Jump to content

Prosopagnosia

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 71.174.251.76 (talk) at 18:47, 17 June 2010 (Famous sufferers). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Prosopagnosia
SpecialtyNeurology Edit this on Wikidata
Frequency2.4—2.6%

Prosopagnosia (sometimes known as face blindness) is a disorder of face perception where the ability to recognize faces is impaired, while the ability to recognize other objects may be relatively intact. The term originally referred to a condition following acute brain damage, but recently a congenital form of the disorder has been proposed, which may be inherited by about 2.5% of the population.[1] The specific brain area usually associated with prosopagnosia is the fusiform gyrus.[2]

Few successful therapies have so far been developed for affected people, although individuals often learn to use 'piecemeal' or 'feature by feature' recognition strategies. This may involve secondary clues such as clothing, hair color, body shape, and voice. Because the face seems to function as an important identifying feature in memory, it can also be difficult for people with this condition to keep track of information about people, and socialize normally with others.

Some also use the term prosophenosia, which refers to the inability to recognize faces following extensive damage of both occipital and temporal lobes.[3][4]

Overview

Selective inabilities to recognize faces were reported throughout the 19th century, and included case studies by Hughlings Jackson and Charcot. However, it was not named until the term prosopagnosia was first used in 1947 by Joachim Bodamer, a German neurologist. He described three cases, including a 24-year old man who suffered a bullet wound to the head and lost his ability to recognise his friends, family, and even his own face. However, he was able to recognize and identify them through other sensory modalities such as auditory, tactile, and even other visual stimuli patterns (such as gait and other physical mannerisms). Bodamer gave his paper the title Die Prosop-Agnosie, derived from classical Greek πρόσωπον (prosopon) meaning "face" and αγνωσία (agnosia) meaning "non-knowledge".

The study of prosopagnosia has been crucial in the development of theories of face perception. Because prosopagnosia is not a unitary disorder (i.e., different people may show different types and levels of impairment) it has been argued that face perception involves a number of stages, each of which can be separately damaged.[5] This is reflected not just in the amount of impairment displayed but also in the qualitative differences in impairment that a person with prosopagnosia may present with.

This sort of evidence has been crucial in supporting the theory that there may be a specific face perception system in the brain. This is counter-intuitive to many people as they do not experience faces as 'special' or perceived in a different way from the rest of the world.

A recent case report described a closely related condition called prosopamnesia, in which the subject, from birth, could perceive faces normally but had a severely impaired ability to remember them.[6]

It has also been argued that prosopagnosia may be a general impairment in understanding how individual perceptual components make up the structure or gestalt of an object. Psychologist Martha Farah has been particularly associated with this view.

Until early in the 21st century, prosopagnosia was thought to be quite rare and solely associated with brain injury or neurological illness affecting specific areas of the brain. However, recently a form of congenital prosopagnosia has been proposed, in which people are born with an impairment in recognising and perceiving faces, as well as other objects and visual scenes. The cases that have been reported suggest that this form of the disorder may be heritable and much more common than previously thought (about 2.5% of the population may be affected), although this congenital disorder is commonly accompanied by other forms of visual agnosia, and may not be "pure" prosopagnosia.[1] It has been suggested that very mild cases of face blindness are much more common, perhaps affecting 10% of the population, although there have not been any studies confirming this.[2] The inability to keep track of the identity of characters in movies is a common complaint.[7]

A classic case of a prosopagnosia is presented by "Dr P" in Oliver Sacks' 1985 book The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat. Although Dr P could not recognize his wife from her face, he was able to recognize her by her voice. His recognition of pictures of his family and friends appeared to be based on highly specific features, such as his brother's square jaw and big teeth.

Subtypes

Apperceptive prosopagnosia

Apperceptive prosopagnosia is thought to be a disorder of some of the earliest processes in the face perception system. People with this disorder cannot make any sense of faces and are unable to make same-different judgements when they are presented with pictures of different faces. They may also be unable to work out attributes such as age or gender from a face. However, they may be able to recognise people based on non-face clues such as their clothing, hairstyle or voice.

Associative prosopagnosia

Associative prosopagnosia is thought to be an impairment to the links between early face perception processes and the semantic information we hold about people in our memories. People with this form of the disorder may be able to say whether photos of people's faces are the same or different and derive the age and gender from a face (suggesting they can make sense of some face information) but may not be able to subsequently identify the person or provide any information about them such as their name, occupation or when they were last encountered. They may be able to recognise and produce such information based on non-face information such as voice, hair, or even particularly distinctive facial features (such as a distinctive moustache) that does not require the structure of the face to be understood. Typically such people do not report that 'faces make no sense' but simply that they do not look distinctive in any way.

Developmental prosopagnosia

Developmental prosopagnosia (DP) is a face recognition deficit that is lifelong, manifests itself in early childhood and that cannot be attributed to acquired brain damage. However, a number of studies have found functional deficits in DP both on the basis of EEG measures and fMRI. It has been suggested that a genetic factor is responsible for the condition.

There seem to be two categories of DP patients:
- patients who are impaired in basic face processing (age estimation, judgment of facial affect) and also show deficits on other forms of visual processing;
- patients with pure face recognition impairments in the presence of intact basic visual processing.
The first group of patients fail to obtain view-centered descriptions. According to the Bruce and Young model of face recognition, these are precursors of the more abstract expression-independent descriptions. View-centered descriptions do not seem to be specific for faces, as the patients with impairments of processing the physical aspects of faces also show difficulties in non-facial tasks like object recognition or tests of visuo-spatial abilities.
However, there is as yet only limited evidence for a classification into different subtypes.[8]

There are many developmental disorders that incorporate within themselves an increased likelihood that the person will have differences in face perception, of which the person may or may not be aware. That is to say, the person may or may not have insight in the clinical sense of the word. However, the mechanism by which these effects take place is largely unknown. A partial list of some disorders that often have prosopagnosiac components would include nonverbal learning disorder, Williams syndrome, and autism spectrum disorders in general. However, these types of disorders are very complicated, so arbitrary assumptions should be avoided.[citation needed]

Unconscious face recognition

One particularly interesting feature of prosopagnosia is that it suggests both a conscious and unconscious aspect to face recognition. Experiments have shown that when presented with a mixture of familiar and unfamiliar faces, people with prosopagnosia may be unable to successfully identify the people in the pictures, or even make a simple familiarity judgement ("this person seems familiar / unfamiliar"). However, when a measure of emotional response is taken (typically a measure of skin conductance), there tends to be an emotional response to familiar people even though no conscious recognition takes place[9]

This suggests emotion plays a significant role in face recognition, perhaps unsurprising when basic survival (particularly security) relies on identifying the people around you.

It is thought that Capgras delusion may be the reverse of prosopagnosia. In this condition people report conscious recognition of people from faces, but show no emotional response, perhaps leading to the delusional belief that their relative or spouse has been replaced by an impostor.

Famous sufferers

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Grüter T, Grüter M, Carbon CC (2008). "Neural and genetic foundations of face recognition and prosopagnosia". J Neuropsychol. 2 (1): 79–97. doi:10.1348/174866407X231001. PMID 19334306.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ a b Face blindness not just skin deep - CNN.com
  3. ^ Paulev, Poul-Erik (1999–2000). "Chapter 4. Brain Function, Locomotion And Disorders". Textbook in Medical Physiology And Pathophysiology Essentials and clinical problems. Copenhagen Medical Publishers. ISBN 87-984078-0-5. {{cite book}}: Check date values in: |year= (help); Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help); External link in |title= (help)CS1 maint: year (link)
  4. ^ Weis. "Nervous System Pathways". Biol 2401 A & P Lecture Notes. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameters: |month= and |coauthors= (help)
  5. ^ Dissociable deficits after brain injury by Newcombe, F., de Haan, E.H.F., Small, M. & Hay, D.C. in Young, Andrew W. (1998). Face and Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-852420-X. {{cite book}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  6. ^ Williams MA, Berberovic N, Mattingley JB (2007). "Abnormal FMRI adaptation to unfamiliar faces in a case of developmental prosopamnesia". Curr. Biol. 17 (14): 1259–64. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.042. PMID 17614283. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ Prosopagnosia Research Centers at Harvard University and University College London
  8. ^ T Kress & I. Daum: Developmental prosopagnosia: a review. Behavioural Neurology 14 (2003) 109–121 (reference for the whole section starting with: "Developmental prosopagnosia (DP) is a face recognition deficit...")
  9. ^ Bauer RM (1984). "Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: a neuropsychological application of the Guilty Knowledge Test". Neuropsychologia. 22 (4): 457–69. PMID 6483172.
  10. ^ "Jane Goodall Interview - page 5 / 9 - Academy of Achievement". Achievement.org. 2009-08-20. Retrieved 2010-02-20.
  11. ^ a b Grüter T, Grüter M (2007). "Prosopagnosia in biographies and autobiographies". Perception. 36 (2): 299–301. doi:10.1068/p5716. PMID 17402670. Cite error: The named reference "Grutzer" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  12. ^ a b Goodall J, Berman P (2000). Reason for Hope. London: Thorsons. pp. XIII–XIV. ISBN 0722540426. I suffer from an embarrassing, curiously humbling neurological condition called prosopagnosia, which, translated, means I have a problem in face recognition. I used to think, that it was due to some mental laziness, and I desperately tried to memorize the faces of people I met ... Quite by chance, when talking to a friend, I found out that he suffered from the same problem. I could not believe it. Then I discovered that my own sister, Judy, knew similar embarrassment. Perhaps, others did also. I wrote to the well-known neurologist Dr. Oliver Sacks. Had he ever heard of such an unusual condition? Not only had he heard of it—he suffered from it himself. And his situation was far more extreme than mine.
  13. ^ kos, "Midday open thread," January 28, 2010 at 12:05:56 PM PST, found at Daily Kos website ("Given the chance, I'd be as reclusive as J.D. Salinger. For me, being social has been a life-long struggle, one not helped by a condition called prosopagnosia, or face blindness. So if you've met me and I don't seem to recognize you, it's because I don't. It's a shitty condition to have when your job suddenly requires you to spend significant time in social situations."), citing the Wikipedia article here at Prosopagnosia. Retrieved January 28, 2010.
  14. ^ "Everyone looks the same to me". mirror.co.uk. 2007-11-09. Retrieved 2010-02-20.

Further reading

  • Bruce, V. and Young, A. (2000) In the Eye of the Beholder: The Science of Face Perception. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-852439-0
  • Cante, Richard C. (2008). Gay Men and the Forms of Contemporary US Culture. London: Ashgate Publishing. ISBN 0 7546 7230 1. Chapter 7: The Gay Visage (and Related Forms of Thought). {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  • Farah, M.J. (1990) Visual agnosia: Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision. MIT: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-06238-0
  • "About face". Economist. 2004. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  • "Your mother's smile". Economist. 2006. Evidence mounts that making, and perhaps recognising, expressions is inherited. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  • Abedin, Shahreen (2007). "Face blindness not just skin deep". CNN.com. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)