Jump to content

Yawn

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 199.31.3.198 (talk) at 22:20, 10 October 2010. The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Joseph Ducreux pandiculating; self-portrait ca. 1783

A yawn (from the Middle English yanen, an alteration of yonen or yenen, which in turn comes from the Old English geonian[1]), is a reflex of simultaneous inhalation of air and stretching of the eardrums, followed by exhalation of breath. Pandiculation is the act of stretching and yawning simultaneously.[2]

Yawning is associated with tiredness, stress, overwork, lack of stimulation, and boredom. Yawning can also be a powerful non-verbal message with several possible meanings, depending on the circumstances. In humans, yawning has an infectious quality (i.e., seeing a person yawning, talking to someone on the phone who is yawning, or just thinking of yawning can trigger yawning.) which is a typical example of positive feedback.[3] Infectious yawning has also been noted in chimpanzees.

Proposed causes

There are a number of theories that attempt to explain why animals yawn.[4][5] It is likely that there are a number of triggers—not just one—for the behavior. However, there are a few select theories that attempt to explain the primary evolutionary reason for the yawn. None of them has been empirically substantiated. Greg it is because you need more oxygen in your sytem because I said so. The first states that yawning occurs when one's blood contains increased amounts of carbon dioxide and, therefore, becomes in need of the influx of oxygen (or expulsion of carbon dioxide) that a yawn can provide.[4] Researchers believed this theory to be true at least since Hippocrates, but studies have since shown it to be either incorrect or, at the very best, flawed.[6] Yawning may, in fact, reduce oxygen intake compared to normal respiration, not increase it.[7]

Another speculated reason for yawning is the desire to stretch one's muscles.[8] Yawns are often accompanied by the urge to stretch. Animals often have to be ready to spring into action at any given moment. The muscles required to do this function better when stretched, which is why human athletes stretch before exercise. Considering the increased amount of oxygen the body needs to sustain physical activity, it may be that, to perform best, the lungs and throat muscles need to be "limbered up" as well. Therefore, yawning could be the body's way of helping to keep the body ready for action. There have been studies that suggest that yawning, especially psychological "contagious" yawning, may have developed as a way of keeping a group of animals alert.[9] If an animal is drowsy or bored, it may not be as alert as it should be to be prepared to spring into action. Therefore, the "contagious" yawn could be an instinctual reaction to a signal from one member of the "herd" reminding the others to stay alert. Nervousness has also been suggested as a possible reason. Nervousness often indicates the perception of an impending need for action. Anecdotal evidence suggests that yawning helps increase the state of alertness of a person--paratroopers have been noted to yawn in the moments before they exit the aircraft.[10]

Research data strongly suggest that neither contagious nor story-induced yawning are reliable in children below the age of six years.[11]

Another notion states that yawning is the body's way of controlling brain temperature.[12] The process is thought to cool off the brain, much like a fan cools the inside of a computer.[13]In 2007, researchers (a professor of psychology) from the University of Albany proposed that yawning may be a means to keep the brain cool. Mammalian brains operate best within a narrow temperature range. In two experiments, they demonstrated that both subjects with cold packs attached to their foreheads and subjects asked to breathe strictly nasally exhibited reduced contagious yawning when watching videos of people yawning.[14][15] A similar recent hypothesis is that yawning is used for regulation of body temperature. Similarly, a study by Jared Guttmann and Celena Dopart at Worcester Polytechnic Institute found that when a subject wearing earplugs yawned, a breeze is heard caused by the flux of the air moving between the subject's ear and the environment. Researchers Guttmann and Dopart determined that a yawn causes one of three possible situations to occur: the brain cools down due to an influx or outflux of oxygen, the pressure in the brain is reduced by an outflux of oxygen, or the pressure of the brain is increased by an influx of air caused by increased cranial space.

Another hypothesis is that yawns are caused by the same chemicals (neurotransmitters) in the brain that affect emotions, mood, appetite, and other phenomena. These chemicals include serotonin, dopamine, glutamic acid, and nitric oxide. As more (or less) of these compounds are activated in the brain, the frequency of yawning increases. Conversely, a greater presence in the brain of opioid neurotransmitters such as endorphins reduces the frequency of yawning. Individuals in opioid withdrawal exhibit a greatly increased frequency of yawning. Patients taking the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors Paxil (paroxetine HCl) or Celexa (citalopram) have been observed yawning more often. Excessive yawning is more common during the first three months of taking the SSRI's. Anecdotal reports by users of psilocybin mushrooms often describe a marked stimulation of yawning while intoxicated, often associated with excess lacrimation and nasal mucosal stimulation, especially while "peaking" (i.e., undergoing the most intense portion of the psilocybin experience). While opioids have been demonstrated to reduce this yawning and lacrimation provoked by psilocybin, it is not clear that the same pathways that induce yawning as a symptom of opioid abstinence in habituated users are the mode of action in yawning in mushroom users. While even opioid-dependent users of psilocybin on stable opioid therapy often report yawning and excess lacrimation while undergoing this entheogenic mushroom experience, there are no reports in the literature of habituated users experiencing other typical opioid withdrawal symptoms such as cramping, physical pain, anxiety, gooseflesh, etc. on mushrooms.

Recent research carried out by Garrett Norris, a lecturer in psychology at the University of Leeds, involving monitoring the behavior of students kept waiting in a reception area, indicates a connection (supported by neuro-imaging research) between empathic ability and yawning. "We believe that contagious yawning indicates empathy. It indicates an appreciation of other people's behavioral and physiological state," said Garrett.[16]

Yawning behavior may be altered as a result of medical issues such as diabetes,[17] stroke,[18] or adrenal conditions.[19]

It is also possible that yawning is a less potent territorial reflex. Usually being associated with boredom or lack of interest, yawning is often displayed when the subject is faced with a worrying or dangerous situation. Therefore, to yawn in the presence of a rival for territory would portray the subject as unthreatened by the rival, in order to deter it from entering the subject's territory. This behaviour is seen in many primates, as well as some feline species.

To look at the issue in terms of a possible evolutionary advantage, yawning might be a herd instinct.[20] For example, theories suggest that the yawn serves to synchronize mood in gregarious animals, similar to the howling of the wolf pack. It signals tiredness to other members of the group in order to synchronize sleeping patterns and periods.

Contagiousness

Two ironing women by Edgar Degas

The yawn reflex has long been observed to be contagious: in 1508, Erasmus wrote: "One man's yawning makes another yawn",[21] and the French proverbialized the idea to "Un bon bâilleur en fait bâillier deux." ("One good gaper makes two others gape").[22] Often, if one person yawns, this may cause another person to "sympathetically" yawn.[7][23] Observing another person's yawning face (especially his/her eyes), even reading, or thinking about yawning, or looking at a yawning picture can cause a person to yawn.[24][25][26] The proximate cause for contagious yawning may lie with mirror neurons, i.e., neurons in the frontal cortex of certain vertebrates, which upon being exposed to a stimulus from conspecific (same species) and occasionally interspecific organisms, activates the same regions in the brain.[27] Mirror neurons have been proposed as a driving force for imitation which lies at the root of much human learning, e.g., language acquisition. Yawning may be an offshoot of the same imitative impulse.

A 2007 study found that young children with autism spectrum disorders do not increase their yawning frequency after seeing videos of other people yawning, in contrast to typically developing children. In fact, the autistic children actually yawned less during the videos of yawning than during the control videos. This supports the claim that contagious yawning is related to empathic capacity.[28]

This phenomenon has been observed among various primates. Here the yawn is a threat gesture, a way of maintaining order in the primates' social structure. Specific studies were conducted on chimpanzees[29] and stumptail macaques.[30] A group of these animals was shown a video of other conspecifics yawning; both species yawned as well. This helps to partly confirm a yawn's "contagiousness."

As well, the Discovery Channel's show "Mythbusters" also tested this concept. In their small scale informal study they concluded that yawning is contagious.[31]

Gordon Gallup, who hypothesizes that yawning may be a means of keeping the brain cool, also hypothesizes that "contagious" yawning may be a survival instinct inherited from our evolutionary past. "During human evolutionary history when we were subject to predation and attacks by other groups, if everybody yawns in response to seeing someone yawn, the whole group becomes much more vigilant, and much better at being able to detect danger."[14]

A recent study by the University of London has suggested that the "contagiousness" of yawns by a human will pass to dogs. The study observed that 21 of 29 dogs yawned when a stranger yawned in front of them, but did not yawn when the stranger only opened his mouth.[32]

Non-human yawning

In animals, yawning can serve as a warning signal. For example, Charles Darwin, in his book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, mentioned that baboons yawn to threaten their enemies, possibly by displaying large, canine teeth.[33] Similarly, Siamese fighting fish yawn only when they see a conspecific (same species) or their own mirror-image, and their yawn often accompanies aggressive attack.[34] Guinea pigs also yawn in a display of dominance or anger, displaying their impressive incisor teeth. This is often accompanied by teeth chattering, purring and scent marking. Adelie Penguins employ yawning as part of their courtship ritual. Penguin couples face off and the males engage in what is described as an "ecstatic display," their beaks open wide and their faces pointed skyward. This trait has also been seen among Emperor Penguins. Researchers have been attempting to discover why these two different species share this trait, despite not sharing a habitat. Snakes yawn, both to realign their jaws after a meal, and for respiratory reasons, as their trachea can be seen to expand when they do this. Dogs often yawn after seeing people yawn, and when they are confused.[35] Fish can also yawn, and they increase this behavior due to lack of oxygen or excessive heat.[36]

Culture

A soldier hides his yawn from his lady companion in this detail from a painting by Oscar Bluhm titled Ermüdende Konversation, or "Tedious conversation".

Certain cultures ascribe superstitions to the act of yawning. The most common of these is the belief that it is necessary to cover one's mouth when one is yawning in order to prevent one's soul from escaping the body. The Ancient Greeks believed that yawning was not a sign of boredom, but that a person's soul was trying to escape from its body, so as to rest with the gods in the skies. This belief was also shared by the Mayans.[37]

Several superstitions have been concocted regarding the act of yawning and the harm that the act can do to the individual yawning. These superstitions may not only have arisen to prevent people from committing the faux pas of yawning loudly in another's presence—one of Mason Cooley's aphorisms is "A yawn is more disconcerting than a contradiction", and in 1663 Francis Hawkins advised "In yawning howl not, and thou shouldst abstain as much as thou can to yawn, especially when thou speakest"[38]—but may also have arisen from concerns over public health. Polydore Vergil (c. 1470–1555), in his De Rerum Inventoribus, writes that it was customary to make the Sign of the Cross over one's mouth, since "alike deadly plague was sometime in yawning, wherefore men used to fence themselves with the sign of the cross...which custom we retain at this day."[39] The Islamic prophet Muhammad, is reported by Bukhari to have mentioned, "Yawning is from Satan and if anyone of you yawns, he should check his yawning as much as possible, for if anyone of you (during the act of yawning) should say: 'Ha', Satan will laugh at him."[40]

Yawning is often considered rude due to the implication of boredom, and may even lead to penalties for contempt of court.[41]

References

  1. ^ "Online Etymology Dictionary". Retrieved 2007-07-24.
  2. ^ MedOnline.net, "pandiculate"
  3. ^ Camazine, Deneubourg, Franks, Sneyd, Theraulaz, Bonabeau, Self-Organization in Biological Systems, Princeton University Press, 2003. ISBN 0-691-11624-5, ISBN 0-691-01211-3 (pbk.) p. 18.
  4. ^ a b MSN.com, "Little Mystery: Why Do We Yawn?"
  5. ^ Chudler, Eric H (July 31, 2007). "Yawning...and Why Yawns are Contagious". University of Washington. Retrieved 2009-10-03.
  6. ^ Thinkquest.org, Brain: Organ of the Mind"
  7. ^ a b Provine RR; Tate, BC; Geldmacher, LL (2005). "Yawning". American Scientist. 93 (6): 532. doi:10.1511/2005.6.532. PMID 3120687. Archived from the original on June 4, 2008. Retrieved 2009-05-28.
  8. ^ Thinkquest.org, - "Brain: Organ of the Mind"
  9. ^ Reallyworks.org - "What Causes Yawning: The Real Reason Why People Yawn"
  10. ^ Hooper, Rowan (2 July 2007). "Yawning may boost brain's alertness". New Scientist. Retrieved 2009-09-01.
  11. ^ James R. Anderson and Pauline Meno (2007). "Psychological Influences on Yawning in Children". Current psychology letters [Online]. 2 (11). {{cite journal}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help)
  12. ^ Science Daily: "Psychologists Attribute Yawning To The Need To Cool The Brain And Pay Attention"
  13. ^ "Discovery News". Retrieved 2008-12-15.
  14. ^ a b Gordon G. Gallup (2007). Good Morning America - The Science of Yawning (July 30, 2007) (TV-Series). USA: ABC. {{cite AV media}}: External link in |title= (help)
  15. ^ Gallup AC & Gallup GG Jr (2007). "Yawning as a brain cooling mechanism: Nasal breathing and forehead cooling diminish the incidence of contagious yawning" (PDF). Evolutionary Psychology. 5 (1). Retrieved 2009-05-28.
  16. ^ Sign of empathy
  17. ^ Zheng H, Bidasee KR, Mayhan WG, Patel KP (2006). "Lack of central nitric oxide triggers erectile dysfunction in diabetes". Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 292 (3): R1158–R1164. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00429.2006. PMID 17095652. Retrieved 2009-05-28.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  18. ^ Telegraph.co.uk
  19. ^ Anías-Calderóna J,Verdugo-Díaz L, Drucker-Colín R (2004). "Adrenalectomy and dexamethasone replacement on yawning behavior". Behavioural Brain Research. 154 (1): 255–259. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.02.013. PMID 15302132. Retrieved 2009-05-28.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ Schürmann; Hesse, MD; Stephan, KE; Saarela, M; Zilles, K; Hari, R; Fink, GR; et al. (2005). "Yearning to yawn: the neural basis of contagious yawning". NeuroImage. 24 (4): 1260–1264. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.022. PMID 15670705. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help) (see also Platek; Mohamed, FB; Gallup Jr, GG; et al. (2005). "Contagious Yawning and The Brain". Cognitive Brain Research. 23 (2–3): 448–52. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.11.011. PMID 15820652. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help))
  21. ^ Erasmus Adagio Chil. III, cent. iv, No 95 (1508) quoted in Stevenson, Burton ed. The Macmillan Book of Proverbs, Maxims, and Famous Phrases. New York: Macmillan, 1948.
  22. ^ Stevenson, Burton ed. The Macmillan Book of Proverbs, Maxims, and Famous Phrases. New York: Macmillan, 1948.non selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors may cause yawning.
  23. ^ The website by Émilie attempts to prove this.
  24. ^ Provine RR; Tate, BC; Geldmacher, LL (2005). "Yawning". American Scientist. 93 (6): 532. doi:10.1511/2005.6.532. PMID 3120687. Archived from the original on June 4, 2008. Retrieved 2009-05-28.
  25. ^ Provine RR (1986). "Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releasing stimulus". Ethology. 72 (2): 109–122. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00611.x.
  26. ^ Krulwich, Robert (September 24, 2007). "The Quest to Design the Perfect Yawn: NPR". NPR. Retrieved 2009-09-01.
  27. ^ V.S. Ramachandran, "Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind "the great leap forward" in human evolution". Retrieved 2006-11-16.
  28. ^ Senju A, Maeda M, Kikuchi Y, Hasegawa T, Tojo Y, Osanai H (2). "Absence of contagious yawning in children with autism spectrum disorder". Biol Lett. 3 (6): 706–8. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2007.0337. PMC 2391210. PMID 17698452. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |year= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: year (link)
  29. ^ Anderson JR, Myowa-Yamakoshi M & Matsuzawa T (2004). "Contagious yawning in chimpanzees". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 271: S468–S470. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0224. PMC 1810104. PMID 15801606.
  30. ^ Paukner A & Anderson JR (2006). "Video-induced yawning in stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides)". Biology Letters. 2 (1): 36–38. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0411. PMC 1617183. PMID 17148320.
  31. ^ http://dsc.discovery.com/fansites/mythbusters/episode/episode-tab-06.html
  32. ^ Carpenter, Jennifer (Aug. 5, 2008). "Pet dogs can 'catch' human yawns". BBC NEWS. Retrieved 2009-10-03. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  33. ^ Chadwick-Jones, John K. (1998). Developing a social psychology of monkeys and apes. Taylor and Francis. p. 48. ISBN 0863778208.
  34. ^ Baenninger R (1987). "Some comparative aspects of yawning in Betta sleepnes, Homo Sapiens, Pantera leo and Papio sphinx". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 101 (4): 349–354. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.101.4.349.
  35. ^ Fisher, Gail T. (Sep. 14, 2008). "Some reasons for dog yawning". All Dogs Gym & Inn. Retrieved 2009-10-03. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  36. ^ "Fish do yawn! An occasional yawn is normal! If all the fish in an aquarium are yawning every few minutes or so, call us. This may indicate a dissolved oxygen or temperature problem." "Fish facts" on AquariumPros.com
  37. ^ Buzzle.com, "What causes a yawn"
  38. ^ Hawkins, Francis Youth's Behavior, or, Decency in Conversation amongst Men (1663) quoted in Mencken, H.L.. A New Dictionary of Quotations on Historical Principles from Ancient and Modern Sources New York: Vintage, 1942
  39. ^ Iona Opie and Moira Tatem, A Dictionary of Superstitions (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1992), 454.
  40. ^ Sahih Al-Bukhari Volume 4, Book 54, Number 509, Narrated by Abu Huraira (رضي الله عنه‎).
  41. ^ Liu, Caitlin (April 20, 2005). "Sleepy Juror Gets Rude Awakening" (Document). Los Angeles Times. {{cite document}}: Unknown parameter |url= ignored (help)