Race and intelligence: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Line 262: Line 262:


Although the literature on RT is vast, far less research has looked at race differences on RT as a potential explanation for the race/IQ gap. The general pattern, however, is that race differences exist on ECT performance, and that these differences are in line with those found on traditional IQ tests. For example, a recent study in the journal Intelligence looked at race differences on the [[Wonderlic Personnel Test]] (a traditional paper and pencil IQ test) and performance on two ECTs (an inspection time and choice reaction time task). A black/white difference was found on the Wonderlic, and both ECTs. [[Statistical mediation]] was found in that controlling for race differences on the ECTs resulted in the race difference on the Wonderlic no longer being significant.
Although the literature on RT is vast, far less research has looked at race differences on RT as a potential explanation for the race/IQ gap. The general pattern, however, is that race differences exist on ECT performance, and that these differences are in line with those found on traditional IQ tests. For example, a recent study in the journal Intelligence looked at race differences on the [[Wonderlic Personnel Test]] (a traditional paper and pencil IQ test) and performance on two ECTs (an inspection time and choice reaction time task). A black/white difference was found on the Wonderlic, and both ECTs. [[Statistical mediation]] was found in that controlling for race differences on the ECTs resulted in the race difference on the Wonderlic no longer being significant.

===Caste-like minorities===
The book ''[[Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]]'' (1996) claims that it is not lower average intelligence that leads to the lower status of racial and ethnic minorities, it is instead their lower status that leads to their lower average intelligence test scores. To substantiate this claim, the book presents a table comparing social status or caste position with test scores and measures of school success in several countries around the world.<ref name="bell myth">''[http://press.princeton.edu/chapters/s5877.html Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]'' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 192.</ref> The authors note, however, that the comparisons made in the table do not represent the results of all relevant findings, nor do they reflect the fact that the tests and procedures varied greatly from study to study. The comparison of Jews and Arabs, for example, is based on a news report that, in 1992, 26% of Jewish high school students passed their matriculation exam, as opposed to 15% of Arab students.<ref name="bell myth">''[http://press.princeton.edu/chapters/s5877.html Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]'' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 191.</ref>


===Rearing conditions===
===Rearing conditions===

Revision as of 06:20, 24 April 2010

Template:Systemic bias

Race and intelligence research investigates differences in the distributions of cognitive skills among human races. Debates in popular science and academic research over the possible connection of race and intelligence originally began as a comparison of African Americans and Caucasians in the United States, but were later extended to other races and regions of the world. In the US, intelligence quotient (IQ) tests have consistently demonstrated statistical differences: the scores of the African American population are on average lower than that of the White American population; the Asian American population on average scores higher; Amerinds scores on average fall between Caucasian and African American scores. Similar findings have been reported for populations around the world, most notably in Africa, though these are generally considered far less reliable due to the relative paucity of test data and the difficulties inherent in the cross-cultural comparison of intelligence test scores. The distribution of IQ scores has considerable range – individuals in every racial group may have IQ's that lie anywhere on the spectrum of scores. These difference show primarily in aggregate studies.

There are no universally accepted definitions of either race or intelligence in academia, and many factors that could potentially influence the development of intelligence have been advanced to explain the racial IQ gap. There is general agreement that environmental and/or cultural factors affect individual IQ scores, and it is widely assumed that a significant portion of the racial IQ gap is attributable to such factors, though none are conclusively supported by direct empirical evidence. The more controversial view that a significant portion of the racial IQ gap is ultimately of genetic origin has been advanced by academics such as Arthur Jensen, J. Philippe Rushton and Richard Lynn. This claim met with widespread criticism in the popular media, particularly after the publication of "The Bell Curve", and has not to date gained acceptance by the wider academic community.

The racial IQ gap has remained relatively stable since IQ testing began, although IQ scores as a whole have themselves been subject to change over time. The American Psychological Association has concluded that the racial IQ gap is not the result of bias in the content or administration of tests, but that no adequate explanation of it has so far been given.[1]

History

Alfred Binet (1857–1911), inventor of the first intelligence test

The history of the race and intelligence controversy concerns the historical development of a debate, primarily in the United States, concerning possible explanations of group differences in intelligence. Although it has never been disputed that there are systematic differences between average scores in IQ tests of different population groups, sometimes called "racial IQ gaps", there has been no agreement on whether this is mainly due to environmental and cultural factors, or whether some inherent hereditarian factor is at play, related to genetics.

In the late nineteenth and early twentieth century, group differences in intelligence were assumed to be due to race and, apart from intelligence tests, research relied on measurements such as brain size or reaction times. By the mid-1930s most psychologists had adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role. In 1969 the educational psychologist Arthur Jensen published a long article reviving the older hereditarian point of view, with the suggestion that eugenics was more likely to increase the average intelligence in the US than remedial education for blacks. His work, publicized by the Nobel laureate William Shockley, sparked controversy amongst the academic community and even led to student unrest. A similar debate amongst academics followed the publication in 1994 of The Bell Curve, a book by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray which argued in favor of the hereditarian viewpoint. It not only provoked the publication of several interdisciplinary books on the environmental point of view, some in popular science, but also led to a public statement from the American Psychological Association acknowledging a gap between average IQ scores of whites and blacks as well as the absence of any adequate explanation of it, either environmental or genetic. The hereditarian line of research continues to be pursued by a group of researchers, mostly psychologists, some of whom are supported by the Pioneer Fund.[2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9]

Group differences

Intelligence is most commonly measured using IQ tests. These tests are often geared to measure the psychometric variable g (for general intelligence factor). Other tests that measure g (e.g, the Armed Forces Qualifying Test, SAT, GRE, GMAT and LSAT) also serve as measures of cognitive ability. Several conclusions about these types of tests are now largely accepted:[1][10][11][12]

  • IQ scores measure many of the qualities that people mean by intelligent or smart.
  • IQ scores are fairly stable over much of a person's life.
  • IQ tests predict school and job performance to a degree that does not significantly vary by socio-economic or racial-ethnic background.
  • Intelligence is heritable.
  • Family environment and community culture affect IQ, more so in children than in adults.

Test scores

Most of the evidence of intelligence differences between racial groups is based on studies of IQ test scores, almost always using self-reported racial data. Self-reports have been shown to be reliable indicators of genetic race to the extent that they match up with genetic clusters derived from mathematical clustering techniques, but these techniques do not determine whether these clusters themselves have any relation to intelligence.[13] There are observed differences in average test score achievement between racial groups, which vary depending on the populations studied and the type of tests used. In the United States, self-identified Blacks and Whites have been the subjects of the greatest number of studies. Black-White average IQ differences appear to increase with age, reaching an average of nearly 17 points by age 24, which is slightly more than 1 standard deviation.[14] Using data primarily from the United States and Europe, Jensen and Rushton have estimated the average IQ of Blacks/Africans to be around 85; of whites/Europeans to be around 100, and of East Asians to be around 106.[15] Estimates from other researchers are more or less similar.[1][16]

Arthur Jensen has found, further, that black and white populations regress to these different means, using Galton’s Law of Ancestral Heredity,[17] and argues that this supports a genetic theory, on the grounds that it is difficult to explain this kind of regression based on environmental factors.[18] Richard Nisbett recognizes the existence of this effect, but believes that it could be produced by environmental factors alone, such as parenting practices and subculture pressures.[19] Further research by Jensen and other researchers using structural equation modeling concluded that a model in which genetic and environmental contributions to the IQ gap are in roughly equal proportions best fit the data.[20]

Gaps are seen in other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, including university admission exams, military aptitude tests and employment tests in corporate settings.[21]

The IQ distributions of other racial and ethnic groups in the United States are less well studied. The few Amerindian populations that have been systematically tested, including Arctic Natives,[22][23] tend to score worse on average than White populations but better on average than Black populations.[21] East Asian populations score higher on average than White populations in the United States as they do elsewhere.[13]

Racial differences in IQ scores are observed around the world.[24][25][26] Lynn's meta-analysis lists East Asians (105), Europeans (102), Inuit (91), Southeast Asians and Amerindians (87 each), Pacific Islanders (85), South Asians/North Africans (84), Non-Bushmen sub-Saharan Africans (67), Australian Aborigines (62) and Bushmen (54).[27][28][26][29][25] This data is generally considered less reliable than data from the United States and Europe, both because of the questions about the validity of Lynn’s methods and because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures.[30][31] International achievement test scores, including TIMSS and PISA, have also been used to estimate average IQ worldwide where data is available, producing results similar to those reported by Lynn.[32][33][34]

African IQ

The very low IQ scores reported for sub-Saharan African populations (average of 70) are particularly controversial.[35][36][37] According to anthropologist Mark Cohen, using Western standards this would mean that African countries should be largely dysfunctional. Given that individuals in these countries lead "vibrant artistic, symbolic and spiritual lives", according to Cohen these scores are not likely to be accurate.[38] Nicholas Mackintosh has also raised doubts as to whether Kalahari bushmen, whose average IQ Lynn gives as 54, would be capable of learning the skills required for surviving in a desert environment if this score accurately represented their intelligence.[39]

A large body of research has shown that average IQ scores of states and nations correlates significantly with a number of other factors including average health, average income, infant mortality and crime.[40][41][42] This correlation is in fact stronger among developing countries in Sub-Saharan Africa than it is in Western countries.[43][44] In a 1988 literature review, S. H. Irvine, John W. Berry have also reported that individual IQ scores in Southern Africa have the same predictive validity regardless of race, so that an African white with a score of 70 would tend to have the same occupational and educational performance as an African black of the same score, and the same would be true of African blacks and whites with IQs of 100. However, these authors disagree with Lynn about the low average IQ of African blacks being due partially to genetic differences, concluding instead that they are primarily the result of cultural and environmental factors.[45]

Debate assumptions and methodology

Race and intelligence research involves debate over the links, if any, between race and intelligence. This research is grounded in two controversial assumptions:

Both assumptions are disputed.

There are several conflicting positions. Some scientists argue that the history of eugenics makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science.[46][47] Other scientists insist that, independent of ethical concerns, research into race and intelligence makes little sense because intelligence is poorly measured and because race is a social construction.[48] According to this view, intelligence is ill-defined and multi-dimensional, or has definitions that vary between cultures. This would make contrasting the intelligence of groups of people, especially groups that came from different cultures, dependent mainly on which culture’s definition of intelligence is being used. Moreover, this view asserts that even if intelligence were as simple to measure as height, racial differences in intelligence would still be meaningless since race exists only as a social construct, with no basis in biology.

Unsurprisingly, almost all scientists actively engaged in research in race and intelligence disagree with these two positions.[49] These researchers fall into two groups: hereditarians and environmentalists. Both argue that, although race and intelligence are fuzzy concepts, they can be operationalized enough to draw conclusions about the connections, if any, between the two. In this research, race is almost always measured via self-identification. Subjects are presented with a set of racial options and allowed to place themselves in one (or more) category, or are placed by an interviewer. The set of categories allowed and the words used to describe them varies from study to study and from country to country. Intelligence is generally measured with some form of IQ test.

Hereditarians argue that genetics explain a significant portion (approximately 50%) of the differences in measured intelligence among human races. Leading scholars of this view include Arthur Jensen, Philippe Rushton, Richard Herrnstein, Linda Gottfredson, Charles Murray and Richard Lynn.

Environmentalists argue that the hereditarians are wrong, and that genetic differences are not an important cause of differences in measured intelligence among human races. Leading scholars of this view include Richard Lewontin, Stephen J. Gould, James Flynn, Richard Nisbett and Stephen Ceci.

Other scientists, although accepting the basic assumptions of the debate, believe that there is currently not enough evidence to determine what part, if any, genetics plays in racial differences.[1]

In theory, the dispute could be resolved with a simple experiment. Select 1,000 fertilized eggs at random from the relevant racial groups, say White, Black and East Asian. Place all 1,000 in identical environments, both pre- and post-natal. This would involved creating three separate societies which would be perfectly equal and as similar as possible to contemporary developed countries. Nutrition, family environment, education, popular culture and all other factors which might influence intelligence would need to be identical. For example, the Nobel Prize winners in the White society would all be White, those in the Black society would all be Black and so on.

Then, after 18 years, give the thousand subjects from each racial group an intelligence test. If the group averages are the same, then the hereditarian hypothesis is refuted. Since ethical and practical issues make such an experiment impossible, researchers in race and intelligence need to rely on simpler and less conclusive experiments and data analysis.[50]

There are two primary methods for controlling factors (education, income and so on) which are correlated with IQ test scores and which co-vary with race The first constrains participant selection so that members of all races are equal on the factor in question. For example, if a researcher thinks education is the explanation for the difference, then she could compare the IQs of only similarly-educated members of each group. Showing that the difference is zero for persons matched on education levels would suggest that education is the cause of the difference. Showing that the difference remains for similarly educated people would make it less likely that education differences across race explain differences in average IQ. The second method is similar to the first, but uses statistics (rather than participant selection) to control the factor.

Debate overview

Richard Nisbett,[51][52] in replying to hereditarian arguments,[53][27][15][54][55] structures the debate into several major areas.

Heritability within and between groups

There is a consensus among intelligence researchers that IQ, like height, within the same population is significantly heritable.[1][56][57][58][59][60][61] However, it is the subject of debate whether the factors causing one group to have a higher or lower average than another are the same as the factors that cause individuals within the same group to differ amongst themselves.

An environmental factor that varies between groups but not within groups can cause group differences in a trait that is otherwise 100% heritable. The height of this "ordinary genetically varied corn" is 100% heritable, but the difference between the groups is totally environmental. This is because the nutrient solution varies between populations, but not within populations.[62]

Much of the research on this topic has been conducted by Arthur Jensen and James Flynn. Flynn and Jensen consider two general classes of environmental factors: common environmental factors, which vary both within and between groups; and X-factors, which vary between groups but not within groups. Flynn explains in Race, IQ and Jensen (1980) why common environmental factors are inadequate as an explanation for the IQ gap:

After all, if an environmental factor is potent enough to account for the 15-point performance gap between black and white, and if it varies much from person to person within the black population, it would be extremely odd if it accounted for none of the variable performance within the black population! And if it did, it would of course increase the role of environmental factors in explaining IQ variance and thus lower the h2 (within-group heritability) estimate for blacks. [...] If we seize on SES (socio-economic status) as a between-population explanation, who can deny that there are large differences in SES within black America; if we seize on education, who can deny that blacks differ significantly in terms of quality of education?[63]

The alternative to common environmental factors is the hypothesis that the racial IQ gap can be accounted for by X-factors: factors which vary between groups but not within groups. A frequently-cited example of an X-factor from Richard Lewontin describes two populations of corn, one of which is grown in a normal environment, and the other of which is grown in a nutrient-deficient environment. The height of this corn is 100% heritable when it is grown in a uniform environment. Therefore in such a scenario, the within-group heritability of height is 100% in both populations, but the substantial difference between groups are due entirely to environmental factors. Jensen and Flynn agree that no X-factors have yet been identified that could account for the racial IQ gap. Jensen believes that under these circumstances, the “default hypothesis” should be that the differences in average IQ between races is caused by the same factors that cause within-group variance in IQ, while Flynn believes that the racial IQ gap is caused by X-factors that have yet to be discovered.[64]

Score convergence

The overall average Black-White gap has reduced by one third over the course of the 20th century. For example, the black men inducted into the US armed forces during World War II averaged about 1.5 standard deviations below their white counterparts.[65] This improvement is also reflected in Black-White differences on school achievement tests, which have shrunk from about 1.2 to about 0.8 standard deviations. Flynn claims that the Black-White gap has reduced throughout the 20th century[66]. However, Murray claims that these improvements may have stalled for people born after the early 1970s [67].

Flynn effect

Although modern IQ tests are unbiased[68], average test scores over the last century have risen steadily around the world. This rise is known as the "Flynn effect," named for James R. Flynn, who did much to document it and promote awareness of its implications. The effect increase has been continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to the present.

This means, given the same test, the mean performance of Blacks today could be higher than the mean for Whites in 1920, though the gains causing this appear to have occurred predominantly in the lower half of the IQ distribution. If an unknown environmental factor can cause changes in IQ over time, then contemporary differences between groups could also be due to an unknown environmental factor.

Nichols (1987)[69] critically summarized the argument as follows:

  1. We do not know what causes the test score changes over time.
  2. We do not know what causes racial differences in intelligence.
  3. Since both causes are unknown, they must, therefore, be the same.
  4. Since the unknown cause of changes over time cannot be shown to be genetic, it must be environmental.
  5. Therefore, racial differences in intelligence are environmental in origin.

Dickens (2005) states that "Although the direct evidence on the role of environment is not definitive, it mostly suggests that genetic differences are not necessary to explain racial differences. Advocates of the hereditarian position have therefore turned to indirect evidence ... The indirect evidence on the role of genes in explaining the Black-White gap does not tell us how much of the gap genes explain and may be of no value at all in deciding whether genes do play a role. Because the direct evidence on ancestry, adoption, and cross-fostering is most consistent with little or no role for genes, it is unlikely that the Black-White gap has a large genetic component."[70]

Spearman's Hypothesis

An illustration of Charles Spearman's two-factor intelligence theory. Each small oval is a hypothetical mental test. The blue areas show the variance attributed to a specific content of the test and the purple areas the variance attributed to g, the general intelligence factor. Newer and more complex theories of g now exist.

Spearman's hypothesis asserts that group differences on intelligence test scores are caused primarily by group differences on the general intelligence factor (abbreviated g). The general factor is a statistical construct that measures what is common to the scores of all IQ test items. How well a person does on one IQ sub-test is usually correlated with how well he or she does on other sub-tests. This is the essence of g.

Jensen developed a statistical technique known as the method of correlated vectors to test Spearman's hypothesis. The idea is that a rank ordering of IQ sub-test items by g-loadings should correlate with the magnitude of the race difference on those items, if indeed g is their cause. For example, digit span backward is more g-loaded than is digit-span forward. And, the race difference on the former is about twice as large as the race difference on the latter.

Spearman's hypothesis is not without its critics. Psychologists Hunt and Carlson write[13] write:

One of the most widely cited pieces of evidence (although not the only one) for biological differences in intelligence, sometimes referred to as Spearman's hypothesis (Jensen, 1998), rests on an indirect argument constructed from three facts. The first is that various IQ measures are substantially correlated, providing evidence for general intelligence. Although tests do vary in the extent of their g loading, factor structures are similar over several test batteries (Johnson, Bouchard, Krueger, McGue, & Gottesman, 2004). The second is that, within Whites, the g factor appears to have a substantial genetic component (see citations in Rushton & Jensen, 2005a). The third fact is that the g loadings of tests are substantially and positively correlated with the difference between the mean White and African American score on each subtest within a battery of tests. This analysis has been referred to as the "method of correlated vectors" (Jensen, 1998). Because it has also been well established that general intelligence has a substantial genetic component, results from the method of correlated vectors have been offered as putative evidence that the "default hypothesis" ought to be that about 50% of the variance in the African American versus White difference reflects genetic differences in a potential for intelligence (Jensen, 1998; Rushton and Jensen, 2005a).

They[13] further summarize criticisms of this position:

Technical objections have been made to the method of correlated vectors and to a somewhat stronger condition: that if the within-group correlations between measures are identical across groups, the between-group differences must arise from the same cause as the within-group correlations (Widaman, 2005). The essence of these objections is that the method of correlated vectors does not consider alternative hypotheses concerning the latent traits that might give rise to the observed difference in test scores. When a more appropriate method of analysis, multigroup confirmatory factor analysis, is applied, it has been found that Spearman's hypothesis (i.e., that the difference is due to differences in general intelligence) is only one of several models that could give rise to the observed distributions in test scores (Dolan, 2000). These findings render the method of correlated vectors ambiguous—which is not the same as saying that the Jensen-Rushton position is incorrect. Our point is that the argument for the default hypothesis is an indirect one. It would be far better if a direct causal argument could be made linking racial/ethnic genetic differences to studies of the development of the brain.

Variables potentially affecting intelligence in groups

Socioeconomic environment

File:TBC-BW-IQ-SES-withDiff.png
Socioeconomic status (SES) varies both between and within populations, but Black-White differences in IQ persist among the children of parents matched for SES, and the gap is largest among the children of wealthiest and best educated parents.[71]

According to the report of a 1996 APA task force, socioeconomic factors (SES) cannot be the whole explanation for racial-ethnic group differences in IQ. Their first reason for this conclusion is that that the black-white test score gap is not eliminated when individuals and groups are matched on SES. Second, excluding extreme conditions, nutritional and biological factors that may vary with SES have little effect on IQ. Third, the relationship between IQ and SES is not simply one in which SES determines IQ, rather it is more likely that intelligence causes differences in SES than the other way around. Lastly, they argue that income and education simply fail to capture important categories of cultural experience which differ between racial and ethnic groups.[1]

Health and nutrition

Environmental factors including lead exposure[72], breast feeding[73], and nutrition[74][75] can significantly affect cognitive development and functioning. For example, iodine deficiency causes a fall, in average, of 12 IQ points [76]. Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth. Comprehensive policy recommendations targeting reduction of cognitive impairment in children have been proposed.[77]

Geographic ancestry

African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors.[78] Several studies performed without the use of DNA-based ancestry estimation attempted to correlate estimates of African or European ancestry with IQ. These studies have found that mixed-race individuals tended to have IQ intermediate between those of unmixed blacks and whites, with a correlation of .17 between the estimated degree of difference in ancestry and the size of the difference in average IQ.[79][80] These studies have been criticized for their imprecise method of estimating ancestry, which was based primarily on skin tone, as well as for their small sample sizes.

Rowe (2005) and others have suggested using DNA-based methods to reproduce these studies with reliable estimates of ancestry. Such experiments have never been published, although the requirements for such a study have been discussed in the academic literature.[81]

Stereotype threat

Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance.[82] Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups that already score lower on average. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups but do not explain the gaps found in non-threatening test conditions.

A 2009 meta-analysis by Jelte Wicherts found evidence of significant publication bias in 55 studies of stereotype threat and its effect on IQ, in which those that found a strong effect were more likely to be published than those which did not. Reviewing both the published and unpublished studies, Wicherts found that stereotype threat did not have an effect on all test-taking settings in which a difference in average scores is observed between races, and therefore was not an adequate explanation for the racial IQ gap.[83]

Brain size

On average, the brains of African-Americans are 5% smaller than the brains of Whites and 6% smaller than East Asians, according to studies of brain weight at autopsy, endocranial volume of empty skulls, head size measurements by the U.S. military and NASA, and two dozen MRI volumetric studies[84][85][86][87][88]. Proponents of both the environmental and hereditarian perspective believe that this variation is relevant to the racial IQ gap, although they disagree as to its cause. Ulric Neisser, The Chair of the APA’s Task Force on intelligence, acknowledges the brain size difference, but points out that brain size is known to be influenced by environmental factors such as nutrition, and that this fact has been demonstrated experimentally in rats. He thus believes that data on brain size cannot be considered strong evidence for a genetic component to the IQ difference.[89] Rushton and Jensen disagree, citing several studies of malnourished East Asians showing that they have larger brains than Whites, and studies demonstrating the brain size difference at birth and prenatally just a few weeks after conception. [90] [91]

A third perspective is offered by Leonard Lieberman, who believes that human variation in brain size is primarily genetic and an adaptation to climate, but that this variation should be viewed as being based on biogeographic ancestry and independently of “race”.[92]

Much of the research into the neuroscience of intelligence has involved indirect approaches, such as searching for correlations between psychometric test scores and variables associated with the anatomy and physiology of the brain. Historically, research was conducted on non-human animals or on postmortem brains. More recent studies have involved non-invasive techniques such as MRI scans as they can be conducted on living subjects. MRI scans can be used to measure the size of various structures within the brain, or they can be used to detect areas of the brain that are active when subjects perform certain mental tasks.

In a study of the head growth of 633 term-born children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children cohort, it was shown that prenatal growth and growth during infancy were associated with subsequent IQ. The study’s conclusion was that the brain volume a child achieves by the age of 1 year helps determine later intelligence.[93] Within human populations, studies conducted to determine whether there is a relationship between brain size and a number of cognitive measures have "yielded inconsistent findings with correlations from 0 to 0.6, with most correlations 0.3 or 0.4."[94]

A study on twins showed that frontal gray matter volume was correlated with g and highly heritable.[95] A related study has reported that the correlation between brain size (reported to have a heritability of 0.85) and g is 0.4, and that correlation is mediated entirely by genetic factors.[96]

Processing efficiency

Reaction time (RT) is the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. RT is often used in experimental psychology to measure the duration of mental operations, an area of research known as mental chronometry. In psychometric psychology, RT is considered to be an index of speed of processing. That is, RT indicates how fast the thinker can execute the mental operations needed by the task at hand. In turn, speed of processing is considered an index of processing efficiency. The behavioral response is typically a button press but can also be an eye movement, a vocal response, or some other observable behavior.

Scores on many but not all RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on paper and pencil IQ tests. This is especially true for so-called elementary cognitive tasks (ECTs). These require participants to perform trivially simple cognitive tasks, like deciding which of two briefly-presented lines is longer (the inspection time task), or which of three lighted buttons is farthest away from the other two (the odd man out task).

Most people can perform ECTs with near 100% accuracy, but individual differences in RT on these tasks are large and correlate well with IQ scores. Jensen (2001) argues that ECTs could replace traditional IQ tests as measures of intelligence, because the former are measured on a ratio scale whereas IQ tests only rank people on an ordinal scale. Jensen has invented a Jensen box to present ECT task stimuli to participants in a precise, standardized fashion.

Not all RT tasks, however, are good measures of intelligence. In general, RT on tasks that take between 200 milliseconds and 2 seconds to perform tend to correlate well with IQ. Tasks that most people can do faster than 200 milliseconds generally measure the efficiency of sensory processes (seeing, hearing) rather than intelligence. Tasks that take longer than about 2 seconds typically allow for strategic differences among people which cloud any relationship between RT and IQ (for these tasks, accuracy-- versus speed-- is likely more related to IQ).

Reaction time best predicts IQ test scores when participants perform many trials (i.e., 100s) of the same ECT. Aggregating average reaction times across different ECTs also produces significantly larger RT/IQ correlations. In many studies, the within person variability of RT is also a strong predictor of IQ. Participants showing relatively large RT differences from trial to trial tend to score lower on IQ tests than do participants who do not deviate much in their reaction time from trial to trial. Finally, the slowest trials for any person tend to better predict that person's IQ relative to either his or her average or fastest response.

Although the literature on RT is vast, far less research has looked at race differences on RT as a potential explanation for the race/IQ gap. The general pattern, however, is that race differences exist on ECT performance, and that these differences are in line with those found on traditional IQ tests. For example, a recent study in the journal Intelligence looked at race differences on the Wonderlic Personnel Test (a traditional paper and pencil IQ test) and performance on two ECTs (an inspection time and choice reaction time task). A black/white difference was found on the Wonderlic, and both ECTs. Statistical mediation was found in that controlling for race differences on the ECTs resulted in the race difference on the Wonderlic no longer being significant.

Caste-like minorities

The book Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth (1996) claims that it is not lower average intelligence that leads to the lower status of racial and ethnic minorities, it is instead their lower status that leads to their lower average intelligence test scores. To substantiate this claim, the book presents a table comparing social status or caste position with test scores and measures of school success in several countries around the world.[97] The authors note, however, that the comparisons made in the table do not represent the results of all relevant findings, nor do they reflect the fact that the tests and procedures varied greatly from study to study. The comparison of Jews and Arabs, for example, is based on a news report that, in 1992, 26% of Jewish high school students passed their matriculation exam, as opposed to 15% of Arab students.[97]

Rearing conditions

The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study examined the IQ test scores of 130 black/interracial children adopted by advantaged White families.[98][99][100] The aim of the study was to determine the contribution of genetic factors to the poor performance of black children on IQ tests as compared to White children. The following table provides a summary of the results.[101][102][103]

Biological parents Number of children Initial testing 10-year follow-up
Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study initially tested at age 7
Black-black 21 91.4 83.7
Black-white 55 105.4 93.2
White-white 16 111.5 101.5
Biological children 101 110.5 105.5
Moore (1986) initially tested at age 7–10
Black-black 9 108.7 not done
Black-white 14 107.2 not done
Eyferth (1961) initially tested at age 5–13
Black-white 171 96.5 not done
White-white 70 97.2 not done

Policy relevance

In response to criticism that their conclusions would have a negative effect on society if they were to gain wide acceptance, Jensen and Rushton have justified their research in this area as being necessary to answer the question of how much racism should be held responsible for ethnic groups' unequal performance in certain areas. They maintain that when racism is blamed for disparities which are the result of biological differences, the result is mutual resentment, and unjustified punishment of the more successful group. They state:

[T]he view that one segment of the population is largely to blame for the problems of another segment can be even more harmful to racial harmony, by first producing demands for compensation and thereby inviting a backlash. Equating group disparities in success with racism on the part of the more successful group guarantees mutual resentment. As overt discrimination fades, still large racial disparities in success lead Blacks to conclude that racism is not only pervasive but also insidious because it is so unobservable and "unconscious." Whites resent that nonfalsifiable accusation and the demands to compensate blacks for harm they do not believe they caused.[15]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ a b c d e f Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J.; et al. (1996). "Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns" (PDF). American Psychologist. 51: 77–101. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) "African American IQ scores have long averaged about 15 points below those of Whites, with correspondingly lower scores on academic achievement tests. In recent years the achievement-test gap has narrowed appreciably. It is possible that the IQ-score differential is narrowing as well, but this has not been clearly established. The cause of that differential is not known; it is apparently not due to any simple form of bias in the content or administration of the tests themselves. The Flynn effect shows that environmental factors can produce differences of at least this magnitude, but that effect is mysterious in its own right. Several culturally-based explanations of the Black/White IQ differential have been proposed; some are plausible, but so far none has been conclusively supported. There is even less empirical support for a genetic interpretation. In short, no adequate explanation of the differential between the IQ means of Blacks and Whites is presently available."
  2. ^ Benjamin, Ludy T. (2006), Brief History of Modern Psychology, Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 188–191, ISBN 140513206X
  3. ^ Hothersall, David (2003), History of Psychology (4th ed.), McGraw-Hill, pp. 440–441, ISBN 0072849657
  4. ^ Lynn, Richard (2001), The science of human diversity: a history of the Pioneer Fund, University Press of America, ISBN 076182040X
  5. ^ Mackintosh, N.J. (1998), IQ and Human Intelligence, Oxford University Press, ISBN 019852367X
  6. ^ Maltby, John; Day; Macaskill, Ann (2007), Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence, Pearson Education, ISBN 0131297600 {{citation}}: Unknown parameter |furst2= ignored (help)
  7. ^ Richards, Graham (1997), Race, racism, and psychology: towards a reflexive history, Routledge, ISBN 0415101417
  8. ^ Tucker, William H. (2002), The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund, University of Illinois Press, ISBN 0252027620
  9. ^ Wooldridge, Adrian (1995), Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England c.1860-c.1990, Cambridge University Press, ISBN 0521395151
  10. ^ David J. Bartholomew (2004). Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0521544785.
  11. ^ Ian J. Deary (2001). Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0192893211.
  12. ^ N. J. Mackintosh (1998). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford University Press. ISBN 019852367X.
  13. ^ a b c d Earl Hunt and Jerry Carlson (2007). "Considerations Relating to the Study of Group Differences in Intelligence". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (2): 194-213."Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition. Tang et al. (2005) applied mathematical clustering techniques in order to sort genomic markers for over 3,600 people in the United States and Taiwan into four groups. There was almost perfect agreement between cluster assignment and individuals’ self-reports of racial/ethnic identification as White, Black, East Asian, or Latino." Cite error: The named reference "Hunt and Carlson" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  14. ^ James R. Flynn (2007). What Is Intelligence? Beyond the Flynn Effect. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0521880076.
  15. ^ a b c J. Philippe Rushton and Arthur Jensen (2005). "Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 11 (2): 235–294. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235.
  16. ^ James R. Flynn (2007). What Is Intelligence? Beyond the Flynn Effect. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0521880076.
  17. ^ Jensen's study matched black and white children for IQ and compared the IQs of their siblings, and found that siblings of black children had on average lower IQ scores than siblings of white children, suggesting that the two populations were regressing towards the different population means shown by the IQ gap. For example, black children with an IQ of 120 would tend to have siblings with IQ's averaging 100, while white children with a 120 IQ would have siblings averaging close to 110. Jensen 1973, pg. 107–109
  18. ^ Jensen 1998, pg. 467–472
  19. ^ Nisbett 2009 pg. 222–223
  20. ^ Jensen 1998, pg. 464–467
  21. ^ a b Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi= 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x instead.
  22. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1080/00207596608247156, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1080/00207596608247156 instead.
  23. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1080/00207596808246642, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1080/00207596808246642 instead.
  24. ^ "We should accept, then, without further ado that there is a difference in average IQ between blacks and white." Mackintosh (1998), page 150.
  25. ^ a b Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X
  26. ^ a b Lynn, R. (2006). Race Differences in Intelligence: An Evolutionary Analysis. Washington Summit Books. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |isbd= ignored (help)
  27. ^ a b Herrnstein, Richard J. (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press. ISBN 0-02-914673-9. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help) Cite error: The named reference "The Bell Curve" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  28. ^ Lynn, R. (1991). "Race Differences in Intelligence: A Global Perspective" (PDF). Mankind Quarterly. 31: 255–296. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |month= (help)
  29. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.004, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi= 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.004 instead.
  30. ^ E. Hunt & W. Wittmann (2008). "National intelligence and national prosperity". Intelligence. 36 (1): 1–9. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  31. ^ K. Richardson (2004). "Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations". Heredity. 92 (4): 359–360. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800418.
  32. ^ Rindermann, H. (2006). What do international student assessments measure?. Psychologische Rundschau, 57, 69–86.
  33. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.09.003, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2007.09.003 instead.
  34. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.06.001, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi= 10.1016/j.intell.2006.06.001 instead.
  35. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002 instead.
  36. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.009, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.009 instead.
  37. ^ Cohen, Mark N. year = 2005. "Race and IQ Again: A Review of Race: The Reality of Human Differences by Vincent Sarich and Frank Miele" (PDF). Evolutionary Psychology. 3: 255–262. {{cite journal}}: Missing pipe in: |first= (help)CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  38. ^ Cohen, Mark N. (2005). "Race and IQ Again: A Review of Race: The Reality of Human Differences by Vincent Sarich and Frank Miele". Evolutionary Psychology. Volume 3, pp. 255–262.
  39. ^ Mackintosh 2006, p. 94 "Can anyone seriously accept Lynn's conclusion that the majority of San Bushmen, whose average IQ is 54, are mentally retarded? Lynn sees no problem: an adult with an IQ of 54 has the mental age of an 8-year-old European, and 8-year-old European children would have no difficulty learning the skills needed to survive in the Kalahari desert".
  40. ^ Charlie L. Reeve and Debra Basalik. year = 2010. "Average state IQ, state wealth and racial composition as predictors of state health statistics: Partial support for 'g' as a fundamental cause of health disparities". Intelligence. 38: 282-289. {{cite journal}}: Missing pipe in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  41. ^ Bryan J. Pesta, Michael A. McDaniel and Sharon Bertsch. year = 2010. "Toward an index of well-being for the fifty U.S. states". Intelligence. 38: 160-168. {{cite journal}}: Missing pipe in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  42. ^ Charlie L. Reeve year = 2009. "Expanding the g-nexus: Further evidence regarding the relations among national IQ, religiosity and national health outcomes". Intelligence. 37: 495-505. {{cite journal}}: Missing pipe in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  43. ^ Earl Hunt and Werner Wittmann year = 2008. "National intelligence and national prosperity". Intelligence. 36: 1-9. {{cite journal}}: Missing pipe in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  44. ^ Hanushek, Eric A. and Woessmann, Ludger, The Role of Education Quality for Economic Growth (February 1, 2007). World Bank Policy Research Working Paper No. 4122. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=960379
  45. ^ S. H. Irvine, John W. Berry. Human abilities in cultural context. Cambridge University Press, 1988.
  46. ^ American Anthropological Association (1994), Statement on "Race" and Intelligence, retrieved March 31, 2010
  47. ^ Steven Rose (2009). "Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit". Nature. 457: 786–788. doi:10.1038/457786a.
  48. ^ Robert J. Sternberg, Elena L. Grigorenko, and Kenneth K. Kidd (2005). "Intelligence, Race, and Genetics" (PDF). American Psychologist. 60 (1): 46–59. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  49. ^ Stephen Ceci and Wendy M. Williams (2009). "Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? YES: The scientific truth must be pursued". Nature. 457: 788–789. doi:10.1038/457788a.
  50. ^ John C. Loehlin, Gardner Lindzey and J.N. Spuhler (1975). Race Differences in Intelligence. W H Freeman & Co. ISBN 0716707535.See pp. 5–6 for a discussion of similar hypotheticals.
  51. ^ Nisbett, Richard (2009). Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count. W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 0393065057.
  52. ^ Richard Nisbett (2005). "Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen (2005)" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 11 (2): 302–310. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302.
  53. ^ Jensen, Arthur (1969). "How Much Can We Boost IQ and School Achievement?". Harvard Educational Review. 39: 1–123. "So all we are left with are various lines of evidence, no one of which is definitive alone, but which, viewed all together, make it a not unreasonable hypothesis that genetic factors are strongly implicated in the average Negro-white intelligence difference. The preponderance of the evidence is, in my opinion, less consistent with a strictly environmental hypothesis than with a genetic hypothesis, which, of course, does not exclude the influence of environment or its interaction with genetic factors."
  54. ^ J. Philippe Rushton and Arthur R. Jensen (2005). "WANTED: More Race Realism, Less Moralistic Fallacy" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 11 (2): 328–336. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.328.
  55. ^ J. Philippe Rushton and Arthur R. Jensen (2010). "Race and IQ: A theory-based review of the research in Richard Nisbett's Intelligence and How to Get It" (PDF). The Open Psychology Journal. 3: 9–35.
  56. ^ R. J. Sternberg (2000) Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  57. ^ David J. Bartholomew (2004) Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  58. ^ Ian J. Deary. (2001) Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press
  59. ^ Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence, 24(1), 13–23.
  60. ^ Robert Plomin, John C. DeFries, Gerald E. McClearn, and Peter McGuffin (2000) Behavioral Genetics. Worth Publishers; Fourth Edition edition
  61. ^ Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  62. ^ How Heritability Misleads about Race
  63. ^ Flynn 1980, pg. 59-60
  64. ^ Flynn (1980) and Flynn (1999)
  65. ^ Loehlin, J. C., Lindzey, G., & Spuhler, J. N. (1975). Race differences in intelligence. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.
  66. ^ William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples". Psychological Science. 16 (10): 825–924.
  67. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004 instead.
  68. ^ "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attaintment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority." Mackintosh (1998), page 174.
  69. ^ Nichols, R. C. (1987). Interchange: Nichols replies to Flynn. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy (pp. 233–234). New York, NY: Falmer.
  70. ^ Genetic Differences and School Readiness Dickens, William T. The Future of Children – Volume 15, Number 1, Spring 2005, pp. 55–69
  71. ^ Reviewed in Neisser et al. (1996). Data from the NLSY as reported in figure adapted from Herrnstein and Murray (1994), p. 288.
  72. ^ Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study David C. Bellinger PhD, MSc1, Karen M. Stiles PhD, MN1, and Herbert L. Needleman MD1. Pediatrics Vol. 90 No. 6 December 1992, pp. 855–861
  73. ^ Caspi A, Williams B, Kim-Cohen J; et al. (2007). "Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (47): 18860. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704292104. PMC 2141867. PMID 17984066. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  74. ^ Ivanovic DM, Leiva BP, Pérez HT; et al. (2004). "Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain". Neuropsychologia. 42 (8): 1118–31. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022. PMID 15093150. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  75. ^ Saloojee H, Pettifor JM (2001). "Iron deficiency and impaired child development". BMJ. 323 (7326): 1377–8. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377. PMC 1121846. PMID 11744547. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  76. ^ Qian M, Wang D, Watkins WE; et al. (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  77. ^ Olness K (2003). "Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic". J Dev Behav Pediatr. 24 (2): 120–30. PMID 12692458. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  78. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909559107, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi= 10.1073/pnas.0909559107 instead.
  79. ^ Lynn, R. (2002). Skin color and intelligence in African Americans. Population and Environment, 23, 365–375.
  80. ^ Rowe, D. C., & Rodgers, J. L. (2002). Expanding variance and the case of historical changes in IQ means: A critique of Dickens and Flynn (2001). Psychological Review, 109, 759 –763.
  81. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60 instead.
  82. ^ Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005
  83. ^ Wicherts, Jelte M.; Cor de Haan (2009). "Stereotype threat and the cognitive test performance of African Americans". University of Amsterdam. [1]
  84. ^ Beals, K. L., Smith, C. L., & Dodd, S. M. (1984). Brain size, cranial morphology, climate, and time machines. Current Anthropology 25, 301–330.
  85. ^ Ho, K. C., Roessmann, U., Straumfjord, J. V., & Monroe, G. (1980). Analysis of brain weight: I and II. Archives of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine 104, 635–645.
  86. ^ Johnson F. W. & Jensen (1994). Race and sex differences in head size and IQ. Intelligence 18: 309–33
  87. ^ Rushton JP. (1997). Cranial size and IQ in Asian Americans from birth to age seven. Intelligence 25: 7–20.
  88. ^ Rushton JP (1991). Mongoloid-Caucasoid differences in brain size from military samples [and NASA]. Intelligence 15: 351–9.
  89. ^ Neisser, U. (1997). Never a dull moment. American Psychologist, 52, 79–81.
  90. ^ Jensen A.R. & Rushton J.P. (2005). Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability. Psychology, Public Policy and Law 11: 235–294
  91. ^ The Open Psychology Journal, 2010, 3, 9–35
  92. ^ Lieberman L. (2001). How “Caucasoids” Got Such Big Crania and Why They Shrank. Current Anthropology Vol. 42 No. 1.
  93. ^ Catharine R. Gale; et al. (2006). "The Influence of Head Growth in Fetal Life, Infancy, and Childhood on Intelligence at the Ages of 4 and 8 Years". PEDIATRICS. 118 (4): 1486–1492. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-262. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  94. ^ S. F. Witelson, H. Beresh and D. L. Kigar (2006). "Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factor". Brain. 129 (2). Oxford University Press: 386–398. doi:10.1093/brain/awh696.
  95. ^ Paul Thompson, Tyrone D. Cannon, Katherine L. Narr; et al. (2001). "Genetic influences on brain structure" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience. 4 (12): 1253–1258. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  96. ^ Danielle Posthuma, Eco J. C. De Geus, Wim F. C. Baare, Hilleke E. Hulshoff Pol, Rene S. Kahn and Dorret I. Boomsma (2002). "The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin". Nature Neuroscience. 5: 83–84. doi:10.1038/nn0202-83.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  97. ^ a b Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 192. Cite error: The named reference "bell myth" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  98. ^ S. Scarr and R.A. Weinberg (1976). "IQ test performance of black children adopted by white families". American Psychologist. 31: 726–739.
  99. ^ R.A. Weinberg, S. Scarr and I. D. Waldman (1992). "The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A follow-up of IQ test performance at adolescence". Intelligence. 16: 117–135.
  100. ^ I. D. Waldman, R.A. Weinberg and S. Scarr (1994). "Racial-group differences in IQ in the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A reply to Levin and Lyn". Intelligence. 19: 29–44.
  101. ^ John Loehlin (2000). Robert Sternberg (ed.). Handbook of Human Intelligence. p. 185.
  102. ^ K. Eyferth (1961). "Leistungern verscheidener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern in Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK)". Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie. 113: 222–41.
  103. ^ EGJ Moore (1986). "Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted black children". Dev Psychol. 22: 317–326.

References