Evolution and Extinction in the Hologenome.|chapter= What is the big deal about evolutionary gaps?|publication-date=2013|publisher= Universal Publishers.|location= Boca Raton, Florida.|isbn=10:1-61233-2773|pages=395-396}}</ref><ref name=unnikrishnan1>{{cite journal | doi = 10.1016/ j.jtbi.2009.08.011 |author-last1= Unnikrishnan| author-first1=M.K.|year = 2009 | title = How is the individuality of a face recognized?|url = | journal = Journal of Theoretical Biology|volume =261| issue =3| pages = 469–474. }}</ref><ref name=unnikrishnan2>{{cite journal | doi = 10.1016/ j.jtbi.2009.08.011 |author-last1= Unnikrishnan| author-first1=M.K.|year = 2012 | title = Koinophilia revisited: the evolutionary link between mate selection and face recognition. |url = | journal = Current Science|volume =102|issue =4|pages = 563-570 }}</ref> The term derives from the Greek, ''koinos'', "the usual", and ''philos'', "fondness".


[[Natural selection]] causes [[Fitness (biology)|beneficial]] inherited features to become more common and eventually to replace their disadvantageous counterparts. A sexual creature would be expected to avoid individuals with unusual features, and to prefer to mate with individuals displaying a predominance of [[Averageness|average features]].<ref>Symons, D. (1979) ''The Evolution of Human Sexuality''. Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> As a side effect, mates displaying [[mutation|mutant]] features are also avoided. This is advantageous because most mutations that manifest themselves as changes in appearance, functionality or behavior, are disadvantageous. Because it is impossible to judge whether a new mutation is beneficial or not, koinophilic animals avoid them all, at the cost of avoiding an occasional beneficial mutation. Thus, koinophilia, although not infallible in its ability to distinguish [[Fitness (biology)|fit]] from unfit mates, is a good strategy when [[Sexual selection|choosing a mate]]. A koinophilic choice ensures that offspring are likely to inherit features that have been successful in the past.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" />
[[Natural selection]] causes [[Fitness (biology)|beneficial]] inherited features to become more common and eventually to replace their disadvantageous counterparts. A sexual creature would be expected to avoid individuals with unusual features, and to prefer to mate with individuals displaying a predominance of common or [[Averageness|average features]].<ref name=symons>Symons, D. (1979) ''The Evolution of Human Sexuality''. Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> This means that mates displaying [[mutation|mutant]] features are also avoided. This is advantageous because most mutations that manifest themselves as changes in appearance, functionality or behavior, are disadvantageous. Because it is impossible to judge whether a new mutation is beneficial or not, koinophilic animals avoid them all, at the cost of avoiding the occasional beneficial mutation. Thus, koinophilia, although not infallible in its ability to distinguish [[Fitness (biology)|fit]] from unfit mates, is a good strategy when [[Sexual selection|choosing a mate]]. A koinophilic choice ensures that offspring are likely to inherit features that have been successful in the past.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" />


''Koinophilia'' differs from ''[[assortative mating]]'',<ref>Ayala, F.J. (1982) ''Population and Evolutionary Genetics'' p. 60. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. ISBN 0-8053-0315-4</ref><ref>Buss, D.M. (1985) Human mate selection. ''American Scientist'' '''37''', 47-51</ref> where "like prefers like". If like preferred like, [[Leucism|leucistic]] animals (such as white peacocks) would be sexually attracted to one another, and a leucistic subspecies would come into being. Koinophilia predicts that this is unlikely because leucistic animals are attracted to the average in the same way as other animals. Since non-leucistic animals are not attracted by leucism, few leucistic individuals find mates, and leucistic lineages will rarely form.{{efn|An exception could be where leucism offers a major selective advantage, as might happen in snow covered landscapes, where they might readily become a majority in a low density population.}}
''Koinophilia'' differs from ''[[assortative mating]]'',<ref>Ayala, F.J. (1982) ''Population and Evolutionary Genetics'' p. 60. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. ISBN 0-8053-0315-4</ref><ref>Buss, D.M. (1985) Human mate selection. ''American Scientist'' '''37''', 47-51</ref> where "like prefers like". If like preferred like, [[Leucism|leucistic]] animals (such as white peacocks) would be sexually attracted to one another, and a leucistic subspecies would come into being. Koinophilia predicts that this is unlikely because leucistic animals are attracted to the average in the same way as other animals. Since non-leucistic animals are not attracted by leucism, few leucistic individuals find mates, and leucistic lineages will rarely form.{{efn|An exception could be where leucism offers a major selective advantage, as might happen in snow covered landscapes, where they might readily become a majority in a low density population.}}


According to Koeslag, koinophilia provides a simple explanation for such evolutionary puzzles as the process of [[speciation]] (in particular [[Speciation#Darwin's Dilemma|Darwin's Dilemma]]),<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995">Koeslag, J.H. (1995). "On the engine of speciation". [http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=MImg&_imagekey=B6WMD-45S96TH-5-1&_cdi=6932&_user=613892&_orig=search&_coverDate=12%2F21%2F1995&_sk=998229995&view=c&wchp=dGLbVzb-zSkzS&md5=5ca90c43829c1ac4503e69973e9de576&ie=/sdarticle.pdf ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''177''', 401-409]</ref> evolutionary stasis and [[punctuated equilibrium|punctuated equilibria]],<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995" /> and the evolution of [[Co-operation (evolution)|cooperation]].<ref name="Koeslag, 1997">Koeslag, J.H. (1997). "Sex, the prisoner's dilemma game, and the evolutionary inevitability of cooperation." ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''189''', 53–61</ref><ref name="Koeslag, 2003">Koeslag, J.H. (2003). "Evolution of cooperation: cooperation defeats defection in the cornfield model." [http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6WMD-491BYXG-5&_coverDate=10%2F07%2F2003&_alid=109914822&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_qd=1&_cdi=6932&_sort=d&view=c&_acct=C000032099&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=613892&md5=307073e49eb8b5c28621efc7fea4d8fb ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''224''', 399-410]</ref> Koinophilia might also contribute to the ''maintenance'' of [[Evolution of sexual reproduction|sexual reproduction]], preventing its reversion to the much simpler and inherently more advantageous [[Asexual reproduction|asexual form of reproduction]].<ref name="Koeslag, 1994">Koeslag, P.D., Koeslag, J.H. (1994). "Koinophilia stabilizes bi-gender sexual reproduction against asex in an unchanging environment." ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''166''', 251-260</ref><ref name="Koeslag, 1993">Koeslag, J.H., Koeslag, P.D. (1993). "Evolutionarily stable meiotic sex." ''J. Heredity'' '''84''', 396–399</ref>
Koinophilia provides simple explanations for what constitutes a beautiful face,<ref name="Langlois & Roggman, 1990">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L. (1990). "Attractive faces are only average." ''Psychol. Sci.'' '''1''', 115–121</ref><ref name="Langlois 1991">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A., Musselman, L., Acton, S. (1991). "A picture is worth a thousand words: Reply to 'On the difficulty of averaging faces'." ''Psychological Science'' '''2''', 354–357.</ref><ref name="Langlois, 1994">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A., Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about attractive faces? ''Psychological Science'' '''5''', 214–220</ref><ref name="Langlois, 1995">Langlois, J.H., Musselman, L. (1995). The myths and mysteries of beauty. In D.R. Calhoun (Ed.), ''1996 Yearbook of Science and the Future'' , pp. 40–61. Chicago: Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc.</ref><ref name="Kalick">Kalick, S.M., Zebrowitz, L.A., Langlois, J.H., Johnson, R.M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. ''Psychological Science'' '''9''', 8–13</ref><ref name="Rubenstein">Rubenstein, A.J., Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A. (2002). "What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty." In G. Rhodes & L.A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), ''Facial Attractiveness: Evolutionary, Cognitive, and Social Perspectives'': Westport, CT: Ablex</ref><ref name="Hoss">Hoss, R.A., Langlois, J.H. (2003). Infants prefer attractive faces. In O. Pascalis & A. Slater (Eds.), ''The Development of Face Processing in infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives'' pp. 27–38. New York: Nova Science Publishers.</ref> how the individuality of a face is recognized,<ref name=unnikrishnan1 /><ref name=unnikrishnan2 /> [[speciation]] (in particular [[Speciation#Darwin's Dilemma|Darwin's Dilemma]]),<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995">Koeslag, J.H. (1995). "On the engine of speciation". [http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=MImg&_imagekey=B6WMD-45S96TH-5-1&_cdi=6932&_user=613892&_orig=search&_coverDate=12%2F21%2F1995&_sk=998229995&view=c&wchp=dGLbVzb-zSkzS&md5=5ca90c43829c1ac4503e69973e9de576&ie=/sdarticle.pdf ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''177''', 401-409]</ref> evolutionary stasis and [[punctuated equilibrium|punctuated equilibria]],<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name=miller /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995" /> and the evolution of [[Co-operation (evolution)|cooperation]].<ref name="Koeslag, 1997">Koeslag, J.H. (1997). "Sex, the prisoner's dilemma game, and the evolutionary inevitability of cooperation." ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''189''', 53–61</ref><ref name="Koeslag, 2003">Koeslag, J.H. (2003). "Evolution of cooperation: cooperation defeats defection in the cornfield model." [http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6WMD-491BYXG-5&_coverDate=10%2F07%2F2003&_alid=109914822&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_qd=1&_cdi=6932&_sort=d&view=c&_acct=C000032099&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=613892&md5=307073e49eb8b5c28621efc7fea4d8fb ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''224''', 399-410]</ref> Koinophilia might also contribute to the ''maintenance'' of [[Evolution of sexual reproduction|sexual reproduction]], preventing its reversion to the much simpler and inherently more advantageous [[Asexual reproduction|asexual form of reproduction]].<ref name="Koeslag, 1994">Koeslag, P.D., Koeslag, J.H. (1994). "Koinophilia stabilizes bi-gender sexual reproduction against asex in an unchanging environment." ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''166''', 251-260</ref><ref name="Koeslag, 1993">Koeslag, J.H., Koeslag, P.D. (1993). "Evolutionarily stable meiotic sex." ''J. Heredity'' '''84''', 396–399</ref>


As a [[mating strategy]] it was identified in humans by Judith Langlois and her coworkers,<ref name="Langlois & Roggman, 1990" /><ref name="Langlois 1991" /><ref name="Langlois, 1995" /><ref name="Rubenstein" /> who found that the average of two human faces<ref>[http://www.uni-regensburg.de/Fakultaeten/phil_Fak_II/Psychologie/Psy_II/beautycheck/english/morphing/morphing.htm the average of two human faces]</ref> was more attractive than either of the faces from which that average was derived. The more faces (of the same gender and age) that were used in the averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became.<ref>[http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/HomePage/Group/LangloisLAB/averagenessbeauty.html averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became]</ref> In this context “koinophilia” is generally known as '''[[Averageness]]'''.
==Introduction==

The term ''koinophilia'' derives from the Greek, ''koinos'', "the usual", and ''philos'', "fondness". As a [[mating strategy]] it was identified in humans by Judith Langlois and her coworkers,<ref name="Langlois & Roggman, 1990">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L. (1990). "Attractive faces are only average." ''Psychol. Sci.'' '''1''', 115–121</ref><ref name="Langlois 1991">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A., Musselman, L., Acton, S. (1991). "A picture is worth a thousand words: Reply to 'On the difficulty of averaging faces'." ''Psychological Science'' '''2''', 354–357.</ref><ref name="Langlois, 1994">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A., Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about attractive faces? ''Psychological Science'' '''5''', 214–220</ref><ref name="Langlois, 1995">Langlois, J.H., Musselman, L. (1995). The myths and mysteries of beauty. In D.R. Calhoun (Ed.), ''1996 Yearbook of Science and the Future'' , pp. 40–61. Chicago: Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc.</ref><ref name="Kalick">Kalick, S.M., Zebrowitz, L.A., Langlois, J.H., Johnson, R.M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. ''Psychological Science'' '''9''', 8–13</ref><ref name="Rubenstein">Rubenstein, A.J., Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A. (2002). "What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty." In G. Rhodes & L.A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), ''Facial Attractiveness: Evolutionary, Cognitive, and Social Perspectives'': Westport, CT: Ablex</ref><ref name="Hoss">Hoss, R.A., Langlois, J.H. (2003). Infants prefer attractive faces. In O. Pascalis & A. Slater (Eds.), ''The Development of Face Processing in infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives'' pp. 27–38. New York: Nova Science Publishers.</ref> who found that the average of two human faces<ref>[http://www.uni-regensburg.de/Fakultaeten/phil_Fak_II/Psychologie/Psy_II/beautycheck/english/morphing/morphing.htm the average of two human faces]</ref> was more attractive than either of the faces from which that average was derived. The more faces (of the same gender and age) that were used in the averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became.<ref>[http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/HomePage/Group/LangloisLAB/averagenessbeauty.html averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became]</ref>

Koinophilia is based on the idea that evolution is imposed on species or groups. It is not planned or striven for in some [[Lamarckism|Lamarckist]] way.<ref name=GouldHopefulMonster>Gould, S.J. (1980) ''Return of the Hopeful Monster''. in The Panda's Thumb. p. 186-193. New York: W.W. Norton.</ref> [[Mutation]]s are random events, and, except for the "[[silent mutation]]s" which do not affect the functionality or appearance of the carrier, are thus usually disadvantageous. The chance that an unusual feature might prove useful is vanishingly small, and such mutations do not increase an individual's evolutionary [[fitness (biology)|fitness]].<ref name=GouldHopefulMonster/>

While a species or group might benefit from being able to adapt to a new environment by accumulating a wide range of genetic variation, this is to the detriment of the ''individuals'' who have to carry these mutations until a small, unpredictable minority of them ultimately contributes to such an adaptation. Thus, the ''capability'' to evolve would have to be a [[Group selection|group adaptation]], a concept discredited by (for example) [[George C. Williams]],<ref>[[George C. Williams|Williams, G.C.]] (1972) [[Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought]]. Princeton University Press.ISBN 0-691-02357-3</ref> [[John Maynard Smith]]<ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1038/2011145a0 | last1 = Maynard Smith | first1 = J. | authorlink = John Maynard Smith | year = 1964 | title = Group selection and kin selection | url = | journal = [[Nature (journal)|Nature]] | volume = 201 | issue = 4924| pages = 1145–1147 }}</ref> and [[ Richard Dawkins]]<ref name="River_Out_of_Eden">See the chapter ''[[God's utility function]]'' in {{cite book | first = Richard | last = Dawkins | authorlink = Richard Dawkins | title = [[River Out of Eden]] | publisher = Basic Books | location = New York | year = 1995 | isbn = 0-465-06990-8 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Dawkins | first1 = R. | authorlink = Richard Dawkins | year = 1994 | title = Burying the Vehicle Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection | journal = Behavioural and Brain Sciences | volume = 17 | issue = 4| pages = 616–617 | url =http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1994burying_the_vehicle.shtml | archiveurl = http://web.archive.org/web/20060915085010/http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1994burying_the_vehicle.shtml | archivedate = 2006-09-15 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Dennett | first1 = D.C. | authorlink = Daniel Dennett | year = 1994 | title = E Pluribus Unum? Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection | url =http://cogprints.org/281/0/wilsonso.htm | journal = Behavioural and Brain Sciences | volume = 17 | issue = 4| pages = 617–618 | archiveurl=http://web.archive.org/web/20071227065311/http://cogprints.org/281/0/wilsonso.htm | archivedate=2007-12-27 }}</ref> <ref>Pinker, S. (2012). [http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection The False Allure of Group Selection]. Edge, Jun 19, 2012. http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection</ref> as selectively disadvantageous to the individual.


==Physical attractiveness==
==Physical attractiveness==
Line 26: Line 20:
[[File:Jessica Alba Face Proportions.png|thumb|left|A [[University of Toronto]] study found that the facial proportions of celebrities including [[Jessica Alba]] were close to the average of all female profiles.<ref name=twsZE23>{{cite news |author= Fiona Macrae |title= Skin deep: Beautiful faces have Miss Average proportions |publisher= ''Daily Mail'' |quote= All were head shots of the same person with different distances from eyes to mouth or between the eyes. She was at her most attractive when the space between her pupils was just under half, or 46 per cent, of the width of her face from ear to ear. The other perfect dimension was when the distance between her eyes and mouth was just over a third, or 36 per cent, of the overall length of her face from hairline to chin. ... Celebrities whose faces are in perfect proportion include Jessica Alba, Liz Hurley and Shania Twain... Professor Kang Lee, of the University of Toronto, said: "...Our study proves that the structure of faces also contributes to our perception of facial attractiveness." |date= 27 December 2009 |url= http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-1236636/Skin-deep-Beautiful-faces-Miss-Average-proportions.html |accessdate= 2011-07-31}}</ref>]]
[[File:Jessica Alba Face Proportions.png|thumb|left|A [[University of Toronto]] study found that the facial proportions of celebrities including [[Jessica Alba]] were close to the average of all female profiles.<ref name=twsZE23>{{cite news |author= Fiona Macrae |title= Skin deep: Beautiful faces have Miss Average proportions |publisher= ''Daily Mail'' |quote= All were head shots of the same person with different distances from eyes to mouth or between the eyes. She was at her most attractive when the space between her pupils was just under half, or 46 per cent, of the width of her face from ear to ear. The other perfect dimension was when the distance between her eyes and mouth was just over a third, or 36 per cent, of the overall length of her face from hairline to chin. ... Celebrities whose faces are in perfect proportion include Jessica Alba, Liz Hurley and Shania Twain... Professor Kang Lee, of the University of Toronto, said: "...Our study proves that the structure of faces also contributes to our perception of facial attractiveness." |date= 27 December 2009 |url= http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-1236636/Skin-deep-Beautiful-faces-Miss-Average-proportions.html |accessdate= 2011-07-31}}</ref>]]


Despite the novelty of this finding, Galton's observations were forgotten until Judith Langlois and Lori Roggman<ref name="Langlois & Roggman, 1990">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. ''Psychol. Sci.'' '''1''', 115–121.</ref> created computer generated composite images in the late 1980s. They found that facial attractiveness increased in proportion to the number of faces that went into creating the composite. Many studies, using different averaging techniques, including the use of line drawings<ref name="Rhodes & Tremewan, 1997">Rhodes, G., Tremewan, T. (1997) Averageness, exaggeration, and facial attractiveness. ''Psychol. Sci. '' '''7''', 105–110.</ref> and face profiles,<ref name="Valentine, 2004">Valentine, T., Darling, S., Donnelly, M. (2004). Why are average faces attractive? The effect of view and averageness on the attractiveness of the attractiveness of female faces. ''Psychon. Bull. Rev.'' '''11''', 482–487</ref> have subsequently shown that this is a general principle: average faces are consistently more attractive than the faces used to generate them.<ref>[http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/HomePage/Group/LangloisLAB/averagenessbeauty.html average faces are consistently more attractive than the faces used to generate them]</ref><ref name="Rubenstein, 2002">Rubenstein, A.J., Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A. (2002). What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty. In G. Rhodes & L.A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), ''Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives'': Westport, CT: Ablex.</ref><ref name="Grammar, 2003">Grammer, K., Fink, B., Moller, A.P., Thornhill, R. (2003). "Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty." ''Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. '' '''78''', 385–407.</ref><ref name="Rhodes, 2006">Rhodes, G. (2006) "The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty." ''Annu. Rev. Psychol.'' '''57''', 199–226.</ref> Furthermore if a female composite (averaged) face made of 32 different faces is overlain with the face of an extremely attractive female model, the two images often line up closely, indicating that the model's facial configuration is very similar to the composite's.<ref>[http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/HomePage/Group/LangloisLAB/averagenessbeauty.html model's facial configuration is very similar to the composite's facial configuration]</ref> See, for example, the illustration of Jessica Alba on the left.<ref name="Rubenstein, 2002"/><ref name="Grammar, 2003"/>
Despite the novelty of this finding, Galton's observations were forgotten until Judith Langlois and Lori Roggman<ref name="Langlois & Roggman, 1990">Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. ''Psychol. Sci.'' '''1''', 115–121.</ref> created computer generated composite images in 1990. They found that facial attractiveness increased in proportion to the number of faces that went into creating the composite. Many studies, using different averaging techniques, including the use of line drawings<ref name="Rhodes & Tremewan, 1997">Rhodes, G., Tremewan, T. (1997) Averageness, exaggeration, and facial attractiveness. ''Psychol. Sci. '' '''7''', 105–110.</ref> and face profiles,<ref name="Valentine, 2004">Valentine, T., Darling, S., Donnelly, M. (2004). Why are average faces attractive? The effect of view and averageness on the attractiveness of the attractiveness of female faces. ''Psychon. Bull. Rev.'' '''11''', 482–487</ref> have subsequently shown that this is a general principle: average faces are consistently more attractive than the faces used to generate them.<ref>[http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/HomePage/Group/LangloisLAB/averagenessbeauty.html average faces are consistently more attractive than the faces used to generate them]</ref><ref name="Rubenstein, 2002">Rubenstein, A.J., Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A. (2002). What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty. In G. Rhodes & L.A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), ''Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives'': Westport, CT: Ablex.</ref><ref name="Grammar, 2003">Grammer, K., Fink, B., Moller, A.P., Thornhill, R. (2003). "Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty." ''Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. '' '''78''', 385–407.</ref><ref name="Rhodes, 2006">Rhodes, G. (2006) "The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty." ''Annu. Rev. Psychol.'' '''57''', 199–226.</ref> Furthermore if a female composite (averaged) face made of 32 different faces is overlain with the face of an extremely attractive female model, the two images often line up closely, indicating that the model's facial configuration is very similar to the composite's.<ref>[http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/HomePage/Group/LangloisLAB/averagenessbeauty.html model's facial configuration is very similar to the composite's facial configuration]</ref> See, for example, the illustration of Jessica Alba on the left.<ref name="Rubenstein, 2002"/><ref name="Grammar, 2003"/>


[[File:Hadzabe4.jpg|thumb|upright|Transcending culture: [[Hadza people]] rated averaged Hadza faces as more attractive than actual faces from the tribe.]]
[[File:Hadzabe4.jpg|thumb|upright|Transcending culture: [[Hadza people]] rated averaged Hadza faces as more attractive than actual faces from the tribe.]]
This principle transcends culture. For instance, Coren Apicella and her co-workers from Harvard University<ref name= "Apicella, 2007">Apicella, C.L., Little, A.C., Marlowe, F.W. (2007). "Facial averageness and attractiveness in an isolated population of hunter-gatherers." ''Perception'' '''36''', 1813–1820.</ref> created average faces of an isolated [[hunter-gatherer]] tribe of 1,000 in [[Tanzania]], Africa, the [[Hadza people]]. Hadza people rated the averaged Hadza faces as more attractive than the actual faces in the tribe. While Europeans also rated average Hadza faces as attractive, the Hadza people expressed no preference for average European faces. Apicella<ref name="Apicella, 2007"/> attributes this difference to the wider visual experiences of the Europeans, as they had been exposed to both Western and African faces. Thus the indifference of the Hadza towards average European faces could have been the result of lacking the European norm in their visual experience.<ref name="Unnikrishnan">Unnikrishnan, M.K. (2009). How is the individuality of a face recognized? ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''261''', 469-474</ref> These results suggest that the rules for extracting attractive faces are culture-independent and innate, but the results of applying the rules depend on the environment and cultural experience.<ref name="Perrett, 1994"/><ref name="Grammar, 2003"/>
This principle transcends culture. For instance, Coren Apicella and her co-workers from Harvard University<ref name= "Apicella, 2007">Apicella, C.L., Little, A.C., Marlowe, F.W. (2007). "Facial averageness and attractiveness in an isolated population of hunter-gatherers." ''Perception'' '''36''', 1813–1820.</ref> created average faces of an isolated [[hunter-gatherer]] tribe of 1,000 in [[Tanzania]], Africa, the [[Hadza people]]. Hadza people rated the averaged Hadza faces as more attractive than the actual faces in the tribe. While Europeans also rated average Hadza faces as attractive, the Hadza people expressed no preference for average European faces. Apicella<ref name="Apicella, 2007"/> attributes this difference to the wider visual experiences of the Europeans, as they had been exposed to both Western and African faces. Thus the indifference of the Hadza towards average European faces could have been the result of lacking the European norm in their visual experience.<ref name=unnikrishnan1 /> These results suggest that the rules for extracting attractive faces are culture-independent and innate, but the results of applying the rules depend on the environment and cultural experience.<ref name="Perrett, 1994"/><ref name="Grammar, 2003"/>


That the preference for the average is biological rather than cultural has been supported by studies on babies, who gaze longer at attractive faces than at unattractive ones.<ref name="Langlois, 1991">Langlois, J.H., Ritter, J.M., Roggman, L.A., Vaughn, L.S. (1991). "Facial diversity and infant preferences for attractive faces." ''Dev. Psychol.'' '''27''', 79–84.</ref><ref name="Slater, 1998">Slater, A.M., Von Der Schulenburg, C., Brown, E., et al. (1998). "Newborn infants prefer attractive faces." ''Infant Behav. Dev.'' ''' 21''', 345–354.</ref><ref name="Kramer, 1995">Kramer, S., Zebrowitz, L.A., San Giovanni, J.P., Sherak, B. (1995). "Infants' preferences for attractiveness and babyfaceness." In Bardy, B.G., Bootsma, R.J., Guiard, Y. (Eds.) ''Studies in perception and action III''. pp. 389–392. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Associates.</ref> Furthermore, Mark Stauss<ref name="Straus, 1979">Strauss, M.S. (1979). "Abstraction of prototypical information by adults and 10-month-old infants." ''J. Exp. Psychol.: Human Learn. Mem.'' '''5''', 618–632.</ref> reported that 10-month-old children respond to average faces in the same way as they respond to attractive faces, and that these infants can extract the average from simply drawn faces consisting of only 4 features. Adam Rubenstein and coworkers<ref name="Rubenstein, 1999">Rubenstein, A.J, Kalakanis, L., Langlois, J.H. (1999). Infant preferences for attractive faces: a cognitive explanation. ''Dev. Psychol.'' '''35''', 848–855.</ref> showed that already at six months of age, children not only treat average faces the same as they treat attractive faces, but they are also able to extract the central tendency (i.e. the average) from a set of complex, naturalistic faces presented to them (i.e. not just the very simple 4-features faces used by Strauss). Thus the ability to extract the average from a set of realistic facial images operates from an early age, and is therefore almost certainly instinctive.<ref name="Straus, 1979" /><ref name="Rubenstein, 1999" />
That the preference for the average is biological rather than cultural has been supported by studies on babies, who gaze longer at attractive faces than at unattractive ones.<ref name="Langlois, 1991">Langlois, J.H., Ritter, J.M., Roggman, L.A., Vaughn, L.S. (1991). "Facial diversity and infant preferences for attractive faces." ''Dev. Psychol.'' '''27''', 79–84.</ref><ref name="Slater, 1998">Slater, A.M., Von Der Schulenburg, C., Brown, E., et al. (1998). "Newborn infants prefer attractive faces." ''Infant Behav. Dev.'' ''' 21''', 345–354.</ref><ref name="Kramer, 1995">Kramer, S., Zebrowitz, L.A., San Giovanni, J.P., Sherak, B. (1995). "Infants' preferences for attractiveness and babyfaceness." In Bardy, B.G., Bootsma, R.J., Guiard, Y. (Eds.) ''Studies in perception and action III''. pp. 389–392. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Associates.</ref> Furthermore, Mark Stauss<ref name="Straus, 1979">Strauss, M.S. (1979). "Abstraction of prototypical information by adults and 10-month-old infants." ''J. Exp. Psychol.: Human Learn. Mem.'' '''5''', 618–632.</ref> reported that 10-month-old children respond to average faces in the same way as they respond to attractive faces, and that these infants can extract the average from simply drawn faces consisting of only 4 features. Adam Rubenstein and coworkers<ref name="Rubenstein, 1999">Rubenstein, A.J, Kalakanis, L., Langlois, J.H. (1999). Infant preferences for attractive faces: a cognitive explanation. ''Dev. Psychol.'' '''35''', 848–855.</ref> showed that already at six months of age, children not only treat average faces the same as they treat attractive faces, but they are also able to extract the central tendency (i.e. the average) from a set of complex, naturalistic faces presented to them (i.e. not just the very simple 4-features faces used by Strauss). Thus the ability to extract the average from a set of realistic facial images operates from an early age, and is therefore almost certainly instinctive.<ref name="Straus, 1979" /><ref name="Rubenstein, 1999" />
Line 38: Line 32:
{{Main|Speciation#Darwin's Dilemma|Punctuated equilibrium|Living fossils}}
{{Main|Speciation#Darwin's Dilemma|Punctuated equilibrium|Living fossils}}


[[File:Kgalagadi Springbok.JPG|left|thumb|300 px|The overwhelming impression of strict uniformity, involving all the external features of the adult members of a species, is illustrated by this herd of [[Springbok]], ''Antidorcas marsupialis'', in the [[Kalahari Desert]]. This homogeneity in appearance is typical, and virtually diagnostic, of almost all species,<ref name=Hockey>{{cite book|editor1-last=Hockey|editor1-first =P.A.R.|editor2-last=Dean|editor2-first=W.R.J.|editor3-last=Ryan|editor3-first=P.G.|title=Roberts – Birds of Southern Africa, VIIth Ed.|date=2003|publisher=The John Voelcker Bird Book Fund|location=Cape Town|isbn=0620340517|pages=14, 60-1138|}}</ref> and a great evolutionary mystery.<ref name="Gould 1980">Gould, S.J. (1980). "A Quahog is a Quahog." In ''The Panda's Thumb''. pp. 204–213. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.</ref><ref name="Maynard Smith">Maynard Smith, J. (1989). ''Evolutionary Genetics'' p. 273. Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> [[Charles Darwin|Darwin]] emphasized individual variation, which is unquestionably present in any herd such as this, but is extraordinarily difficult to discern, even after long-term familiarity with the herd.<ref>{{cite journal |
[[File:Kgalagadi Springbok.JPG|left|thumb|300 px|The overwhelming impression of strict uniformity, involving all the external features of the adult members of a species, is illustrated by this herd of [[Springbok]], ''Antidorcas marsupialis'', in the [[Kalahari Desert]]. This homogeneity in appearance is typical, and virtually diagnostic, of almost all species,<ref name=Hockey>{{cite book|editor1-last=Hockey|editor1-first =P.A.R.|editor2-last=Dean|editor2-first=W.R.J.|editor3-last=Ryan|editor3-first=P.G.|title=Roberts – Birds of Southern Africa, VIIth Ed.|date=2003|publisher=The John Voelcker Bird Book Fund|location=Cape Town|isbn=0620340517|pages=14, 60-1138|}}</ref> and a great evolutionary mystery.<ref name="Gould 1980">Gould, S.J. (1980). "A Quahog is a Quahog." In ''The Panda's Thumb''. pp. 204–213. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.</ref><ref name="Maynard Smith">Maynard Smith, J. (1989). ''Evolutionary Genetics'' p. 273. Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> [[Charles Darwin|Darwin]] emphasized individual variation, which is unquestionably present in any herd such as this, but is extraordinarily difficult to discern, even after long-term familiarity with the herd. <ref>{{cite journal |
doi = |
author-last1= White |
author-first1=G.C.|
author-last2=Burnham|
author-first2=K.P.|
year = 1999 |
title = Program MARK. Survival estimates from marked animals.|
url = |
journal = Bird Study |
volume =46 |
issue =|
pages = S120-S139 |
}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |
doi = |
doi = |
author-last1= Reynolds |
author-last1= Reynolds |
Line 53: Line 60:
issue =|
issue =|
pages = 106-113 |
pages = 106-113 |
}}</ref>

<ref>{{cite journal |
doi = |
author-last1= White |
author-first1=G.C.|
author-last2=Burnham|
author-first2=K.P.|
year = 1999 |
title = Proram MARK. Survival estimates from marked animals.|
url = |
journal = Bird Study |
volume =46 |
issue =|
pages = S120-S139 |
}}</ref>
}}</ref>
]]
]]
Line 77: Line 69:
This is, however, only one aspect of a two-dimensional problem.<ref name="Bernstein et al. 1985">Bernstein, H., Byerly, H.C., Hopf, F.A., Michod, R.E. (1985). "Sex and the emergence of species." ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''117''' 665–690.</ref><ref name="Hopt 1990">Hopf, F.A. (1990). "Darwin's dilemma of transitional forms: a comparison of model with data." In: ''Organisational Constraints of the Dymnamics of Evolution'' Eds. Maynard Smith, J., Vida, G. pp. 357–372. Manchester: Manchester University Press.</ref> The "horizontal" dimension refers to the almost complete absence of transitional, or intermediate forms between present-day species (e.g., between lions, leopards, cheetahs and lynxes).<ref name="Gould 1980"/><ref name="Gould 1985">Gould, S.J. (1985). ''The Flamingo's Smile''. pp. 160, 169. London: Penguin Books.</ref><ref name="Maynard Smith, 1983">Maynard Smith, J. (1983). "The genetics of stasis and punctuation." ''Ann. rev. Genet.'' '''17''', 11–25.</ref> The "vertical" dimension concerns the fossil record. Fossil species are frequently remarkably stable over extremely long periods of geological time, despite continental drift, major climate changes, and mass extinctions.<ref name="EldredgeN">Eldredge, N., Gould, S.J. (1972). [http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/ridley/classictexts/eldredge.asp "Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism"] In Schopf, T.J.M., ed., ''Models in Paleobiology''. San Francisco: Freeman Cooper. pp. 82–115. Reprinted in Eldredge, N. (1985) ''Time frames''. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press.</ref><ref name="williams 1992">Williams, G.C. (1992). Stasis. In ''Natural Selection: Domains, Levels and Challenges''. pp. 127–142. New York: Oxford University Press.</ref> When a change in form occurs, it tends to be abrupt in geological terms, again producing phenotypic gaps (i.e., an absence of intermediate forms), but now between successive species, which then often co-exist for long periods of time. Thus the fossil record suggests that evolution occurs in bursts, interspersed by long periods of evolutionary stagnation in so-called [[Punctuated equilibrium|punctuated equilibria]].<ref name="EldredgeN" /> Why this is so, has been one of evolution's great mysteries.<ref name="williams 1992"/>
This is, however, only one aspect of a two-dimensional problem.<ref name="Bernstein et al. 1985">Bernstein, H., Byerly, H.C., Hopf, F.A., Michod, R.E. (1985). "Sex and the emergence of species." ''J. Theor. Biol.'' '''117''' 665–690.</ref><ref name="Hopt 1990">Hopf, F.A. (1990). "Darwin's dilemma of transitional forms: a comparison of model with data." In: ''Organisational Constraints of the Dymnamics of Evolution'' Eds. Maynard Smith, J., Vida, G. pp. 357–372. Manchester: Manchester University Press.</ref> The "horizontal" dimension refers to the almost complete absence of transitional, or intermediate forms between present-day species (e.g., between lions, leopards, cheetahs and lynxes).<ref name="Gould 1980"/><ref name="Gould 1985">Gould, S.J. (1985). ''The Flamingo's Smile''. pp. 160, 169. London: Penguin Books.</ref><ref name="Maynard Smith, 1983">Maynard Smith, J. (1983). "The genetics of stasis and punctuation." ''Ann. rev. Genet.'' '''17''', 11–25.</ref> The "vertical" dimension concerns the fossil record. Fossil species are frequently remarkably stable over extremely long periods of geological time, despite continental drift, major climate changes, and mass extinctions.<ref name="EldredgeN">Eldredge, N., Gould, S.J. (1972). [http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/ridley/classictexts/eldredge.asp "Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism"] In Schopf, T.J.M., ed., ''Models in Paleobiology''. San Francisco: Freeman Cooper. pp. 82–115. Reprinted in Eldredge, N. (1985) ''Time frames''. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press.</ref><ref name="williams 1992">Williams, G.C. (1992). Stasis. In ''Natural Selection: Domains, Levels and Challenges''. pp. 127–142. New York: Oxford University Press.</ref> When a change in form occurs, it tends to be abrupt in geological terms, again producing phenotypic gaps (i.e., an absence of intermediate forms), but now between successive species, which then often co-exist for long periods of time. Thus the fossil record suggests that evolution occurs in bursts, interspersed by long periods of evolutionary stagnation in so-called [[Punctuated equilibrium|punctuated equilibria]].<ref name="EldredgeN" /> Why this is so, has been one of evolution's great mysteries.<ref name="williams 1992"/>


Koinophilia could explain both the horizontal and vertical manifestations of [[speciation]], and why it usually involves the entire external appearance of the creatures concerned.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995"/> Since koinophilia affects the entire external appearance, the members of an interbreeding group are driven to look alike in every detail.<ref>Stuart, C. & Stuart, T. (2007). ''Field guide to mammals of Southern Africa''. pp. 174–175. Cape Town, Struik. ISBN 978-1-77007-404-0</ref> Each interbreeding group will rapidly develop its own characteristic appearance. An individual from one group which wanders into another group will consequently be recognized as different, and will be discriminated against during the mating season. This koinophilia-induced reproductive isolation might thus be the first crucial step in the development of, ultimately, physiological, anatomical and behavioral barriers to hybridization, and thus, ultimately, full specieshood. Koinophilia will thereafter defend that species' appearance and behavior against invasion by unusual or unfamiliar forms (which might arise by immigration or mutation), and thus be a paradigm of punctuated equilibria (or the "vertical" aspect of the speciation problem<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995"/>), and an explanation for the existence of many "[[living fossils]]" (i.e., creatures that have remained almost unchanged in appearance for, sometimes, hundreds of millions of years, surviving [[extinction event|mass extinctions]], alternating periods of global warming and glaciation, as well as extensive remodeling of the earth's geography through [[continental drift]]).<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" />
Koinophilia could explain both the horizontal and vertical manifestations of [[speciation]], and why it usually involves the entire external appearance of the creatures concerned.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name=miller /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995"/> Since koinophilia affects the entire external appearance, the members of an interbreeding group are driven to look alike in every detail.<ref>Stuart, C. & Stuart, T. (2007). ''Field guide to mammals of Southern Africa''. pp. 174–175. Cape Town, Struik. ISBN 978-1-77007-404-0</ref><ref>Hockey, P.A.R., Dean, W.R.J., Ryan, P.G. (eds.) (2005) ''Roberts Birds of Southern Africa'', p. 14, 60-1138. Trustees of the John Voelcker Bird Book Fund, Cape Town.</ref> Each interbreeding group will rapidly develop its own characteristic appearance.<ref name=miller /> An individual from one group which wanders into another group will consequently be recognized as different, and will be discriminated against during the mating season. This koinophilia-induced reproductive isolation might thus be the first crucial step in the development of, ultimately, physiological, anatomical and behavioral barriers to hybridization, and thus, ultimately, full specieshood. Koinophilia will thereafter defend that species' appearance and behavior against invasion by unusual or unfamiliar forms (which might arise by immigration or mutation), and thus be a paradigm of punctuated equilibria (or the "vertical" aspect of the speciation problem<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Koeslag, 1995"/>), and an explanation for the existence of many "[[living fossils]]" (i.e., creatures that have remained almost unchanged in appearance for, sometimes, hundreds of millions of years, surviving [[extinction event|mass extinctions]], alternating periods of global warming and glaciation, as well as extensive remodeling of the earth's geography through [[continental drift]]).<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" />


==Rate of evolution==
==Rate of evolution==
Line 127: Line 119:
<blockquote>''The question of evolutionary change in relation to available geological time is indeed a serious theoretical challenge, but the reasons are exactly the opposite of that inspired by most people’s intuition. Organisms in general have not done nearly as much evolving as we should reasonably expect. Long term rates of change, even in lineages of unusual rapid evolution, are almost always far slower than they theoretically could be. The basis for such expectation is to be found most clearly in observed rates of evolution under artificial selection, along with the often high rates of change in environmental conditions that must imply rapid change in intensity and direction of selection in nature''.<ref name="williams 1992b" /></blockquote>
<blockquote>''The question of evolutionary change in relation to available geological time is indeed a serious theoretical challenge, but the reasons are exactly the opposite of that inspired by most people’s intuition. Organisms in general have not done nearly as much evolving as we should reasonably expect. Long term rates of change, even in lineages of unusual rapid evolution, are almost always far slower than they theoretically could be. The basis for such expectation is to be found most clearly in observed rates of evolution under artificial selection, along with the often high rates of change in environmental conditions that must imply rapid change in intensity and direction of selection in nature''.<ref name="williams 1992b" /></blockquote>


Evolution is imposed on species or groups. It is not planned or striven for in some [[Lamarckism|Lamarckist]] way.<ref name=GouldHopefulMonster>Gould, S.J. (1980) ''Return of the Hopeful Monster''. in The Panda's Thumb. p. 186-193. New York: W.W. Norton.</ref> The [[mutation]]s on which the process depends are random events, and, except for the "[[silent mutation]]s" which do not affect the functionality or appearance of the carrier, are thus usually disadvantageous, and their chance of proving to be useful in the future is vanishingly small.
If, as a rule, creatures avoid mates with strange or unusual characteristics, then mutations that affect the external appearance of their carriers will seldom be passed on to the next and subsequent generations. They will therefore seldom be tested by natural selection. Evolution is, therefore, effectively halted or slowed down considerably. The only mutations that can accumulate in a population are ones that have no noticeable effect on the outward appearance and functionality of their bearers (i.e., they are "[[silent mutations|silent]]" or "[[neutral theory of molecular evolution|neutral mutations]]", which can be, and are, used to trace the [[history of molecular evolution|relatedness and age of populations and species]].<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Campbell">Campbell, N.A. (1990) ''Biology'' p. 450–451, 487–490, 499–501. Redwood City CA: Benjamin Cummings Publishing Company.</ref>)
Therefore, while a species or group might benefit from being able to adapt to a new environment by accumulating a wide range of genetic variation, this is to the detriment of the ''individuals'' who have to carry these mutations until a small, unpredictable minority of them ultimately contributes to such an adaptation. Thus, the ''capability'' to evolve is a [[Group selection|group adaptation]], a concept discredited by (for example) [[George C. Williams]],<ref>[[George C. Williams|Williams, G.C.]] (1972) [[Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought]]. Princeton University Press.ISBN 0-691-02357-3</ref> [[John Maynard Smith]]<ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1038/2011145a0 | last1 = Maynard Smith | first1 = J. | authorlink = John Maynard Smith | year = 1964 | title = Group selection and kin selection | url = | journal = [[Nature (journal)|Nature]] | volume = 201 | issue = 4924| pages = 1145–1147 }}</ref> and [[ Richard Dawkins]]<ref name="River_Out_of_Eden">See the chapter ''[[God's utility function]]'' in {{cite book | first = Richard | last = Dawkins | authorlink = Richard Dawkins | title = [[River Out of Eden]] | publisher = Basic Books | location = New York | year = 1995 | isbn = 0-465-06990-8 }}</ref><ref name=dawkins>{{cite journal | last1 = Dawkins | first1 = R. | authorlink = Richard Dawkins | year = 1994 | title = Burying the Vehicle Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection | journal = Behavioural and Brain Sciences | volume = 17 | issue = 4| pages = 616–617 | url =http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1994burying_the_vehicle.shtml | archiveurl = http://web.archive.org/web/20060915085010/http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1994burying_the_vehicle.shtml | archivedate = 2006-09-15 }}</ref><ref name=dennet>{{cite journal | last1 = Dennett | first1 = D.C. | authorlink = Daniel Dennett | year = 1994 | title = E Pluribus Unum? Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection | url =http://cogprints.org/281/0/wilsonso.htm | journal = Behavioural and Brain Sciences | volume = 17 | issue = 4| pages = 617–618 | archiveurl=http://web.archive.org/web/20071227065311/http://cogprints.org/281/0/wilsonso.htm | archivedate=2007-12-27 }}</ref><ref name=pinker>Pinker, S. (2012). [http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection The False Allure of Group Selection]. Edge, Jun 19, 2012. http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection</ref> as selectively disadvantageous to the individual.
.

If sexual creatures avoid mates with strange or unusual characteristics,<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name=miller /><ref name=Symons /><ref name="Langlois & Roggman, 1990" /> then mutations that affect the external appearance of their carriers will seldom be passed on to the next and subsequent generations. They will therefore seldom be tested by natural selection. Evolution is, therefore, effectively halted or slowed down considerably. The only mutations that can accumulate in a population are ones that have no noticeable effect on the outward appearance and functionality of their bearers (i.e., they are "[[silent mutations|silent]]" or "[[neutral theory of molecular evolution|neutral mutations]]", which can be, and are, used to trace the [[history of molecular evolution|relatedness and age of populations and species]].<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /><ref name="Campbell">Campbell, N.A. (1990) ''Biology'' p. 450–451, 487–490, 499–501. Redwood City CA: Benjamin Cummings Publishing Company.</ref>)


This implies that evolution can only occur if mutant mates cannot be avoided, as a result of a severe scarcity of potential mates. This is most likely to occur in [[small population size|small, isolated communities]]. These occur most commonly on small islands, in remote valleys, lakes, river systems, or caves,<ref name="ayala">Ayala, F.J. (1982) ''Population and Evolutionary Genetics'' pp. 73–83, 182–190, 198–215. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. ISBN 0-8053-0315-4</ref> or during the aftermath of a [[mass extinction]].<ref name="Campbell" /> Under these circumstances, not only is the choice of mates severely restricted but [[population bottlenecks]], [[founder effects]], [[genetic drift]] and [[inbreeding]] cause rapid, random changes in the isolated population's genetic composition.<ref name="ayala" /> Furthermore, [[Hybrid (biology)|hybridization]] with a related species trapped in the same isolate might introduce additional genetic changes. If an isolated population such as this [[Toba catastrophe theory#Genetic bottleneck theory|survives its genetic upheavals]], and subsequently expands into an unoccupied niche, or into a niche in which it has an advantage over its competitors, a new species, or subspecies, will have come in being. In geological terms this will be an abrupt event. A resumption of avoiding mutant mates will, thereafter, result, once again, in evolutionary stagnation.
This implies that evolution can only occur if mutant mates cannot be avoided, as a result of a severe scarcity of potential mates. This is most likely to occur in [[small population size|small, isolated communities]]. These occur most commonly on small islands, in remote valleys, lakes, river systems, or caves,<ref name="ayala">Ayala, F.J. (1982) ''Population and Evolutionary Genetics'' pp. 73–83, 182–190, 198–215. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. ISBN 0-8053-0315-4</ref> or during the aftermath of a [[mass extinction]].<ref name="Campbell" /> Under these circumstances, not only is the choice of mates severely restricted but [[population bottlenecks]], [[founder effects]], [[genetic drift]] and [[inbreeding]] cause rapid, random changes in the isolated population's genetic composition.<ref name="ayala" /> Furthermore, [[Hybrid (biology)|hybridization]] with a related species trapped in the same isolate might introduce additional genetic changes. If an isolated population such as this [[Toba catastrophe theory#Genetic bottleneck theory|survives its genetic upheavals]], and subsequently expands into an unoccupied niche, or into a niche in which it has an advantage over its competitors, a new species, or subspecies, will have come in being. In geological terms this will be an abrupt event. A resumption of avoiding mutant mates will, thereafter, result, once again, in evolutionary stagnation.
Line 142: Line 138:
[[Image:Canis lupus pack surrounding Bison.jpg|thumb|Cooperative hunting by wolves allows them to tackle much larger and more nutritious prey than any individual wolf could handle. However, such cooperation could, potentially, be exploited by selfish individuals who do not expose themselves to the dangers of the hunt, but nevertheless share in the spoils.]]
[[Image:Canis lupus pack surrounding Bison.jpg|thumb|Cooperative hunting by wolves allows them to tackle much larger and more nutritious prey than any individual wolf could handle. However, such cooperation could, potentially, be exploited by selfish individuals who do not expose themselves to the dangers of the hunt, but nevertheless share in the spoils.]]


Cooperation is any group behavior that benefits the individuals more than if they were to act as independent agents. In consequence, selfish individuals who benefit even more by not taking part in the group activity, but still enjoy its benefits, can exploit the cooperativeness of others. For instance, a selfish individual which does not join the hunting pack and share its risks, but nevertheless shares in the spoils, has a fitness advantage over the other members of the pack. Thus, although a group of cooperative individuals is fitter than an equivalent group of selfish individuals, selfish individuals interspersed amongst a community of cooperators are always fitter than their hosts. They will raise, on average, more offspring than their hosts, and will ultimately replace them.<ref name="River_Out_of_Eden">See the chapter ''[[God's utility function]]'' in {{cite book | first = Richard | last = Dawkins | authorlink = Richard Dawkins | title = [[River Out of Eden]] | publisher = Basic Books | location = New York | year = 1995 | isbn = 0-465-06990-8 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Dawkins | first1 = R. | authorlink = Richard Dawkins | year = 1994 | title = Burying the Vehicle Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection | journal = Behavioural and Brain Sciences | volume = 17 | issue = 4| pages = 616–617 | url =http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1994burying_the_vehicle.shtml | archiveurl = http://web.archive.org/web/20060915085010/http://www.simonyi.ox.ac.uk/dawkins/WorldOfDawkins-archive/Dawkins/Work/Articles/1994burying_the_vehicle.shtml | archivedate = 2006-09-15 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Dennett | first1 = D.C. | authorlink = Daniel Dennett | year = 1994 | title = E Pluribus Unum? Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection | url =http://cogprints.org/281/0/wilsonso.htm | journal = Behavioural and Brain Sciences | volume = 17 | issue = 4| pages = 617–618 | archiveurl=http://web.archive.org/web/20071227065311/http://cogprints.org/281/0/wilsonso.htm | archivedate=2007-12-27 }}</ref><ref>Pinker, S. (2012). [http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection The False Allure of Group Selection]. Edge, Jun 19, 2012. http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection</ref>
Cooperation is any group behavior that benefits the individuals more than if they were to act as independent agents. In consequence, selfish individuals who benefit even more by not taking part in the group activity, but still enjoy its benefits, can exploit the cooperativeness of others. For instance, a selfish individual which does not join the hunting pack and share its risks, but nevertheless shares in the spoils, has a fitness advantage over the other members of the pack. Thus, although a group of cooperative individuals is fitter than an equivalent group of selfish individuals, selfish individuals interspersed amongst a community of cooperators are always fitter than their hosts. They will raise, on average, more offspring than their hosts, and will ultimately replace them.<ref name="River_Out_of_Eden" /><ref name=dawkins /><ref name=dennet /><ref name=pinker />


If, however, the selfish individuals are ostracized, and rejected as mates, because of their deviant and unusual behavior, then their evolutionary advantage becomes an evolutionary liability.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /> Cooperation then becomes [[Evolutionarily stable strategy|evolutionarily stable]]. Sociability, social conventions, ritualistic behavior, the expressions of the emotions, and other forms of communication between individuals can all be similarly [[Evolutionarily stable strategy|evolutionarily stabilized]] by koinophilia.<ref name="Koeslag, 1997" /><ref name= "Koeslag, 2003" />
If, however, the selfish individuals are ostracized, and rejected as mates, because of their deviant and unusual behavior, then their evolutionary advantage becomes an evolutionary liability.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990" /> Cooperation then becomes [[Evolutionarily stable strategy|evolutionarily stable]]. Sociability, social conventions, ritualistic behavior, the expressions of the emotions, and other forms of communication between individuals can all be similarly [[Evolutionarily stable strategy|evolutionarily stabilized]] by koinophilia.<ref name="Koeslag, 1997" /><ref name= "Koeslag, 2003" />

Revision as of 05:35, 15 April 2015

This leucistic peacock is unlikely to reproduce because it is unlikely to find a mate.

Koinophilia occurs when mate-seeking animals preferentially choose individuals with a minimum of unusual features.[1][2][3][4] The term derives from the Greek, koinos, "the usual", and philos, "fondness".

Natural selection causes beneficial inherited features to become more common and eventually to replace their disadvantageous counterparts. A sexual creature would be expected to avoid individuals with unusual features, and to prefer to mate with individuals displaying a predominance of common or average features.[5] This means that mates displaying mutant features are also avoided. This is advantageous because most mutations that manifest themselves as changes in appearance, functionality or behavior, are disadvantageous. Because it is impossible to judge whether a new mutation is beneficial or not, koinophilic animals avoid them all, at the cost of avoiding the occasional beneficial mutation. Thus, koinophilia, although not infallible in its ability to distinguish fit from unfit mates, is a good strategy when choosing a mate. A koinophilic choice ensures that offspring are likely to inherit features that have been successful in the past.[1]

Koinophilia differs from assortative mating,[6][7] where "like prefers like". If like preferred like, leucistic animals (such as white peacocks) would be sexually attracted to one another, and a leucistic subspecies would come into being. Koinophilia predicts that this is unlikely because leucistic animals are attracted to the average in the same way as other animals. Since non-leucistic animals are not attracted by leucism, few leucistic individuals find mates, and leucistic lineages will rarely form.[a]

Koinophilia provides simple explanations for what constitutes a beautiful face,[8][9][10][11][12][13][14] how the individuality of a face is recognized,[3][4] speciation (in particular Darwin's Dilemma),[1][15] evolutionary stasis and punctuated equilibria,[1][2][15] and the evolution of cooperation.[16][17] Koinophilia might also contribute to the maintenance of sexual reproduction, preventing its reversion to the much simpler and inherently more advantageous asexual form of reproduction.[18][19]

As a mating strategy it was identified in humans by Judith Langlois and her coworkers,[8][9][11][13] who found that the average of two human faces[20] was more attractive than either of the faces from which that average was derived. The more faces (of the same gender and age) that were used in the averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became.[21] In this context “koinophilia” is generally known as Averageness.

Physical attractiveness

Francis Galton, a half cousin of Charles Darwin, created composite portraits of convicted criminals, hoping to generate a prototypical criminal face. Surprisingly, the composite portrait became more and more attractive with the addition of each new face. Galton published this finding in 1878, concluding that average features combined to create good-looking faces.[22]

A University of Toronto study found that the facial proportions of celebrities including Jessica Alba were close to the average of all female profiles.[23]

Despite the novelty of this finding, Galton's observations were forgotten until Judith Langlois and Lori Roggman[8] created computer generated composite images in 1990. They found that facial attractiveness increased in proportion to the number of faces that went into creating the composite. Many studies, using different averaging techniques, including the use of line drawings[24] and face profiles,[25] have subsequently shown that this is a general principle: average faces are consistently more attractive than the faces used to generate them.[26][27][28][29] Furthermore if a female composite (averaged) face made of 32 different faces is overlain with the face of an extremely attractive female model, the two images often line up closely, indicating that the model's facial configuration is very similar to the composite's.[30] See, for example, the illustration of Jessica Alba on the left.[27][28]

Transcending culture: Hadza people rated averaged Hadza faces as more attractive than actual faces from the tribe.

This principle transcends culture. For instance, Coren Apicella and her co-workers from Harvard University[31] created average faces of an isolated hunter-gatherer tribe of 1,000 in Tanzania, Africa, the Hadza people. Hadza people rated the averaged Hadza faces as more attractive than the actual faces in the tribe. While Europeans also rated average Hadza faces as attractive, the Hadza people expressed no preference for average European faces. Apicella[31] attributes this difference to the wider visual experiences of the Europeans, as they had been exposed to both Western and African faces. Thus the indifference of the Hadza towards average European faces could have been the result of lacking the European norm in their visual experience.[3] These results suggest that the rules for extracting attractive faces are culture-independent and innate, but the results of applying the rules depend on the environment and cultural experience.[32][28]

That the preference for the average is biological rather than cultural has been supported by studies on babies, who gaze longer at attractive faces than at unattractive ones.[33][34][35] Furthermore, Mark Stauss[36] reported that 10-month-old children respond to average faces in the same way as they respond to attractive faces, and that these infants can extract the average from simply drawn faces consisting of only 4 features. Adam Rubenstein and coworkers[37] showed that already at six months of age, children not only treat average faces the same as they treat attractive faces, but they are also able to extract the central tendency (i.e. the average) from a set of complex, naturalistic faces presented to them (i.e. not just the very simple 4-features faces used by Strauss). Thus the ability to extract the average from a set of realistic facial images operates from an early age, and is therefore almost certainly instinctive.[36][37]

Despite these findings, David Perrett and his colleagues[32] found that both men and women considered that a face averaged from a set of attractive faces was more appealing than one averaged from a wide range of women's faces. When the differences between the first face and the second face were slightly exaggerated the new face was judged, on average, to be more attractive still. The three faces are difficult to distinguish one from the other, although close examination shows that the so-called "exaggerated face" looks slightly younger than the average face (composed of women's faces aged 22–46 years). Since the same results were obtained using Japanese subjects and viewers, these findings are probably culture-independent, indicating that people generally find youthful average faces[38] sexually the most attractive.[32]

Speciation and punctuated equilibria

The overwhelming impression of strict uniformity, involving all the external features of the adult members of a species, is illustrated by this herd of Springbok, Antidorcas marsupialis, in the Kalahari Desert. This homogeneity in appearance is typical, and virtually diagnostic, of almost all species,[39] and a great evolutionary mystery.[40][41] Darwin emphasized individual variation, which is unquestionably present in any herd such as this, but is extraordinarily difficult to discern, even after long-term familiarity with the herd. [42][43]

Biologists from Darwin onwards have puzzled over how evolution produces species whose adult members look extraordinarily similar, and distinctively different from the members of other species. Lions and leopards are, for instance, both large carnivores that inhabit the same general environment, and hunt much the same prey, but look quite different. The question is why intermediates do not exist.[40][41]

Speciation poses a "2-dimensional" problem. The discontinuities in appearance between existing species represent the "horizontal dimension" of the problem. The succession of fossil species represent the "vertical dimension".

This is, however, only one aspect of a two-dimensional problem.[44][45] The "horizontal" dimension refers to the almost complete absence of transitional, or intermediate forms between present-day species (e.g., between lions, leopards, cheetahs and lynxes).[40][46][47] The "vertical" dimension concerns the fossil record. Fossil species are frequently remarkably stable over extremely long periods of geological time, despite continental drift, major climate changes, and mass extinctions.[48][49] When a change in form occurs, it tends to be abrupt in geological terms, again producing phenotypic gaps (i.e., an absence of intermediate forms), but now between successive species, which then often co-exist for long periods of time. Thus the fossil record suggests that evolution occurs in bursts, interspersed by long periods of evolutionary stagnation in so-called punctuated equilibria.[48] Why this is so, has been one of evolution's great mysteries.[49]

Koinophilia could explain both the horizontal and vertical manifestations of speciation, and why it usually involves the entire external appearance of the creatures concerned.[1][2][15] Since koinophilia affects the entire external appearance, the members of an interbreeding group are driven to look alike in every detail.[50][51] Each interbreeding group will rapidly develop its own characteristic appearance.[2] An individual from one group which wanders into another group will consequently be recognized as different, and will be discriminated against during the mating season. This koinophilia-induced reproductive isolation might thus be the first crucial step in the development of, ultimately, physiological, anatomical and behavioral barriers to hybridization, and thus, ultimately, full specieshood. Koinophilia will thereafter defend that species' appearance and behavior against invasion by unusual or unfamiliar forms (which might arise by immigration or mutation), and thus be a paradigm of punctuated equilibria (or the "vertical" aspect of the speciation problem[1][15]), and an explanation for the existence of many "living fossils" (i.e., creatures that have remained almost unchanged in appearance for, sometimes, hundreds of millions of years, surviving mass extinctions, alternating periods of global warming and glaciation, as well as extensive remodeling of the earth's geography through continental drift).[1]

Rate of evolution

Some domestic animals that are significantly different from their wild ancestors
Dog
Pekin Ducks.
Domestic turkeys
Sheep
Alpaca
Zebu cattle
Domestic rabbit
Domestic pigeons
Guppy
Gold fish


Humans have created a wide range of new species, and varieties within those species, of both domesticated animals and plants in a very short geological period of time, spanning only a few tens of thousands of years, and sometimes less. Maize, Zea mays, for instance, is estimated to have been created in what is now known as Mexico in only a few thousand years, starting between about 7 000 and 12 000 years ago, from still uncertain origins.[52] In the light of this extraordinarily rapid rate of evolution, through (prehistoric) artificial selection, George C. Williams[53] and others,[48][54][55] have remarked the following:

The question of evolutionary change in relation to available geological time is indeed a serious theoretical challenge, but the reasons are exactly the opposite of that inspired by most people’s intuition. Organisms in general have not done nearly as much evolving as we should reasonably expect. Long term rates of change, even in lineages of unusual rapid evolution, are almost always far slower than they theoretically could be. The basis for such expectation is to be found most clearly in observed rates of evolution under artificial selection, along with the often high rates of change in environmental conditions that must imply rapid change in intensity and direction of selection in nature.[53]

Evolution is imposed on species or groups. It is not planned or striven for in some Lamarckist way.[56] The mutations on which the process depends are random events, and, except for the "silent mutations" which do not affect the functionality or appearance of the carrier, are thus usually disadvantageous, and their chance of proving to be useful in the future is vanishingly small. Therefore, while a species or group might benefit from being able to adapt to a new environment by accumulating a wide range of genetic variation, this is to the detriment of the individuals who have to carry these mutations until a small, unpredictable minority of them ultimately contributes to such an adaptation. Thus, the capability to evolve is a group adaptation, a concept discredited by (for example) George C. Williams,[57] John Maynard Smith[58] and Richard Dawkins[59][60][61][62] as selectively disadvantageous to the individual. .

If sexual creatures avoid mates with strange or unusual characteristics,[1][2][63][8] then mutations that affect the external appearance of their carriers will seldom be passed on to the next and subsequent generations. They will therefore seldom be tested by natural selection. Evolution is, therefore, effectively halted or slowed down considerably. The only mutations that can accumulate in a population are ones that have no noticeable effect on the outward appearance and functionality of their bearers (i.e., they are "silent" or "neutral mutations", which can be, and are, used to trace the relatedness and age of populations and species.[1][64])

This implies that evolution can only occur if mutant mates cannot be avoided, as a result of a severe scarcity of potential mates. This is most likely to occur in small, isolated communities. These occur most commonly on small islands, in remote valleys, lakes, river systems, or caves,[65] or during the aftermath of a mass extinction.[64] Under these circumstances, not only is the choice of mates severely restricted but population bottlenecks, founder effects, genetic drift and inbreeding cause rapid, random changes in the isolated population's genetic composition.[65] Furthermore, hybridization with a related species trapped in the same isolate might introduce additional genetic changes. If an isolated population such as this survives its genetic upheavals, and subsequently expands into an unoccupied niche, or into a niche in which it has an advantage over its competitors, a new species, or subspecies, will have come in being. In geological terms this will be an abrupt event. A resumption of avoiding mutant mates will, thereafter, result, once again, in evolutionary stagnation.

Thus the fossil record of an evolutionary progression typically consists of species that suddenly appear, and ultimately disappear, in many cases close to a million years later, without any change in external appearance.[64][66][67] Graphically, these fossil species are represented by horizontal lines, whose lengths depict how long each of them existed. The horizontality of the lines illustrates the unchanging appearance of each of the fossil species depicted on the graph. During each species' existence new species appear at random intervals, each also lasting many hundreds of thousands of years before disappearing without a change in appearance. The exact relatedness of these concurrent species is generally impossible to determine. This is illustrated in the following diagram depicting the evolution of modern humans from the time that the Hominins separated from the line that led to the evolution of our closest living primate relatives, the chimpanzees.[67]

Human evolution/Species chart

For similar evolutionary time lines see, for instance, the paleontological list of African dinosaurs, Asian dinosaurs, the Lampriformes and Amiiformes.

Evolution of cooperation

Cooperative hunting by wolves allows them to tackle much larger and more nutritious prey than any individual wolf could handle. However, such cooperation could, potentially, be exploited by selfish individuals who do not expose themselves to the dangers of the hunt, but nevertheless share in the spoils.

Cooperation is any group behavior that benefits the individuals more than if they were to act as independent agents. In consequence, selfish individuals who benefit even more by not taking part in the group activity, but still enjoy its benefits, can exploit the cooperativeness of others. For instance, a selfish individual which does not join the hunting pack and share its risks, but nevertheless shares in the spoils, has a fitness advantage over the other members of the pack. Thus, although a group of cooperative individuals is fitter than an equivalent group of selfish individuals, selfish individuals interspersed amongst a community of cooperators are always fitter than their hosts. They will raise, on average, more offspring than their hosts, and will ultimately replace them.[59][60][61][62]

If, however, the selfish individuals are ostracized, and rejected as mates, because of their deviant and unusual behavior, then their evolutionary advantage becomes an evolutionary liability.[1] Cooperation then becomes evolutionarily stable. Sociability, social conventions, ritualistic behavior, the expressions of the emotions, and other forms of communication between individuals can all be similarly evolutionarily stabilized by koinophilia.[16][17]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ An exception could be where leucism offers a major selective advantage, as might happen in snow covered landscapes, where they might readily become a majority in a low density population.

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Koeslag, J.H. (1990). "Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour." J. theor. Biol. 144, 15–35
  2. ^ a b c d e Miller, W.B. (2013). "What is the big deal about evolutionary gaps?". In: The Microcosm within: Evolution and Extinction in the Hologenome. Boca Raton, Florida.: Universal Publishers. pp. 395–396. ISBN 10:1-61233-2773. {{cite book}}: Check |isbn= value: invalid character (help)
  3. ^ a b c Unnikrishnan, M.K. (2009). "How is the individuality of a face recognized?". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 261 (3): 469–474. doi:10.1016/ j.jtbi.2009.08.011. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  4. ^ a b Unnikrishnan, M.K. (2012). "Koinophilia revisited: the evolutionary link between mate selection and face recognition". Current Science. 102 (4): 563–570. doi:10.1016/ j.jtbi.2009.08.011. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  5. ^ Symons, D. (1979) The Evolution of Human Sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  6. ^ Ayala, F.J. (1982) Population and Evolutionary Genetics p. 60. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. ISBN 0-8053-0315-4
  7. ^ Buss, D.M. (1985) Human mate selection. American Scientist 37, 47-51
  8. ^ a b c d Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L. (1990). "Attractive faces are only average." Psychol. Sci. 1, 115–121 Cite error: The named reference "Langlois & Roggman, 1990" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  9. ^ a b Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A., Musselman, L., Acton, S. (1991). "A picture is worth a thousand words: Reply to 'On the difficulty of averaging faces'." Psychological Science 2, 354–357.
  10. ^ Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A., Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about attractive faces? Psychological Science 5, 214–220
  11. ^ a b Langlois, J.H., Musselman, L. (1995). The myths and mysteries of beauty. In D.R. Calhoun (Ed.), 1996 Yearbook of Science and the Future , pp. 40–61. Chicago: Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc.
  12. ^ Kalick, S.M., Zebrowitz, L.A., Langlois, J.H., Johnson, R.M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. Psychological Science 9, 8–13
  13. ^ a b Rubenstein, A.J., Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A. (2002). "What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty." In G. Rhodes & L.A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial Attractiveness: Evolutionary, Cognitive, and Social Perspectives: Westport, CT: Ablex
  14. ^ Hoss, R.A., Langlois, J.H. (2003). Infants prefer attractive faces. In O. Pascalis & A. Slater (Eds.), The Development of Face Processing in infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives pp. 27–38. New York: Nova Science Publishers.
  15. ^ a b c d Koeslag, J.H. (1995). "On the engine of speciation". J. Theor. Biol. 177, 401-409
  16. ^ a b Koeslag, J.H. (1997). "Sex, the prisoner's dilemma game, and the evolutionary inevitability of cooperation." J. Theor. Biol. 189, 53–61
  17. ^ a b Koeslag, J.H. (2003). "Evolution of cooperation: cooperation defeats defection in the cornfield model." J. theor. Biol. 224, 399-410
  18. ^ Koeslag, P.D., Koeslag, J.H. (1994). "Koinophilia stabilizes bi-gender sexual reproduction against asex in an unchanging environment." J. Theor. Biol. 166, 251-260
  19. ^ Koeslag, J.H., Koeslag, P.D. (1993). "Evolutionarily stable meiotic sex." J. Heredity 84, 396–399
  20. ^ the average of two human faces
  21. ^ averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became
  22. ^ Galton, F. (1878). Composite portraits, made by combining those of many different persons in a single resultant figure. J. Anthropol. Inst. 8, 132–144.
  23. ^ Fiona Macrae (27 December 2009). "Skin deep: Beautiful faces have Miss Average proportions". Daily Mail. Retrieved 2011-07-31. All were head shots of the same person with different distances from eyes to mouth or between the eyes. She was at her most attractive when the space between her pupils was just under half, or 46 per cent, of the width of her face from ear to ear. The other perfect dimension was when the distance between her eyes and mouth was just over a third, or 36 per cent, of the overall length of her face from hairline to chin. ... Celebrities whose faces are in perfect proportion include Jessica Alba, Liz Hurley and Shania Twain... Professor Kang Lee, of the University of Toronto, said: "...Our study proves that the structure of faces also contributes to our perception of facial attractiveness." {{cite news}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help)
  24. ^ Rhodes, G., Tremewan, T. (1997) Averageness, exaggeration, and facial attractiveness. Psychol. Sci. 7, 105–110.
  25. ^ Valentine, T., Darling, S., Donnelly, M. (2004). Why are average faces attractive? The effect of view and averageness on the attractiveness of the attractiveness of female faces. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 482–487
  26. ^ average faces are consistently more attractive than the faces used to generate them
  27. ^ a b Rubenstein, A.J., Langlois, J.H., Roggman, L.A. (2002). What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty. In G. Rhodes & L.A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives: Westport, CT: Ablex.
  28. ^ a b c Grammer, K., Fink, B., Moller, A.P., Thornhill, R. (2003). "Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty." Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 78, 385–407.
  29. ^ Rhodes, G. (2006) "The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty." Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226.
  30. ^ model's facial configuration is very similar to the composite's facial configuration
  31. ^ a b Apicella, C.L., Little, A.C., Marlowe, F.W. (2007). "Facial averageness and attractiveness in an isolated population of hunter-gatherers." Perception 36, 1813–1820.
  32. ^ a b c Perrett, D.I., May, K.A., Yoshikawa, S. (1994). "Facial shape and judgements of female attractiveness." Nature (Lond.) 368, 239–242.
  33. ^ Langlois, J.H., Ritter, J.M., Roggman, L.A., Vaughn, L.S. (1991). "Facial diversity and infant preferences for attractive faces." Dev. Psychol. 27, 79–84.
  34. ^ Slater, A.M., Von Der Schulenburg, C., Brown, E., et al. (1998). "Newborn infants prefer attractive faces." Infant Behav. Dev. 21, 345–354.
  35. ^ Kramer, S., Zebrowitz, L.A., San Giovanni, J.P., Sherak, B. (1995). "Infants' preferences for attractiveness and babyfaceness." In Bardy, B.G., Bootsma, R.J., Guiard, Y. (Eds.) Studies in perception and action III. pp. 389–392. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Associates.
  36. ^ a b Strauss, M.S. (1979). "Abstraction of prototypical information by adults and 10-month-old infants." J. Exp. Psychol.: Human Learn. Mem. 5, 618–632.
  37. ^ a b Rubenstein, A.J, Kalakanis, L., Langlois, J.H. (1999). Infant preferences for attractive faces: a cognitive explanation. Dev. Psychol. 35, 848–855.
  38. ^ Rhodes, G., Hickford, C., Jeffery, L. (2000). Sex-typicality and attractiveness: Are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive? Brit. J. Psychol. 91, 125–140.
  39. ^ Hockey, P.A.R.; Dean, W.R.J.; Ryan, P.G., eds. (2003). Roberts – Birds of Southern Africa, VIIth Ed. Cape Town: The John Voelcker Bird Book Fund. pp. 14, 60–1138. ISBN 0620340517. {{cite book}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |1= (help)
  40. ^ a b c Gould, S.J. (1980). "A Quahog is a Quahog." In The Panda's Thumb. pp. 204–213. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
  41. ^ a b Maynard Smith, J. (1989). Evolutionary Genetics p. 273. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  42. ^ White, G.C.; Burnham, K.P. (1999). "Program MARK. Survival estimates from marked animals". Bird Study. 46: S120–S139. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |1= (help)
  43. ^ Reynolds, Richard T.; Joy, Suzanne M.; Leslie, Douglas G. (1994). "Nest productivity, fidelity, and spacing of Northern Goshawks in Arizona". Studies in Avian Biology. 16: 106–113. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |1= (help)
  44. ^ Bernstein, H., Byerly, H.C., Hopf, F.A., Michod, R.E. (1985). "Sex and the emergence of species." J. Theor. Biol. 117 665–690.
  45. ^ Hopf, F.A. (1990). "Darwin's dilemma of transitional forms: a comparison of model with data." In: Organisational Constraints of the Dymnamics of Evolution Eds. Maynard Smith, J., Vida, G. pp. 357–372. Manchester: Manchester University Press.
  46. ^ Gould, S.J. (1985). The Flamingo's Smile. pp. 160, 169. London: Penguin Books.
  47. ^ Maynard Smith, J. (1983). "The genetics of stasis and punctuation." Ann. rev. Genet. 17, 11–25.
  48. ^ a b c Eldredge, N., Gould, S.J. (1972). "Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism" In Schopf, T.J.M., ed., Models in Paleobiology. San Francisco: Freeman Cooper. pp. 82–115. Reprinted in Eldredge, N. (1985) Time frames. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press.
  49. ^ a b Williams, G.C. (1992). Stasis. In Natural Selection: Domains, Levels and Challenges. pp. 127–142. New York: Oxford University Press.
  50. ^ Stuart, C. & Stuart, T. (2007). Field guide to mammals of Southern Africa. pp. 174–175. Cape Town, Struik. ISBN 978-1-77007-404-0
  51. ^ Hockey, P.A.R., Dean, W.R.J., Ryan, P.G. (eds.) (2005) Roberts Birds of Southern Africa, p. 14, 60-1138. Trustees of the John Voelcker Bird Book Fund, Cape Town.
  52. ^ Laws, B (2010).Fifty Plants that Changed the Course of History. pp. 210- 215. Cinincinnati, David and Charles Book
  53. ^ a b Williams, G.C. (1992). Stasis. In Natural Selection: Domains, Levels and Challenges. p. 128. New York: Oxford University Press.
  54. ^ Mayr, E. (1954). Changes of genetic environment and evolution. In: Evolution as a process Eds. Huxley, J., Hardy, A.C., Ford, E.B. pp. 157-180. New York: Columbia University Press.
  55. ^ Maynard Smith, J. (1989). Evolutionary Genetics p. 281. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  56. ^ Gould, S.J. (1980) Return of the Hopeful Monster. in The Panda's Thumb. p. 186-193. New York: W.W. Norton.
  57. ^ Williams, G.C. (1972) Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton University Press.ISBN 0-691-02357-3
  58. ^ Maynard Smith, J. (1964). "Group selection and kin selection". Nature. 201 (4924): 1145–1147. doi:10.1038/2011145a0.
  59. ^ a b See the chapter God's utility function in Dawkins, Richard (1995). River Out of Eden. New York: Basic Books. ISBN 0-465-06990-8.
  60. ^ a b Dawkins, R. (1994). "Burying the Vehicle Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection". Behavioural and Brain Sciences. 17 (4): 616–617. Archived from the original on 2006-09-15.
  61. ^ a b Dennett, D.C. (1994). "E Pluribus Unum? Commentary on Wilson & Sober: Group Selection". Behavioural and Brain Sciences. 17 (4): 617–618. Archived from the original on 2007-12-27.
  62. ^ a b Pinker, S. (2012). The False Allure of Group Selection. Edge, Jun 19, 2012. http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection
  63. ^ Cite error: The named reference Symons was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  64. ^ a b c Campbell, N.A. (1990) Biology p. 450–451, 487–490, 499–501. Redwood City CA: Benjamin Cummings Publishing Company.
  65. ^ a b Ayala, F.J. (1982) Population and Evolutionary Genetics pp. 73–83, 182–190, 198–215. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. ISBN 0-8053-0315-4
  66. ^ Gould, S.J. & Eldredge, N. (1977) "Punctuated equilibria: the tempo and mode of evolution reconsidered." Paleobiology 3 115–151.
  67. ^ a b McCarthy, T. & Rubridge, B. (2005) The Story of Earth and Life. Cape Town: Struik Publishers. ISBN 1-77007-148-2.
  • Beauty Check includes example blended faces and discusses why average face shapes are more attractive.
  • Averaging faces shows how the average of two faces looks more attractive than either of the faces used in the averaging process.