Jump to content

Whiskers: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
Added pmid. Removed parameters. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Headbomb | Category:CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of June 2024 | #UCB_Category 38/305
Bradyb0412 (talk | contribs)
mNo edit summary
Tags: Visual edit Mobile edit Mobile web edit
Line 2: Line 2:
{{otheruses|Whisker (disambiguation)}}
{{otheruses|Whisker (disambiguation)}}
{{Technical|date=April 2021}}
{{Technical|date=April 2021}}
[[Image:Cat whiskers closeup.jpg|thumb|A cat with vibrissae]]
[[Image:Cat whiskers closeup.jpg|thumb|A [[cat]] with vibrissae]]
[[Image:Chinchilla-Patchouli.jpg|thumb|A [[chinchilla]] with large macrovibrissae]]
[[Image:Chinchilla-Patchouli.jpg|thumb|A [[chinchilla]] with large macrovibrissae]]



Revision as of 04:27, 20 July 2024

A cat with vibrissae
A chinchilla with large macrovibrissae

Whiskers or vibrissae (/vəˈbrɪsi/; sg.: vibrissa; /vəˈbrɪsə/) are a type of stiff, functional hair used by most mammals to sense their environment.[1] These hairs are finely specialised for this purpose, whereas other types of hair are coarser as tactile sensors. Although whiskers are specifically those found around the face, vibrissae are known to grow in clusters at various places around the body. Most mammals have them, including all non-human primates[2] and especially nocturnal mammals.

Whiskers are sensitive tactile hairs that aid navigation, locomotion, exploration, hunting, social touch and perform other functions.[3]

This article is primarily about the specialised sensing hairs of mammals, but some birds, fish, insects, crustaceans and other arthropods are known to have similar structures also used to sense the environment.

Etymology

Vibrissae (from Latin vibrāre 'to vibrate') from the characteristic motion seen in a small rodent that is otherwise sitting still. In medicine, the term also refers to the thick hairs found inside human nostrils.[4]

Evolution

The last common ancestor of all extant mammals had vibrissae.[5] All other extant mammal species besides great apes retain the same ancestral layout of the whiskers along with the special facial muscles that move them.[3]

Anatomy

Vibrissae are anatomically distinguished from other hair. They are easily visually identified since they are longer, stiffer, significantly larger in diameter, and stand above the surrounding fur by a considerable amount. In addition, they have well-innervated follicles, and an identifiable representation in the somatosensory cortex of the brain.[6] The largest number and the longest are found among the small, social, arboreal, and nocturnal mammals. Whiskers of aquatic mammals are the most sensitive. During foraging in complex, dark habitats, whiskers are rapidly moved in a cyclic way, tracing small circles at their tips. This motion, called "whisking" can occur at speeds of 25 Hz in mice, which is one of the fastest movements that mammals can make. Small animals use whisking to position their front paws during locomotion.[3]

Vibrissal groups

A Patagonian fox showing four major cranial groups of vibrissae: supraorbital (above the eye), mystacial (where a moustache would be), genal (on the cheek, far left), and mandibular (pointing down, under the snout)

Vibrissae typically grow in clusters. These groups vary somewhat in form and function, but they are relatively consistent among land mammals. Between land and marine mammals, there is less consistency (though commonalities are certainly present).

Many land mammals, like rats[7] and hamsters,[8] have four typical whisker groups on their heads (called cranial vibrissae), which might vary among animals due to different lifestyles. These cranial groups include:[9]

  • above the eyes (supraorbital)
  • on the cheeks (genal)
  • where a moustache would be (mystacial)
  • under the snout (mandibular).
A pet rat clearly showing the grid-like arrangement of the macrovibrissae on the face, and the microvibrissae under the nostrils. The supraorbital vibrissae above the right eye are also visible.

The mystacial whiskers can be roughly identified as macrovibrissae (long whiskers for feeling the space around the head) and microvibrissae (small, down-pointing whiskers for identifying objects).[10] Not only are these two types hard to distinguish on an animal's face (see for example the image of a rat here), there are similarly weak distinctions in how they are used, though the distinction is nonetheless referred to ubiquitously in scientific literature and is considered useful in analysis.

Many land mammals, including domestic cats, also have vibrissae on the underside of the leg just above the paws (called carpal vibrissae).[11] Whilst these five major groups are often reported in studies of land mammals, several other groups have been reported more occasionally; for instance nasal, angular, and submental whiskers.[12]

All the hairs of the manatee may be vibrissae.
Macrovibrissae and supraorbital vibrissae of the common harbor seal (Phoca vitulina)

Marine mammals can have substantially different arrangements of their vibrissae. For instance, whales and dolphins have lost their snout whiskers and gained vibrissae around their blowholes,[13] whereas every single one of the body hairs of the Florida manatee may be a vibrissa (see image).[14] Other marine mammals, like seals and sea-lions, have head vibrissae just like those on land mammals (see image), although these groups function quite differently.

Vibrissal follicles have evolved other functions in dolphins, such as electroreception.

Vibrissae

The vibrissal hair is usually thicker and stiffer than other types of (pelagic) hair[15] but, like other hairs, the shaft consists of an inert material (keratin) and contains no nerves.[15] However, vibrissae are different from other hair structures because they grow from a special hair follicle incorporating a capsule of blood called a blood sinus which is heavily innervated by sensory nerves.[16][17] Vibrissae are symmetrically arranged in groups on the face and supply the trigeminal nerve.[18]

The mystacial macrovibrissae are shared by a large group of land and marine mammals (see images), and it is this group that has received by far the most scientific study. The arrangement of these whiskers is not random: they form an ordered grid of arcs (columns) and rows, with shorter whiskers at the front and longer whiskers at the rear (see images).[10] In the mouse, gerbil, hamster, rat, guinea pig, rabbit, and cat, each individual follicle is innervated by 100–200 primary afferent nerve cells.[16] These cells serve an even larger number of mechanoreceptors of at least eight distinct types.[17] Accordingly, even small deflections of the vibrissal hair can evoke a sensory response in the animal.[19] Rats and mice typically have approximately 30 macrovibrissae on each side of the face, with whisker lengths up to around 50 mm in (laboratory) rats, 30 mm in (laboratory) mice, and a slightly larger number of microvibrissae.[10] Thus, an estimate for the total number of sensory nerve cells serving the mystacial vibrissal array on the face of a rat or mouse might be 25,000. Natural shapes of rat's mystacial pad vibrissae are well approximated by pieces of the Euler spiral. When all these pieces for a single rat are assembled together, they span an interval extending from one coiled domain of the Euler spiral to the other.[20]

Marine mammals may make even greater investment in their vibrissal sensory system than rats and mice. Seal whiskers, which are similarly arrayed across the mystacial region, are each served by around 10 times as many nerve fibres as those in rats and mice, so that the total number of nerve cells innervating the mystacial vibrissae of a seal has been estimated to be in excess of 300,000.[21] Manatees, remarkably, have around 600 vibrissae on or around their lips.[13][full citation needed]

Whiskers can be very long in some species; the length of a chinchilla's whiskers can be more than a third of its body length (see image).[22] Even in species with shorter whiskers, they can be very prominent appendages (see images). Thus, whilst whiskers certainly could be described as "proximal sensors" in contrast to, say, eyes, they offer a tactile sense with a sensing range that is functionally very significant.

Operation

Movement

A yawning cat shows how the mystacial macrovibrissae can be swept forward.

The follicles of some groups of vibrissae in some species are motile. Generally, the supraorbital, genal and macrovibrissae are motile,[8] whereas the microvibrissae are not. This is reflected in anatomical reports that have identified musculature associated with the macrovibrissae that is absent for the microvibrissae.[23] A small muscle 'sling' is attached to each macrovibrissa and can move it more-or-less independently of the others, whilst larger muscles in the surrounding tissue move many or all of the macrovibrissae together.[23][24]

Amongst those species with motile macrovibrissae, some (rats, mice, flying squirrels, gerbils, chinchillas, hamsters, shrews, porcupines, opossums) move them back and forth periodically in a movement known as whisking,[25] while other species (cats, dogs, raccoons, pandas) do not appear to.[6] The distribution of mechanoreceptor types in the whisker follicle differs between rats and cats, which may correspond to this difference in the way they are used.[17] Whisking movements are amongst the fastest produced by mammals.[26] In all whisking animals in which it has so far been measured, these whisking movements are rapidly controlled in response to behavioural and environmental conditions.[6] The whisking movements occur in bouts of variable duration, and at rates between 3 and 25 whisks/second. Movements of the whiskers are closely coordinated with those of the head and body.[6]

Function

Generally, vibrissae are considered to mediate a tactile sense, complementary to that of skin. This is presumed to be advantageous in particular to animals that cannot always rely on sight to navigate or to find food, for example, nocturnal animals or animals which forage in muddy waters. Whiskers can also function as wind detecting antannae such as the supra-orbital ones in rats.[27]

Sensory function aside, movements of the vibrissae may also indicate something of the state of mind of the animal,[28] and the whiskers play a role in social behaviour of rats.[29]

The sensory function of vibrissae is an active research area—experiments to establish the capabilities of whiskers use a variety of techniques, including temporary deprivation either of the whisker sense or of other senses. Animals can be deprived of their whisker sense for a period of weeks by whisker trimming (they soon grow back), or for the duration of an experimental trial by restraining the whiskers with a flexible cover like a mask (the latter technique is used, in particular, in studies of marine mammals[30]). Such experiments have shown that whiskers are required for, or contribute to: object localization,[31][32] orienting of the snout, detection of movement, texture discrimination, shape discrimination, exploration, thigmotaxis, locomotion, maintenance of equilibrium, maze learning, swimming, locating food pellets, locating food animals, and fighting, as well as nipple attachment and huddling in rat pups.[6]

Whisking—the periodic movement of the whiskers—is also presumed to serve tactile sensing in some way. However, exactly why an animal might be driven "to beat the night with sticks", as one researcher once put it,[33] is a matter of debate, and the answer is probably multi-faceted. Scholarpedia[6] offers:

Since rapid movement of the vibrissae consumes energy, and has required the evolution of specialised musculature, it can be assumed that whisking must convey some sensory advantages to the animal. Likely benefits are that it provides more degrees of freedom for sensor positioning, that it allows the animal to sample a larger volume of space with a given density of whiskers, and that it allows control over the velocity with which the whiskers contact surfaces.

Animals that do not whisk, but have motile whiskers, presumably also gain some advantage from the investment in musculature. Dorothy Souza, in her book Look What Whiskers Can Do[34] reports some whisker movement during prey capture (in cats, in this case):

Whiskers bend forward as the cat pounces. Teeth grasp the mouse tightly around its neck. The cat holds on until the prey stops wriggling.

Anecdotally, it is often stated that cats use their whiskers to gauge whether an opening is wide enough for their body to pass through.[citation needed] This is sometimes supported by the statement that the whiskers of individual cats extend out to about the same width as the cat's body, but at least two informal reports indicate that whisker length is genetically determined and does not vary as the cat grows thinner or fatter.[28][35] In the laboratory, rats are able to accurately (within 5–10%) discriminate the size of an opening,[36] so it seems likely that cats can use their whiskers for this purpose. However, reports of cats, particularly kittens, with their heads firmly stuck in some discarded receptacle are commonplace[37] indicating that if a cat has this information available, it does not always make best use of it.

Marine mammals

Pinnipeds have well-developed tactile senses. Their mystacial vibrissae have ten times the innervation of terrestrial mammals, allowing them to effectively detect vibrations in the water.[38] These vibrations are generated, for example, when a fish swims through water. Detecting vibrations is useful when the animals are foraging and may add to or even replace vision, particularly in darkness.[39]

The upper, smooth whisker belongs to a California sea lion. The lower undulated whisker belongs to a harbor seal.

Harbor seals have been observed following varying paths of other organisms that swam ahead several minutes before, similar to a dog following a scent trail,[30][40] and even to discriminate the species and the size of the fish responsible for the trail.[41] Blind ringed seals have even been observed successfully hunting on their own in Lake Saimaa, likely relying on their vibrissae to gain sensory information and catch prey.[42] Unlike terrestrial mammals, such as rodents, pinnipeds do not move their vibrissae over an object when examining it but instead extend their moveable whiskers and keep them in the same position.[39] By holding their vibrissae steady, pinnipeds are able to maximize their detection ability.[43] The vibrissae of seals are undulated and wavy while sea lion and walrus vibrissae are smooth.[44] Research is ongoing to determine the function, if any, of these shapes on detection ability. The vibrissa's angle relative to the flow, and not the fiber shape, however, seems to be the most important factor.[43]

Most cetaceans have whiskers at birth but they are typically lost during maturation. The follicles and any vestigial hair sometimes function as touch or electrical sense organs.[45]

Lines of research

Neuroscience

A large part of the brain of whisker-specialist mammals is involved in the processing of nerve impulses from vibrissae, a fact that presumably corresponds to the important position the sense occupies for the animal. Information from the vibrissae arrives in the brain via the trigeminal nerve and is delivered first into the trigeminal sensory complex of brainstem. From there, the most studied pathways are those leading up through parts of thalamus and into barrel cortex,[46] though other major pathways through the superior colliculus in midbrain (a major visual structure in visual animals) and the cerebellum, to name but a couple, are increasingly coming under scrutiny.[47] Neuroscientists, and other researchers, studying sensory systems favour the whisker system for a number of reasons (see Barrel cortex), not least the simple fact that laboratory rats and mice are whisker, rather than visual, specialists.

Evolutionary biology

The presence of mystacial vibrissae in distinct lineages (Rodentia, Afrotheria, marsupials) with remarkable conservation of operation suggests that they may be an old feature present in a common ancestor of all therian mammals.[48] Indeed, some humans even still develop vestigial vibrissal muscles in the upper lip,[49] consistent with the hypothesis that previous members of the human lineage had mystacial vibrissae. Thus, it is possible that the development of the whisker sensory system played an important role in mammalian development, more generally.[48]

Artificial whiskers

Researchers have begun to build artificial whiskers of a variety of types, both to help them understand how biological whiskers work and as a tactile sense for robots. These efforts range from the abstract,[50] through feature-specific models,[51][52] to attempts to reproduce complete whiskered animals in robot form (ScratchBot[53] and ShrewBot,[54][55][56] both robots by Bristol Robotics Laboratory).

In non-mammals

"Whiskers" on a whiskered auklet

A range of non-mammals possess structures which resemble or function similarly to mammalian whiskers.

In birds

The "whiskers" around the beak of a kākāpō

Some birds possess specialized hair-like feathers called rictal bristles around the base of the beak which are sometimes referred to as whiskers.

The whiskered auklet (Aethia pygmaea) has striking, stiff white feathers protruding from above and below the eyes of the otherwise slate-grey bird, and a dark plume which swoops forward from the top of its head. Whiskered auklets sent through a maze of tunnels with their feathers taped back bumped their heads more than twice as often as they did when their feathers were free, indicating they use their feathers in a similar way to cats.[57]

Other birds that have obvious "whiskers" are kiwis, flycatchers, swallows, nightjars, whip-poor-wills, the kākāpō and the long-whiskered owlet (Xenoglaux loweryi).

In fish

"Whiskers" on a catfish

Some fish have slender, pendulous tactile organs near the mouth. These are often referred to as "whiskers", although they are more correctly termed barbels. Fish that have barbels include the catfish, carp, goatfish, hagfish, sturgeon, zebrafish and some species of shark.

The Pimelodidae are a family of catfishes (order Siluriformes) commonly known as the long-whiskered catfishes.

In pterosaurs

Anurognathid pterosaurs had a rugose (wrinkled) jaw texture that has been interpreted as the attachment sites for vibrissae,[58] though actual vibrissae have not been recorded.[59] More recently, a specific type of feathers has been found around anurognathid mouths.[60]

References

  1. ^ Feldhamer, George A.; Drickamer, Lee C.; Vessey, Stephen H.; Merritt, Joseph H.; Krajewski, Carey (2007). Mammalogy: Adaptation, Diversity, Ecology (3 ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 99. ISBN 978-0-8018-8695-9. OCLC 124031907.
  2. ^ Van Horn, R.N. (1970). "Vibrissae Structure in the Rhesus Monkey". Folia Primatol. 13 (4): 241–285. doi:10.1159/000155325. PMID 5499675.
  3. ^ a b c Grant, Robyn A.; Breakell, Vicki; Prescott, Tony J. (13 June 2018). "Whisker touch sensing guides locomotion in small, quadrupedal mammals". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 285 (1880): 20180592. doi:10.1098/rspb.2018.0592. PMC 6015872. PMID 29899069.
  4. ^ "Vibrissae". The Free Dictionary's Medical dictionary. Farlex, Inc. April 14, 2009. Retrieved April 29, 2009.
  5. ^ Grant, Robyn A.; Haidarliu, Sebastian; Kennerley, Natalie J.; Prescott, Tony J. (1 January 2013). "The evolution of active vibrissal sensing in mammals: evidence from vibrissal musculature and function in the marsupial opossum Monodelphis domestica" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 216 (Pt 18): 3483–3494. doi:10.1242/jeb.087452. PMID 23737559. S2CID 207170888.
  6. ^ a b c d e f Grant, Robyn; Mitchinson, Ben; Prescott, Tony (2011). "Vibrissal behaviour and function". Scholarpedia. 6 (10): 6642. Bibcode:2011SchpJ...6.6642P. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.6642.
  7. ^ Vincent, S. B. (1913). "The tactile hair of the white rat". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 23 (1): 1–34. doi:10.1002/cne.900230101. S2CID 86132752.
  8. ^ a b Wineski, Lawrence E. (1983). "Movements of the cranial vibrissae in the Golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus)". Journal of Zoology. 200 (2): 261–280. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1983.tb05788.x.
  9. ^ Thé, L.; Wallace, M. L.; Chen, C. H.; Chorev, E.; Brecht, M. (2013). "Structure, function, and cortical representation of the rat submandibular whisker trident" (PDF). The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (11): 4815–4824. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4770-12.2013. PMC 6619006. PMID 23486952.
  10. ^ a b c Brecht, Michael; Preilowski, Bruno; Merzenich, Michael M. (1997). "Functional architecture of the mystacial vibrissae". Behavioural Brain Research. 84 (1–2): 81–97. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(97)83328-1. PMID 9079775. S2CID 3993159.
  11. ^ Beddard, Frank E. (1902). "Observations upon the carpal vibrissae in mammals". Journal of Zoology. 72 (1): 127–136. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1902.tb08213.x.
  12. ^ Kulikov, V. F. (2011). "A new vibrissa group in insectivores (Mammalia, Insectivora) and its role in orientation". Doklady Biological Sciences. 438 (1): 154–157. doi:10.1134/s0012496611030021. PMID 21728125. S2CID 27361386.
  13. ^ a b "Whiskers! A Feel For The Dark".
  14. ^ Reep, R. L.; Marshall, C. D.; Stoll, M. L. (2002). "Tactile Hairs on the Postcranial Body in Florida Manatees: A Mammalian Lateral Line?". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 59 (3): 141–154. doi:10.1159/000064161. PMID 12119533. S2CID 17392274.
  15. ^ a b Weldon Owen Pty Ltd. (1993). Encyclopedia of animals – Mammals, Birds, Reptiles, Amphibians. Reader's Digest Association. p. 18. ISBN 1-875137-49-1.
  16. ^ a b Rice, Frank L.; Mance, Ajuan; Munger, Bryce L. (8 October 1986). "A comparative light microscopic analysis of the sensory innervation of the mystacial pad. I. Innervation of vibrissal follicle-sinus complexes". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 252 (2): 154–174. doi:10.1002/cne.902520203. PMID 3782505. S2CID 8228091.
  17. ^ a b c Ebara, Satomi; Kumamoto, Kenzo; Matsuura, Tadao; Mazurkiewicz, Joseph E.; Rice, Frank L. (22 July 2002). "Similarities and differences in the innervation of mystacial vibrissal follicle–sinus complexes in the rat and cat: A confocal microscopic study". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 449 (2): 103–119. doi:10.1002/cne.10277. PMID 12115682. S2CID 6428629.
  18. ^ Halata, Z.; Baumann, K. I.; Grim, M. (2008-01-01). "6.02 – Merkel Cells". In Masland, Richard H.; Albright, Thomas D.; Albright, Thomas D.; Masland, Richard H. (eds.). The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference. New York: Academic Press. pp. 33–38. doi:10.1016/b978-012370880-9.00341-8. ISBN 978-0-12-370880-9. Retrieved 2020-12-09.
  19. ^ Stuttgen, M. C.; Rüter, J.; Schwarz, C. (July 26, 2006). "Two Psychophysical Channels of Whisker Deflection in Rats Align with Two Neuronal Classes of Primary Afferents". The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (30): 7933–7941. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1864-06.2006. PMC 6674210. PMID 16870738.
  20. ^ Starostin, E. L.; et al. (15 January 2020). "The Euler spiral of rat whiskers". Science Advances. 6 (3): eaax5145. Bibcode:2020SciA....6.5145S. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aax5145. PMC 6962041. PMID 31998835.
  21. ^ Marshall, CD; Amin, H.; Kovacs, K. M.; Lydersen, C. (January 2006). "Microstructure and innervation of the mystacial vibrissal follicle sinus complex in bearded seals, Erignathus barbatus (Pinnipedia: Phocidae)". The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary Biology. 288 (1): 13–25. doi:10.1002/ar.a.20273. PMID 16342212.
  22. ^ Spotorno, Angel E.; Zuleta, Carlos A.; Valladares, J. Pablo; Deane, Amy L.; Jiménez, Jaime E. (15 December 2004). "Chinchilla Laniger". Mammalian Species. 758: 1–9. doi:10.1644/758.
  23. ^ a b Dörfl, J (1982). "The musculature of the mystacial vibrissae of the white mouse". Journal of Anatomy. 135 (Pt 1): 147–154. PMC 1168137. PMID 7130049.
  24. ^ Hill, D. N.; Bermejo, R.; Zeigler, H. P.; Kleinfeld, D. (2008). "Biomechanics of the Vibrissa Motor Plant in Rat: Rhythmic Whisking Consists of Triphasic Neuromuscular Activity". The Journal of Neuroscience. 28 (13): 3438–3455. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5008-07.2008. PMC 6670594. PMID 18367610.
  25. ^ RoyalSociety (Oct 2, 2011). "CIA-Rat". Youtube. Archived from the original on Jul 3, 2013. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  26. ^ Jin, T.-E.; Witzemann, V.; Brecht, M. (March 31, 2004). "Fiber Types of the Intrinsic Whisker Muscle and Whisking Behavior". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (13): 3386–3393. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5151-03.2004. PMC 6730039. PMID 15056718.
  27. ^ Mugnaini, Matias; Mehrotra, Dhruv; Davoine, Federico; Sharma, Varun; Mendes, Ana Rita; Gerhardt, Ben; Concha-Miranda, Miguel; Brecht, Michael; Clemens, Ann M. (2023). "Supra-orbital whiskers act as wind-sensing antennae in rats". PLOS Biology. 21 (7): e3002168. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3002168. ISSN 1545-7885. PMC 10325054. PMID 37410722.
  28. ^ a b McSporran, Keith. "Just the cat's whiskers". Archived from the original on 2012-02-12.
  29. ^ Wolfe, Jason; Mende, Carolin; Brecht, Michael (2011). "Social facial touch in rats". Behavioral Neuroscience. 125 (6): 900–910. doi:10.1037/a0026165. ISSN 1939-0084. PMID 22122151.
  30. ^ a b Dehnhardt, G. (2001). "Hydrodynamic trail-following in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina)". Science. 293 (5527): 102–104. doi:10.1126/science.1060514. PMID 11441183. S2CID 9156299.
  31. ^ Ahissar, E.; Knutsen, P. M. (2011). "Vibrissal location decoding". Scholarpedia. 6 (10): 6639. Bibcode:2011SchpJ...6.6639A. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.6639.
  32. ^ Diamond, M.; von Heimendahl, P; Knutsen, P.; Kleinfeld, D.; Ahissar, A. (2008). "'Where' and 'what' in the whisker sensorimotor system". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 9 (8): 601–612. doi:10.1038/nrn2411. PMID 18641667. S2CID 6450408.
  33. ^ Brecht, Michael (September 2004). "What Makes Whiskers Shake?". Journal of Neurophysiology. 92 (3): 1265–1266. doi:10.1152/jn.00404.2004. PMID 15331639.
  34. ^ Souza, Dorothy (2007). Look What Whiskers Can Do. Lerner Publishing Group. p. 17. ISBN 978-0-761-39459-4.
  35. ^ "Focus Magazine Q&A". Archived from the original on 2018-07-18. Retrieved 2012-04-13.
  36. ^ Krupa, David J.; Matell, Matthew S.; Brisben, Amy J.; Oliveira, Laura M.; Nicolelis, Miguel A. L. (2001). "Behavioral Properties of the trigeminal somatosensory system in rats performing whisker-dependent tactile discriminations". The Journal of Neuroscience. 21 (15): 5752–5763. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-05752.2001. PMC 6762640. PMID 11466447.
  37. ^ For example: "Cops save kitten with head stuck in can". Toronto Sun. 2011-01-25. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  38. ^ R. J., Schusterman; D., Kastak; D. H., Levenson; C. J., Reichmuth; B. L., Southall (2000). "Why pinnipeds don't echolocate". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 107 (4): 2256–64. Bibcode:2000ASAJ..107.2256S. doi:10.1121/1.428506. PMID 10790051. S2CID 17153968.
  39. ^ a b Miersch, L.; Hanke, W.; Wieskotten, S.; Hanke, F. D.; Oeffner, J.; Leder, A.; Brede, M.; Witte, M.; Dehnhardt, G. (2011). "Flow sensing by pinniped whiskers". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 366 (1581): 3077–84. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0155. PMC 3172597. PMID 21969689.
  40. ^ Schulte-Pelkum, N.; Wieskotten, S.; Hanke, W.; Dehnhardt, G. & Mauck, B. (2007). "Tracking of biogenic hydrodynamic trails in harbour seals (Phoca vitulina)". Journal of Experimental Biology. 210 (5): 781–787. doi:10.1242/jeb.02708. PMID 17297138.
  41. ^ Grant R, Wieskotten S, Wengst N, Prescott T, Dehnhardt G (2013). "Vibrissal touch sensing in the harbor seal (Phoca vitulina): how do seals judge size?". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 199 (6): 521–531. doi:10.1007/s00359-013-0797-7. PMID 23397461. S2CID 14018274.
  42. ^ Hyvärinen H (1989). "Diving in darkness: whiskers as sense organs of the ringed seal (Phoca hispida saimensis)". Journal of Zoology. 218 (4): 663–678. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1989.tb05008.x.
  43. ^ a b Murphy, T. C.; Eberhardt, W. C.; Calhoun, B. H.; Mann, K. A.; Mann, D. A. (2013). "Effect of Angle on Flow-Induced Vibrations of Pinniped Vibrissae". PLOS ONE. 8 (7): e69872. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...869872M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069872. PMC 3724740. PMID 23922834.
  44. ^ Ginter CC, Fish FE (2010). "Morphological analysis of the bumpy profile of phocid vibrissae". Marine Mammal Science. 26: 733–743. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2009.00365.x.
  45. ^ Mynett, Natasha; Mossman, Hannah L.; Huettner, Tim; Grant, Robyn A. (2022). "Diversity of vibrissal follicle anatomy in cetaceans" (PDF). The Anatomical Record. 305 (3): 609–621. doi:10.1002/ar.24714. PMID 34288543. S2CID 236158643.
  46. ^ Deschenes, Martin; Urbain, Nadia (2009). "Vibrissal afferents from trigeminus to cortices". Scholarpedia. 4 (5): 7454. Bibcode:2009SchpJ...4.7454D. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.7454.
  47. ^ Kleinfeld, Rune w. Berg (1999). "Anatomical loops and their electrical dynamics in relation to whisking by rat". Somatosensory & Motor Research. 16 (2): 69–88. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.469.3914. doi:10.1080/08990229970528. PMID 10449057.
  48. ^ a b Mitchinson, B.; Grant, R. A.; Arkley, K.; Rankov, V.; Perkon, I.; Prescott, T. J. (12 November 2011). "Active vibrissal sensing in rodents and marsupials". Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 366 (1581): 3037–3048. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0156. PMC 3172598. PMID 21969685.
  49. ^ Tamatsu, Yuichi; Tsukahara, Kazue; Hotta, Mitsuyuki; Shimada, Kazuyuki (August 2007). "Vestiges of vibrissal capsular muscles exist in the human upper lip". Clin Anat. 20 (6): 628–31. doi:10.1002/ca.20497. PMID 17458869. S2CID 21055062.
  50. ^ "Invention: Artificial whiskers".
  51. ^ Costandi, Mo (2006-10-05). "Sculpted Face". Neurophilosophy.wordpress.com. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  52. ^ Fend, Miriam; Bovet, Simon; Hafner, Verena Vanessa (2004). The Artificial Mouse - A Robot with Whiskers and Vision. 35th International Symposium on Robotics. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.58.6535.
  53. ^ Archived at Ghostarchive and the Wayback Machine: "Bristol Robotics Lab - Scratchbot". YouTube. 2009-07-01. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  54. ^ Archived at Ghostarchive and the Wayback Machine: "SCRATCHbot - A Rat like Robot". YouTube. 2011-09-15. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  55. ^ Archived at Ghostarchive and the Wayback Machine: "Whiskerbot". YouTube. 2011-09-03. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  56. ^ Archived at Ghostarchive and the Wayback Machine: "A robot inspired by the Etruscan shrew called Shrewbot". YouTube. 2012-01-19. Retrieved 2013-06-24.
  57. ^ Brown, S. (2008). "Bird uses 'whiskers' like a cat". Nature. doi:10.1038/news.2008.674. Retrieved September 28, 2013.
  58. ^ Bennett et al 2007b
  59. ^ Wilton, Mark P. (2013). Pterosaurs: Natural History, Evolution, Anatomy. Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0691150611.
  60. ^ Benton, Michael J.; Xu, Xing; Orr, Patrick J.; Kaye, Thomas G.; Pittman, Michael; Kearns, Stuart L.; McNamara, Maria E.; Jiang, Baoyu; Yang, Zixiao (2019). "Pterosaur integumentary structures with complex feather-like branching" (PDF). Nature Ecology & Evolution. 3 (1): 24–30. doi:10.1038/s41559-018-0728-7. hdl:1983/1f7893a1-924d-4cb3-a4bf-c4b1592356e9. PMID 30568282. S2CID 56480710.