Boomerang effect (psychology)
Part of a series on |
Psychology |
---|
In social psychology, the boomerang effect, also known as "reactance", refers to the unintended consequences of an attempt to persuade resulting in the adoption of an opposing position instead. It is sometimes also referred to as "the theory of psychological reactance", stating that attempts to restrict a person's freedom often produce an "anticonformity boomerang effect".[1] In other words, the boomerang effect is a situation where people tend to pick the opposite of what something or someone is saying or doing because of how it is presented to them. Typically, the more aggressively a position is presented to someone, the more likely they are to adopt an opposing view.[2]
Conditions and explanations
[edit]Early recognition
[edit]Hovland, Janis and Kelly[3] first recorded and named the boomerang effect in 1953, noting that it is more likely under certain conditions:
- When weak arguments are paired with a negative[clarification needed] source.
- When weak or unclear persuasion leads the recipient to believe that the communicator is propounding a different position to that which the communicator really intends.
- When the persuasion triggers aggression or unalleviated emotional arousal.
- When the communication adds to the recipient's knowledge of the norms and increases their conformity.
- When non-conformity to their own group results in feelings of guilt or social punishment.
- When the communicator's position is too far from the recipient's position and thus produces a "contrast" effect and thus enhances their original attitudes.
Later in 1957, Hovland, Sherif and Harvey[4] further discussed the necessity of understanding these unintended attitude changes in persuasion communication and suggested possible approaches for analysis via underlying motivational processes, psychophysical stimuli, as well as ego-involving verbal material. Jack Brehm and Arthur Cohen were among the first to provide theoretical explanations.
Jack Brehm[5] first raised attention to the phenomenon a fait accompli that might conceivably create dissonance if an event has led to the opposite behavior predicted at a prior point. He conducted an experiment to examine the behaviors of eighth graders eating a disliked vegetable. About half of them were told that their parents would be informed on the vegetable they ate. Then liking the vegetable was measured before and after the procedure. The results show that for kids who indicated little or no discrepancy between serving and actually eating the disliked vegetable at home, they should experience little or no dissonance in liking the vegetable from the low to the high consequence condition. They thereby concluded that the greater was the individual's initial dislike, the greater was the pressure produced by the experiment to increase his liking. In Jack Brehm's experiment it shows how even at a young age we are greatly impacted by the boomerang effect and it can have positive or negative outcomes that come with it. There was also larger resistance to change the attitude when the initial attitude was more extreme. However, they argued that in this experiment, the pressure to reduce dissonance increased more rapidly with increasing discrepancy than did the resistance against change, which verified Festinger's cognitive dissonance theory.[6] In a follow-up,[7] Sensenig and Brehm focused on the boomerang effect in experiments and applied Brehm's psychological reactance theory[8] to explain the unintended attitudinal change.
Psychological reactance theory analysis
[edit]Sensenig & Brehm[7] applied Brehm's reactance theory[8] to explain the boomerang effect. They argued that when a person thinks that his freedom to support a position on attitude issue is eliminated, the psychological reactance will be aroused and then he consequently moves his attitudinal position in a way so as to restore the lost freedom. He told college students to write an essay supporting one side of five issues and led some of them believe that their persuasive essays might influence the decision on those issues. Therefore, the people who had the impression that their preference was taken into account in the decision regarding which side they would support on the 1st issue showed attitude change in favor of the preferred position, while others who are concerned with their freedom lost move toward the intended position held by the communicator.
This experiment resulted in various links in the chain of reasoning: (a) when a person's freedom is threatened, his motivational state will move toward restoration of the threatened freedom; (b) the greater the implied threatened freedoms, the greater the tendency to restore the threatened freedom will be; (c) the reestablishment of freedom may take the form of moving one's attitudinal position away from the position forced by others.
Jack Brehm and Sharon Brehm later developed psychological reactance theory[1] and discussed its applications.[9] They also listed a series of reactions reactance can evoke in addition to the boomerang effect, which includes but is not limited to related boomerang effect,[10][11] indirect restoration[1] or vicarious boomerang effects.[10][11]
Cognitive dissonance theory analysis
[edit]The dissonance theory by Leon Festinger[6] has thrived the progress of social psychology research in the 1960s as it is not confined to the prediction of intended influence but can support almost all sub fields of psychology studies. Although Festinger himself was ambiguous about the role of commitment in the theory, later researchers such as Brehm[12] and Cohen[13] have emphasized its importance in providing a general conceptualization of the boomerang effect. Earlier studies by Thibaut and Strickland[14] and Kelley and Volkhart[15] have also provided support to this line of reasoning by Dissonance Theory despite that they were not phrased using the exact terminology.
According to Cohen,[13] dissonance theory can provide not only an explanation, but also a prediction of both the intended and the unintended influence of persuasion communication on attitudinal change. According to Saul McLeod, Cognitive dissonance refers to a situation involving conflicting attitudes, beliefs or behaviors. This produces a feeling of mental discomfort leading to an alteration in one of the attitudes, beliefs or behaviors to reduce the discomfort and restore balance. For example, when people smoke (behavior) and they know that smoking causes cancer (cognition), they are in a state of cognitive dissonance.[16] Showing how the dissonance theory directly correlates to the boomerang effect has made impact on our knowledge of why humans act and can be influenced the way that they are.
In his experiment, he presented factors that can lead to a boomerang effect, while suggesting a broader view of the unintended consequences than simply the case of a response to attempted attitude change. Cohen proposed the following dissonance formulation model for the unintended attitude change by persuasive communication. First, suppose that dissonance aroused in regard to some unspecified cognition. According to Festinger's Cognitive Dissonance Theory, we know the dissonance could be reduced by a change in the cognition. Now suppose the resistance to change is great because the actual event cannot be changed and its meaning is ambiguous (for example, the person is strongly committed to the original cognition position), then the person will resort to other forms to reduce or eliminate the dissonance. In this latter form, one can solve the discrepancy problem through the addition of elements consonant either with the original cognition, in which produced the boomerang effect. Cohen formulated a situation of "mutual boomerang effect", in which the communicator is strongly committed to convince the other person of his attitudinal position by means of a persuasion communication.
Because of this strong original attitude position the communicator holds, Cohen predicts that the more distant the target person's original attitude, the more dissonance will be also experienced by the communicator. The expected "unintended influence" arises when the communicator tried to persuade the other of the worth of his own position by becoming even more extreme in that position. He asked his subjects write a strongly persuasive essay to the partners with an opposite side of attitude on an issue, who are actually confederates. The subjects here thus act as the communicator to bring their partners over to their own sides. The subjects were also asked to rate the partners' likability and friendliness before they read "their partner's essay" returned. Cohen used attitude change of the partners as the manipulation of dissonance where he randomly allocated his subjects into high-dissonance group and low-dissonance group. The results exposed strong boomerang effects for high-dissonance group. He also found out that the response to the likability and friendliness of the partners are relevant. The data showed that the difference between dissonance conditions was largely confined to and exaggerated for those subjects who originally rated their partners to be relatively more likable and friendly.[citation needed]
Cohen's study on boomerang effect has broadened the scope of persuasive communication from merely the recipient's reaction to the persuasive message to the communicator's attempt to influence the target. Dissonance theory suggests that the basic issue is under what conditions a person strengthens his original attitude as a way of reducing some attitudinal inconsistency. Cohen suggested that, one can reduce the dissonance via boomerang when dissonance is created (a) with a strong commitment to convincing the other person, (b) with no anticipation of a further influence attempt, and (c) with no easy chance to repudiate the other person. His results on the likability have strengthened the interpretation as the low-dissonance group who found their partners likable and friendly move toward them in the attitudes more, while likability only increased dissonance for the highs.
In other words, the dissonance can be reduced by becoming more extreme in the original position, thereby increasing the proportion of cognition supporting the initial stand and decreasing the proportion of dissonant cognition.
Other analysis
[edit]Boomerang effect is sometimes also referred to the attribution/attitude boomerang effect. Researchers applied Heider's attribution theory[17] to explain why it would occur. For example, Skowronski, Carlston, Mae, and Crawford demonstrated association-based effects in their study on spontaneous trait transference.[18] Despite that the descriptions of other people are independent of the communicator, simple associative processes link the two together and produce boomerang phenomena.
Examples of applications
[edit]Consumer behavior
[edit]Wendlandt and Schrader[19] studied the resistance of consumers against loyalty programs encountered in relationship marketing. They found that (a) contractual bonds provoke reactance effects, (b) social-psychological bonds increased neither reactance nor perceived utility of the program, (c) economic bonds raised perceived utility to a certain threshold level, from which the reactance effect dominated afterwards. Their results helped managers to evaluate the effects from implementing consumer retention measures and advised a cautious and limited application of loyalty programs. In 2017, a study was done to test the significance of the boomerang effect, Based on this 2×2 matrix we designed a pilot and three experimental studies that examine the different possible combinations. In the pilot study we tested several different biases and contexts to make sure that they are indeed perceived as socially sensitive by participants. In study 1 we replicated the results of Nasie et al. (2014), and demonstrated how teaching people about a neutral bias that might be relevant to their behavior in a neutral context changed their behavior and reduced the bias (combination #1). In Study 2 we taught decision-makers about an inherently sensitive bias that may imply they were holding chauvinistic views in a sensitive social context, of female representation in politics (combination #4). Finally, to test if a sensitive social context is enough to evoke the boomerang effect even toward a neutral bias, in Study 3 we taught decision makers about a neutral bias in a sensitive social context of a gender in the workplace (combination #3). In Studies 2 & 3, we hypothesized that the boomerang effect predicted by self-affirmation literature (Schumann & Dweck, 2014; Sherman & Cohen, 2006; Steele, 1988) would be a result of participants being motivated to justify their actions and stay set in their ways in order to avoid being labeled as chauvinistic or misogynistic. Since inherently sensitive biases are always linked to sensitive social contexts combination #2 was covered by Study 2 as well, and did not call for another Study.[20] In the study they found that using the boomerang effect had a significantly positive outcome. This is an example on how some individuals were aware of the boomerang effect and some were not but both showed a positive result.
Deliberate exploitation
[edit]The tactic of reverse psychology, which is a deliberate exploitation of an anticipated boomerang effect, involves one's attempt of feigning a desire for an outcome opposite to that of the truly desired one, such that the prospect's resistance will work in the direction that the exploiter actually desires (e.g., "Please don't fling me in that briar patch").
Persuasive health communication
[edit]Researchers have reported that some public health interventions have produced effects opposite to those intended in health communication such as smoking and alcohol consumption behaviors, and thus have employed various methods to study them under different contexts. Ringold argued that some consumer's negative reactions on alcoholic beverage warnings and education efforts can be explained concisely by Brehm's psychological reactance theory.[21] These results suggested that boomerang effects should be considered as potential costs of launching mass communication campaigns. Dillard and Shen also emphasized the importance of reactance theory to understand failures in persuasive health communication but argued that there be a measurement problem.[22] They thereby developed four alternative conceptual perspectives on the nature of reactance as well as provided an empirical test of each. Hyland and Birrell[23] found that a government health warning on cigarette advertisements published in 1979 led to a "boomerang effect" leading to an increase desire to smoke after viewing the campaign. The results of their study indicated that the presence of a more aggressive health warning in an advertisement increased the desire to smoke and it decreased the perceived goodness of the advertisement. This means the people viewing the sign decided it was not a helpful campaign to decrease smoking. Another negative impact of this effect happens in drug and supplement marketing. Bolton et al.[24] researched how the marketing of health drugs and supplements lead to less healthy life style due to the drugs' marketing reducing risk perceptions and perceived importance of, and motivation to engage in, complementary health-protective behaviors. Consumers believe it is not important to have a healthy lifestyle if they use the drugs and supplements.
In a 2010 study, researchers explored ways to reduce and/or eliminate the boomerang effect. 289 undergraduate students from the University of Georgia participated, and each of them was assigned to a random section of laptop computers, which would play public service announcement videos on different message topics (on drunk-driving and anti-smoking) and with varying levels of empathy present.[25] After watching each of the five public service announcements they were assigned, participants recorded their immediate perceptions, and rated each clip on how much it invoked an emotional response within them. Students recorded their attitude towards the advocacy of each clip, completed a trait empathy scale and stated some demographic information.[25] Results in this study suggested that empathy-induced messages in storytelling seemed to reduce psychological reactance in users, therefore reducing the boomerang effect.[25]
The Boomerang Effect has also shown up in different public health campaigns. In the 1980s, more campaigns for seatbelt laws were happening around America. In addition, more laws were also being created. However, this caused mixed reactions with some American citizens. While seatbelts were included in cars starting in 1968, there wasn’t legislation endorsing them.[26] Around the late 1970s and into the 1980s, only 14 percent of Americans consistently wore their seatbelts.[27] Some citizens believed the inclusion of these seatbelt laws was resistance towards their rights in a free society. For example, when state representative David Hollister introduced that there would now be a monetary fine if a driver is found not wearing a seatbelt, he received hate mail comparing him to Hitler.[26]
Environmental behaviors
[edit]Mann and Hill[28] investigated the case of litter control and showed that the combination of different positive influence strategies could actually create boomerang effect and decrease the amount of appropriate disposal of waste. Schultz et al. (2007) conducted a field experiment in which the normative messages were used to promote household energy conservation where they found the descriptive message of neighborhood usage created a boomerang effect depending on the high prior household consumption. They also eliminated the boomerang effect by adding an injunctive message about social approval. Their results offered an empirical evidence for prior research on the theoretical framework for boomerang effects.[29] Swatuk et al.[30] found a boomerang effect regarding a call of climate control action from locals after The Paris Agreement. The call was considered 'maladaptation' due to it calling for displacement of communities from traditional lands. They suggest careful articulation of policy and program decisions to improve policy making.
Helping
[edit]Schwartz and Howard discussed the occurrence of boomerang effects in helping as they found out the presence of certain factors presumed to activate norms favoring helping actually result in decreasing helping.[31] They identified three related forms of such boomerang effect in helping behavior. First, when individuals perceived the framing of a help appeal to have excessive statements of need, they become suspicious and concern the motive and the true severity of the original request (i.e., mistrust). Reactance theory was used to provide the second explanation. They stated that individuals would respond to threatened freedoms by either acting counter to the attempted social influence, or declaring themselves helpless. The third type involves undermining internalized benefits by external sanctions.
National and human security
[edit]Liotta attempted to understand policy decisions and future choices driven by a blurring of concerns that involve state-centric security and human security. She suggested that a boomerang effect occurs in the area in which excessive focus on one aspect of security at the expense or detriment of the other is a poor balancing of ends and means in a changing security environment and instead we should focus on both national and human security.[32]
Political beliefs
[edit]Nyhan & Reifler[33] conducted experiments in which subjects read mock news articles including a misleading claim from a politician, or such a claim followed by a correction. They found that the corrections frequently fail to reduce misconceptions for the ideological group targeted by the misinformation. They also found cases of what they called a "backfire effect" (i.e. a boomerang effect) in which the corrections strengthened belief in the misinformation. They attribute this to motivated reasoning on the part of the affected participants. Later research did not find evidence of this effect though, suggesting it was at least not prevalent.[34][35]
Current Research
[edit]Alex Kresovich conducted a 2022 study that examined the influence of pop culture artists. The study[36] conducted multiple surveys using US youth (ages 16–24). Kresovich examined how pop music artists who discuss mental health problems affect their younger audiences. The information was retrieved from the years 2017-2021. Kresovich used the survey method to experiment if contemporary pop music artists would be the most efficient spokespeople about this issue. The study had two different rounds of experiments. Firstly, Kresovich tried using footage from pop artist's mental health campaigns. In this footage, Kresovich categorized two different types of language being used to address the audience (direct language versus mistargeted language). The second round of experiments focused more on if positively relaying the mental health information would be more effective in relating to the younger audience.[36]
For the first set of experiments, Kresovich used footage of direct language versus mistargeted language (referring to the audience as "you" versus more personally as a friend). The findings suggest that using celebrities in public service messages to discuss mental health issues like depression and advocating for support would cause the boomerang effect in its reached audience. Using this strategy with celebrities as the spokesperson increased the stigmatized beliefs in the US youth.
For the second round of experiments, Kresovich showed the celebrities that used more positive associations with depression in order to view it more positively. However, this action led to increased stigma and depression romanticizing from the young audience, both of which are consequential responses to the public health campaign.[36]
Current Examples
[edit]The boomerang effect takes place all around us in our lives. When you tell a toddler to not touch the flowers on the table or to pet the cat nicely, they turn around and knock the flowers over and yank on the cat’s tail. A study was done by Xiomeng Fan, Fengyan Cindy Cai, and Galen V. Bodenhausen on zero pricing incentives[37] in consumer demand. In this study, the comparison of a zero price versus a low price on consumers. The hypothesis was that zero pricing effects depend on the incidental costs that are associated with it. Once the study was finished, the conclusion was that zero pricing ended up raising consumer demand, when the incidental effects were lower.[37] The boomerang effect plays into this because zero pricing incentives can go either way; it depends on which way they are advertised. If they advertised with lower incidental costs, zero pricing is less effective because consumers are not risking anything on lower cost, more common items. When incidental costs are higher, zero pricing incentives are also higher because consumers are not risking anything on nicer, higher-priced items.[37]
COVID-19 is an extremely relevant and current example of the boomerang effect. Mask wearing and social distancing were very prevalent situations that were encouraged and enforced in the height of the pandemic. However, many groups of people refused to follow the suggestions and later the mandates. There were billboard ads, commercials, signs, and many more different kinds of visual and audio messages for the public to wear a mask if they needed to go out and to maintain a distance of six feet between others at all times in order to minimize the spread of the virus. This did not work for everyone because there was always a group of people who refused to wear a mask when they went grocery shopping. It resulted in other angry customers or the refusal of service from the business. The boomerang effect was demonstrated every day during the COVID-19 pandemic because of the opposite reaction that was given by the advertisements.[38] The CDC would put out a video to raise awareness for raising a mask, and those who disagreed would do the opposite and refuse to put a mask on, both indoors and outdoors.
Although a bit more dated than the previous two examples, a third example of the boomerang effect is the Murray-Darling Basin. This basin idea was thought of due to a long-term drought in Australia from 1997-2009.[39] There was a group created for the advocacy and creation of the basin, but it did not turn out as planned. The members of this authority group made it their mission to bring awareness to the public of all the good that the basin can help create and to help with water allocations across the country.[39] The basin was meant to help in future drought scenarios to prevent the issues that arose during the most recent drought and water issues. However, those outside of the authority group saw other economic, social, and environmental issues that would arise with the making of the basin.[39] The boomerang effect is demonstrated here because the authority group sent out messages advocating for all of the positives of the basin, but the opposite message was taken and the public saw more of the negative outcomes.
Mental Health Impact of COVID-19
[edit]Since the onset of the global pandemic, COVID-19, there has been a noticeable increase in awareness surrounding mental health. An article from Mike Ryan at BCC stated, "But what it does mean is that the vast majority of the world remains at risk. We know the pandemic will continue to evolve. We also know we have the tools that work to suppress transmission and save lives right now. They are at our disposal."[40] People's mental stability and strength have been something psychologists and family members have taken very seriously over the past couple of years. The pandemic has significantly affected individuals in high-stress professions, such as nursing. A 2020 study revealed that depressed nurses exhibited lower levels of reaction compared to their calmer counterparts, emphasizing the importance of addressing the mental well-being of healthcare professionals.[41] Another study done in 2021 examines anxiety disorders due to the COVID-19 pandemic. The need for up-to-date information on the mental health impacts of COVID-19 is imperative. This study had important insight on mental health and COVID-19 by stating, "In this study, we aimed to quantify the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the prevalence and burden of major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders globally in 2020."[42] This underscores the broader conclusion that the mental health of those in demanding occupations, like nursing and medicine, is at a higher risk due to the challenging nature of their work.
Sports and Mental Health
[edit]Mental health plays into the sports field more now than ever before, with more awareness around concussions and other significant injuries that have gained more attention. The National Library of Medicine researched multiple studies showing the impact of mental health within sports. There were many takeaways from this article, but the section on training was influential. "Physical training is concentrated towards increasing maximum performance and capacity. On the other hand, physical inactivity is a sedentary behavior in which body movement is absent. Physical inactivity leads to increased risk of poor health, which can affect the well-being of an individual."[43] Sports organizations have used the new mental health issues to increase the attention on player well-being. For example, the WWE, if there is a severe collision, WWE doctors will stop the match, something that did not happen just a few years ago. A notable contact sport athlete, Ronda Rousey, talked about the WWE and their stoppages. Rousey went on to say, "Nikki Bella gave me an open-hand slap [in the days leading up to the match], and I was seeing stars, and I had a headache for the rest of the day."[44] This led to their match being cut short because of the injury.
The most notable example is the NFL implementation of an enhanced concussion protocol to address the long-term effects of brain injuries. Antonio Brown is a prime example of how mental health and sports complement each other. Brown told ESPN in 2019, "If I had CTE (chronic traumatic encephalopathy), I wouldn't be able to have this beautiful gym or be as creative. I wouldn’t be able to communicate. I’m perfectly fine.” A study done by the Concussion Institution showed that Brown could be suffering from a more severe dose of CTE. "Those comments illustrate a fundamental misunderstanding of CTE. Later- stage CTE (stages 3 and 4) is associated with dementia. Still, early-stage CTE (stages 1 and 2) is more associated with what is called neurobehavioral dysregulation, which includes violent, impulsive, or explosive behavior, inappropriate behavior, aggression, rage, “short fuse,” and lack of behavioral control."[45] Brown’s life-changing experience on the field highlights the potential consequences of sustained physical impact, prompting a broader discussion about mental health within the sports community.
Role of Sports in Society
[edit]When overlooking the impact sports have on people's lives and emotions, we found that sports have a profound impact on society and can help with transcending mere entertainment. Major events like the Super Bowl have become significant income generators. An article published by Virginia Tech stated that "The Super Bowl may not be the most gambled-on sporting event, but it is a significant time for sports betting. Last year, approximately 50 million gamblers collectively wagered an estimated $16 billion on the game, with roughly one in five Americans participating in some form."[46] The ability of sports to provide an escape, especially during challenging times like the COVID-19 pandemic, is evident. The concept of a sports "bubble," where rigorous protocols allow for competition in a controlled environment, played a role in positively affecting both fan's and player's mental health. The Bubble had multiple aspects that were not just basketball-driven. The University of Michigan quoted, "The Bubble provided the NBA with a unique marketing opportunity of highlighting its tight-knit community to the rest of the world. By allowing the players to have social justice messages written on the back of their jerseys, the Bubble became synonymous with calls for social and political reform. A fostered sense of community within the Bubble, which the NBA utilized to bolster its brand image.[47] This Bubble is where the athletes and coaches were tested every day and had to follow the multiple protocols involved with COVID-19 just to be able to play. Everyone won, especially when the fans were allowed to come back and support their teams or favorite organizations.[48]
Sports and Social Movements
[edit]The murder of George Floyd triggered a global movement, particularly in the Black Lives Matter (BLM) community, influencing the sports world significantly. Major sports organizations, including the NBA,[49] NHL,[50] and NFL,[51] responded by taking action supporting players and organizations advocating for racial equality. The support led to a platform for athletes, especially those of color, to voice their concerns and contribute to the broader conversation on racial injustice.[52] This brings questions to the NFL and how they are responding to the players and organizations actions and how much freedom they have to express how they feel about this topic. Knowing all of this about the NFL, the amount of attention brought to the Black Lives Matter movement was fascinating. This would include all of the slogans, quotes, symbols, and so on that the players wear on the backs of their shirts, helmets, shoes, and other items. These real-life situations tie back to Jack Brehm and his discovery of the boomerang effect. One of Brehm's original study involved two toys. These toys were different in shape and size but similar in color. Children had the freedom to choose whatever toy they would like to have. Brehm did multiple studies to try and prove his theory of the boomerang effect.[53] Any amount of freedom can create negativity and influence people to do things they would not normally do. Therefore, Brehm found the boomerang effect.
Boomerang Effect in Sports Organizations
[edit]The boomerang effect, as described in Jack Brehm's theory, prompted concerns about its presence in sports organizations. Colin Kaepernick's decision to kneel during the national anthem as a form of protest for the Black Lives Matter movement serves as a pivotal example.[54] The boomerang effect is when someone is trying to persuade someone to do a specific action, but they decide to do the complete opposite of the action they were told to do.[55] At the time of the incident there was a lot of unlawful killing of people of color and black people were not getting the same opportunities. Kaepernick's decision changed how sports organizations and everyday people view the ongoing movement. Kaepernick's goal was to bring awareness and to show the actions that sparked a movement within sports, with more players and associations joining in support over time, and despite the initial backlash, changed the way people look at social movements.[54] There was a lot of backlash from some NFL fans, but as time went on, more and more players started to take a knee for the anthem. Other significant organizations, such as the NBA[56] and the NHL,[57] recognized the impact Kaepernick's decision had and began to promote the Black Lives Matter movement. Kaepernick took the first step by kneeling, effectively sacrificing himself to start a movement. As stated above, initially hesitated at first to take a knee and protest because of the fear of losing their jobs or getting fined. As time went on, the entire NFL and other associations began to take a stand for their players and the unjust treatment going on in the world. All of this highlights how a seemingly controversial action can have a broader positive impact, challenging the conventional norms within sports organizations.
Related effects
[edit]- Backfire effect – Maintaining a belief despite new information that firmly contradicts it
- Sleeper effect – Psychological phenomenon
- Streisand effect – Increased awareness of information caused by efforts to suppress it
See also
[edit]- Unintended consequences – Unforeseen outcomes of an action
- Imperial boomerang – Concept in political science
- Persuasion – Umbrella term of influence and mode of communication
- Reactance (psychology) – Unpleasant emotion experienced when behavioral freedom is threatened
References
[edit]- ^ a b c Brehm, Sharon S.; Brehm, Jack Williams (1981). Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-129840-1.[page needed]
- ^ Cohen, Arthur R. (March 1962). "A dissonance analysis of the boomerang effect1". Journal of Personality. 30 (1): 75–88. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1962.tb02306.x. PMID 13880221.
- ^ Hovland, Carl Iver; Janis, Irving Lester; Kelley, Harold H. (1953). Communication and Persuasion: Psychological Studies of Opinion Change. Greenwood Press. ISBN 978-0-313-23348-7.[page needed]
- ^ Hovland, Carl I.; Harvey, O. J.; Sherif, Muzafer (1957). "Assimilation and contrast effects in reactions to communication and attitude change". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 55 (2): 244–252. doi:10.1037/h0048480. PMID 13474895.
- ^ Brehm, Jack W. (May 1959). "Increasing cognitive dissonance by a fait accompli". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 58 (3): 379–382. doi:10.1037/h0047791. PMID 13653889. S2CID 105750.
- ^ a b Festinger, Leon (1962). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford University Press. ISBN 978-0-8047-0911-8.[page needed]
- ^ a b Sensenig, J.; Brehm, J. W. (April 1968). "Attitude change from an implied threat to attitudinal freedom". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 8 (4): 324–330. doi:10.1037/h0021241. PMID 5645590. S2CID 15768110.
- ^ a b Brehm, Jack Williams (1966). A Theory of Psychological Reactance. Academic Press. OCLC 1070025516.[page needed]
- ^ Brehm, Jack W. (1989). "Psychological Reactance: Theory and Applications". Advances in Consumer Research. 16: 72–75.
- ^ a b Quick, Brian L.; Stephenson, Michael T. (2007). "Further Evidence That Psychological Reactance Can be Modeled as a Combination of Anger and Negative Cognitions". Communication Research. 34 (3): 255–276. doi:10.1177/0093650207300427. S2CID 34608676.
- ^ a b Quick, Brian L.; Stephenson, Michael T. (2008). "Examining the Role of Trait Reactance and Sensation Seeking on Perceived Threat, State Reactance, and Reactance Restoration". Human Communication Research. 34 (3): 448–476. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00328.x.
- ^ Brehm, J. W. (1960). "A dissonance analysis of attitude-discrepant behavior". In Rosenberg, Milton J.; Hovland, Carl I.; McGuire, William J.; Abelson, Robert P.; Brehm, Jack W. (eds.). Attitudes organization and change: an analysis of consistency among attitude components. Yale University Press. pp. 164–197. OCLC 442255684.
- ^ a b Cohen, Arthur R. (1962). "A dissonance analysis of the boomerang effect1". Journal of Personality. 30: 75–88. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1962.tb02306.x. PMID 13880221.
- ^ Thibaut, John W.; Strickland, Lloyd H. (1956). "Psychological Set and Social Conformity1". Journal of Personality. 25 (2): 115–129. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1956.tb01292.x. PMID 13385786.
- ^ Kelley, Harold H.; Volkart, Edmund H. (1952). "The Resistance to Change of Group-Anchored Attitudes". American Sociological Review. 17 (4): 453–465. doi:10.2307/2088001. JSTOR 2088001.
- ^ Mcleod, Saul (3 November 2022). "What Is Cognitive Dissonance? Definition and Examples". Simply Psychology.
- ^ Heider, F. (2013). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. doi:10.4324/9780203781159. ISBN 978-1-134-92218-5.[page needed]
- ^ Skowronski, John J.; Carlston, Donal E.; Mae, Lynda; Crawford, Matthew T. (1998). "Spontaneous trait transference: Communicators take on the qualities they describe in others". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (4): 837–848. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.837. PMID 9569648.
- ^ Wendlandt, Mark; Schrader, Ulf (2007). "Consumer reactance against loyalty programs". Journal of Consumer Marketing. 24 (5): 293–304. doi:10.1108/07363760710773111.
- ^ Levy, Aharon; Maaravi, Yossi (2018-01-02). "The boomerang effect of psychological interventions" (PDF). Social Influence. 13 (1): 39–51. doi:10.1080/15534510.2017.1421571. S2CID 96438727.
- ^ Ringold, Debra Jones (2002). "Boomerang Effects in Response to Public Health Interventions: Some Unintended Consequences in the Alcoholic Beverage Market". Journal of Consumer Policy. 25: 27–63. doi:10.1023/A:1014588126336. S2CID 152818207.
- ^ Dillard, James Price; Shen, Lijiang (2005). "On the Nature of Reactance and its Role in Persuasive Health Communication". Communication Monographs. 72 (2): 144–168. doi:10.1080/03637750500111815. S2CID 145303261.
- ^ Hyland, Michael; Birrell, James (April 1979). "Government Health Warnings and the 'Boomerang' Effect". Psychological Reports. 44 (2): 643–647. doi:10.2466/pr0.1979.44.2.643. PMID 461656. S2CID 38334649.
- ^ Bolton, Lisa E.; Reed, Americus; Volpp, Kevin G.; Armstrong, Katrina (February 2008). "How Does Drug and Supplement Marketing Affect a Healthy Lifestyle?". Journal of Consumer Research. 34 (5): 713–726. doi:10.1086/521906.
- ^ a b c Shen, Lijiang (7 June 2010). "Mitigating Psychological Reactance: The Role of Message-Induced Empathy in Persuasion". Human Communication Research. 36 (3): 397–422. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01381.x.
- ^ a b Roos, Dave (31 August 2020). "When Americans Resisted Seat Belt Laws". History.com.
- ^ How States Achieve High Seat Belt Use Rates. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 2008.[page needed]
- ^ Hill, Thomas; Mann, Millard F. (1984). "Persuasive Communications and the Boomerang Effect: Some Limiting Conditions to the Effectiveness of Positive Influence Attempts". Advances in Consumer Research. 11: 66–70.
- ^ Schultz, P. Wesley; Nolan, Jessica M.; Cialdini, Robert B.; Goldstein, Noah J.; Griskevicius, Vladas (2007). "The Constructive, Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms". Psychological Science. 18 (5): 429–434. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x. hdl:10211.3/199684. PMID 17576283. S2CID 19200458.
- ^ Swatuk, Larry A.; Thomas, Bejoy K.; Wirkus, Lars; Krampe, Florian; Batista da Silva, Luis Paulo (2 January 2021). "The 'boomerang effect': insights for improved climate action". Climate and Development. 13 (1): 61–67. Bibcode:2021CliDe..13...61S. doi:10.1080/17565529.2020.1723470. S2CID 212895133.
- ^ Schwartz, S.H.; Howard, J.A. (1981). "A Normative decision-Making Model of Altruism". In Rushton, J. Philippe; Sorrentino, Richard M. (eds.). Altruism and Helping Behavior: Social, Personality, and Developmental Perspectives. L. Erlbaum Associates. pp. 189–211. ISBN 978-0-89859-155-2.
- ^ Liotta, P. H. (2002). "Boomerang Effect: The Convergence of National and Human Security". Security Dialogue. 33 (4): 473–488. doi:10.1177/0967010602033004007. S2CID 154729974.
- ^ Nyhan, Brendan; Reifler, Jason (2010). "When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political Misperceptions". Political Behavior. 32 (2): 303–330. doi:10.1007/s11109-010-9112-2. S2CID 10715114.
- ^ Mantzarlis, Alexios (2 November 2016). "Fact-checking doesn't 'backfire,' new study suggests". Poynter.
- ^ Wood, Thomas; Porter, Ethan (2019). "The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes' Steadfast Factual Adherence". Political Behavior. 41: 135–163. doi:10.1007/s11109-018-9443-y. S2CID 151582406. SSRN 2819073.
- ^ a b c Kresovich, Alex (2022). The Influence of Celebrity Pop Music Artists Who Disclose Mental Health Difficulties on the Depression Support-Seeking Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions of At-Risk U.S. Youth (Thesis). The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill University Libraries. doi:10.17615/wxkg-te05. ProQuest 2714895805.[page needed]
- ^ a b c Fan, Xiaomeng; Cai, Fengyan Cindy; Bodenhausen, Galen V. (May 2022). "The boomerang effect of zero pricing: when and why a zero price is less effective than a low price for enhancing consumer demand". Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. 50 (3): 521–537. doi:10.1007/s11747-022-00842-1. PMC 8852885. PMID 35194264.
- ^ Li, Tom; Liu, Yan; Li, Man; Qian, Xiaoning; Dai, Susie Y. (14 August 2020). "Mask or no mask for COVID-19: A public health and market study". PLOS ONE. 15 (8): e0237691. Bibcode:2020PLoSO..1537691L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0237691. PMC 7428176. PMID 32797067.
- ^ a b c Gale, Melanie; Edwards, Merinda; Wilson, Lou; Greig, Alastair (June 2014). "The Boomerang Effect: A Case Study of the Murray-Darling Basin Plan: The Boomerang Effect". Australian Journal of Public Administration. 73 (2): 153–163. doi:10.1111/1467-8500.12051.
- ^ Mahase, Elisabeth (2022-05-19). "Covid-19: Second boosters may benefit at-risk groups but have "minimal" impact for others, says WHO". BMJ. 377: o1259. doi:10.1136/bmj.o1259. ISSN 1756-1833. PMID 35589118.
- ^ "APA Upgrades APA PsycNET Content Delivery Platform". PsycEXTRA Dataset. 2017. doi:10.1037/e500792018-001. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
- ^ Mani, Subha (2022-05-10). "Impact of COVID-19 on Mental Health". AEA Randomized Controlled Trials. doi:10.1257/rct.9419-1.0. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
- ^ Congsheng, Lu; Kayani, Sumaira; Khalid, Amna (2022-09-02). "An empirical study of physical activity and sports affecting mental health of university students". Frontiers in Psychology. 13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.917503. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 9478583. PMID 36118484.
- ^ "Veranstaltungstipps". Kma - Klinik Management Aktuell. 29 (2/03): 25. March 2024. doi:10.1055/s-0044-1785578. ISSN 1439-3514.
- ^ "11656, 1828-03-03, FRIES (Cte Maurice de)". Art Sales Catalogues Online. doi:10.1163/2210-7886_asc-11656. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
- ^ "INFORMS in the News". Volume 45, Number 1, February 2018. 2018-08-01. doi:10.1287/orms.2018.01.09. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
- ^ Montenegrin Journal of Economics. 11 (2). 2015-12-20. doi:10.14254/1800-5845.2015/11-2. ISSN 1800-5845 https://doi.org/10.14254%2F1800-5845.2015%2F11-2.
{{cite journal}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help) - ^ academic.oup.com https://academic.oup.com/jalm/article/8/6/1017/7288728#. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
{{cite web}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help) - ^ Schmidt, Samuel H.; Frederick, Evan L.; Kennedy, Heather; Pegoraro, Ann (March 2023). "An Examination of MLB, NBA, NFL, NHL, and WNBA Team Social Media Statements in Response to George Floyd's Death". Journal of Sports Media. 18 (1): 153–181. doi:10.1353/jsm.2023.a919645. ISSN 1940-5073.
- ^ Symmes, Tommy (2020-11-16). "Sound Systems at the George Floyd Protests in Minneapolis During the Summer of 2020". Dancecult. 12 (1). doi:10.12801/1947-5403.2020.12.01.06. ISSN 1947-5403.
- ^ Daly, Dan, "JOHNNY BLOOD", One for the Thumb, University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 43–46, doi:10.2307/j.ctt155jmwj.10, retrieved 2024-04-12
- ^ Powers, Madison (2019-09-19), "What Structural Injustice Is", Structural Injustice, Oxford University Press, pp. 85–115, doi:10.1093/oso/9780190053987.003.0004, ISBN 978-0-19-005398-7, retrieved 2024-04-12
- ^ Bhasin, Hitesh (2021-12-01). "Boomerang Effect - Definition, Theory and Examples". Marketing91. Retrieved 2024-04-17.
- ^ a b Joseph, Jared (2018). Local vs. National: How Twitter Reflects News Coverage of Colin Kaepernick Protests (Thesis). Louisiana State University Libraries. doi:10.31390/gradschool_theses.4783.
- ^ Hilse, Annika (2024). "Mit der App zum schadstofffreien Gebäude". Technische Sicherheit. 14 (3–04): 12–13. doi:10.37544/2191-0073-2024-03-04-12. ISSN 2191-0073.
- ^ Sturt, Andy; Humayun, Muhammad Fahad (2022-04-10). "Magic 10 and NBA Bubble Sports Journalists' Practices During NBA Season 2019–2020". Journalism Practice. 18 (3): 529–547. doi:10.1080/17512786.2022.2054851. ISSN 1751-2786.
- ^ Suri, idhika (2024-04-06). "Football Player Classification". International Journal of Scientific Research in Engineering and Management. 08 (4): 1–5. doi:10.55041/ijsrem30220. ISSN 2582-3930.