Jump to content

Gender pay gap in the United States: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Removed for misleading usage. The source cited only refers to federal employees of the US government, not the total labor force (see talk page).
Edited revision 603789668 by 200.50.10.174 (talk)
Line 10: Line 10:
In 2010 the median income of FTYR workers was $42,800 for men, compared to $34,700 for women. The female-to-male earnings ratio was 0.81, slightly higher than the 2008 ratio.<ref name="BLS2011" /> The female-to-male earnings ratio of 0.81 means that, in 2009, female FTYR workers earned 19% less than male FTYR workers. The statistic does not take into account differences in experience, skill, occupation, education or hours worked, as long as it qualifies as [[Full-time|full-time work]]. However, in 2010, an economist testified to the [[United States Congress Joint Economic Committee|U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee]] that studies "''always'' find that some portion of the wage gap is unexplained" even after controlling for measurable factors that are assumed to influence earnings. The unexplained portion of the wage gap is attributed by some to [[gender discrimination]].<ref name="jec">[[United States Congress Joint Economic Committee]]. [http://jec.senate.gov/public/?a=Files.Serve&File_id=9118a9ef-0771-4777-9c1f-8232fe70a45c ''Invest in Women, Invest in America: A Comprehensive Review of Women in the U.S. Economy.''] Washington, DC, December 2010</ref>{{rp|80}}
In 2010 the median income of FTYR workers was $42,800 for men, compared to $34,700 for women. The female-to-male earnings ratio was 0.81, slightly higher than the 2008 ratio.<ref name="BLS2011" /> The female-to-male earnings ratio of 0.81 means that, in 2009, female FTYR workers earned 19% less than male FTYR workers. The statistic does not take into account differences in experience, skill, occupation, education or hours worked, as long as it qualifies as [[Full-time|full-time work]]. However, in 2010, an economist testified to the [[United States Congress Joint Economic Committee|U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee]] that studies "''always'' find that some portion of the wage gap is unexplained" even after controlling for measurable factors that are assumed to influence earnings. The unexplained portion of the wage gap is attributed by some to [[gender discrimination]].<ref name="jec">[[United States Congress Joint Economic Committee]]. [http://jec.senate.gov/public/?a=Files.Serve&File_id=9118a9ef-0771-4777-9c1f-8232fe70a45c ''Invest in Women, Invest in America: A Comprehensive Review of Women in the U.S. Economy.''] Washington, DC, December 2010</ref>{{rp|80}}


The estimates for the discriminatory component of the gender pay gap include 5%<ref name="justicetalking">{{cite web|url=http://www.justicetalking.org/ShowPage.aspx?ShowID=604|date=2007-05-28|accessdate=2012-03-13|title=Justice Talking: The Women's Equality Amendment / What Does It Mean and Is It Necessary?}}</ref>{{rp|2}} and in at least one study grow as men and women's careers progress.<ref name="jec" />{{rp|93}} One economist testified to Congress that hundreds of studies have consistently found unexplained pay differences which potentially include discrimination.<ref name="jec" />{{rp|80}} Another criticized these studies as insufficiently controlled, and opined that men and women would have equal pay if they made the same choices and had the same experience, education, etc.<ref name="justicetalking" />{{rp||3}} Other studies have found direct evidence of discrimination. For example, fewer replies to identical resumes with female names<ref name="jec" />{{rp|10}} and more jobs went to women when orchestras moved to blind auditions.<ref name="justicetalking" />
The extent to which discrimination plays a role in explaining gender wage disparities is somewhat difficult to quantify, due to a number of potentially confounding variables.<ref name="justicetalking">{{cite web|url=http://www.justicetalking.org/ShowPage.aspx?ShowID=604|date=2007-05-28|accessdate=2012-03-13|title=Justice Talking: The Women's Equality Amendment / What Does It Mean and Is It Necessary?}}</ref>{{rp|2}} However, at least one study has shown that discriminatory disparities, where they do exist, grow as men and women's careers progress.<ref name="jec" />{{rp|93}} One economist testified to Congress that hundreds of studies have consistently found unexplained pay differences which potentially include discrimination.<ref name="jec" />{{rp|80}} Another criticized these studies as insufficiently controlled, and opined that men and women would have equal pay if they made the same choices and had the same experience, education, etc.<ref name="justicetalking" />{{rp||3}} Other studies have found direct evidence of discrimination. For example, fewer replies to identical resumes with female names<ref name="jec" />{{rp|10}} and more jobs went to women when orchestras moved to blind auditions.<ref name="justicetalking" />


==Gender pay gap statistics==
==Gender pay gap statistics==

Revision as of 20:35, 11 April 2014

Median weekly earnings of full-time wage and salary workers, by sex, race, and ethnicity, 2009.[1]
Women's weekly earnings as a percentage of men's in the U.S. by age, 1979–2005

Male–female income difference, also referred to as the "gender gap in earnings" in the United States, and as the "gender wage gap", the "gender earnings gap", "gender income difference" and the "gender pay gap", refers usually to the ratio of female to male median yearly earnings among full-time, year-round (FTYR) workers.

The statistic is used by government agencies and economists, and is gathered by the United States Census Bureau as part of the Current Population Survey.

In 2010 the median income of FTYR workers was $42,800 for men, compared to $34,700 for women. The female-to-male earnings ratio was 0.81, slightly higher than the 2008 ratio.[2] The female-to-male earnings ratio of 0.81 means that, in 2009, female FTYR workers earned 19% less than male FTYR workers. The statistic does not take into account differences in experience, skill, occupation, education or hours worked, as long as it qualifies as full-time work. However, in 2010, an economist testified to the U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee that studies "always find that some portion of the wage gap is unexplained" even after controlling for measurable factors that are assumed to influence earnings. The unexplained portion of the wage gap is attributed by some to gender discrimination.[3]: 80 

The extent to which discrimination plays a role in explaining gender wage disparities is somewhat difficult to quantify, due to a number of potentially confounding variables.[4]: 2  However, at least one study has shown that discriminatory disparities, where they do exist, grow as men and women's careers progress.[3]: 93  One economist testified to Congress that hundreds of studies have consistently found unexplained pay differences which potentially include discrimination.[3]: 80  Another criticized these studies as insufficiently controlled, and opined that men and women would have equal pay if they made the same choices and had the same experience, education, etc.[4] Other studies have found direct evidence of discrimination. For example, fewer replies to identical resumes with female names[3]: 10  and more jobs went to women when orchestras moved to blind auditions.[4]

Gender pay gap statistics

Women's median yearly earnings relative to men's rose rapidly from 1980 to 1990 (from 60.2% to 71.6%), and less rapidly from 1990 to 2000 (from 71.6% to 73.7%) and from 2000 to 2010 (from 73.7% to 81%).[2][5]

By state

Women’s earnings as a percentage of men’s earnings, by state and Puerto Rico, 2007. Data from the Income, Earnings, and Poverty Data From the 2007 American Community Survey.[6]

In 2007, women’s earnings were lower than men's earnings in all states and the District of Columbia according to the Income, Earnings, and Poverty Data From the 2007 American Community Survey by the Census Bureau. The national female-to-male earnings ratio was 77.5%. In the South, five states (Maryland, North Carolina, Florida, Georgia, and Texas) and the District of Columbia had ratios higher than the national ratio, as did three states in the West (California, Arizona, and Colorado). Two states in the Northeast (Vermont and New York) had ratios higher than the national ratio. There were no states in the Midwest that had ratios higher than the national ratio. As a result, women’s earnings were closer to men’s in more states in the South and the West than in the Northeast and the Midwest.[6]

According to an analysis of Census Bureau data released by Reach Advisors in 2008, single childless women between ages 22 and 30 were earning more than their male counterparts in most United States cities, with incomes that were 8% greater than males on average. This shift is driven by the growing ranks of women who attend colleges and move on to high-earning jobs.[7]

By industry and occupation

Women's median usual weekly earnings as percentage of men's, for full-time workers, by industry, 2009.[8]

Women's median weekly earnings were lower than men's median weekly earnings in all industries in 2009. The industry with the largest gender pay gap was financial activities. Median weekly earnings of women employed in financial activities were 70.5% of men's median weekly earnings in that industry. Construction was the industry with the smallest gender pay gap, with women earning 92.2% of what men earned.[8][9]

In 2009, women’s weekly median earnings were higher than men’s in only four of the 108 occupations for which sufficient data were available to the Bureau of Labor Statistics. The four occupations with higher weekly median earnings for women than men were "Other life, physical, and social science technicians" (102.4%), "bakers" (104.0%), "teacher assistants" (104.6%), and "dining room and cafeteria attendants and bartender helpers" (111.1%). The four largest gender wage gaps were found in well-paying occupations such as "Physicians and surgeons" (64.2%), "securities, commodities and financial services sales agents" (64.5%), "financial managers" (66.6%), and "other business operations specialists" (66.9%).[1][10]

This is a chart illustrating the inequality in wages between men and women when educational attainment is controlled. 2006 data

Men's and fathers' rights activist Warren Farrell stated that in 2003 there were at least 39 jobs where women earned at least 5% more than men. He stated the higher pay for women over men ranged from a high of 43% higher pay for female sales engineers over their male counterparts, to an 5% higher pay for female advertising and promotions managers over their male counterparts.[11]

In 2009 Bloomberg News reported that the sixteen women heading companies in the Standard & Poor’s 500 Index averaged earnings of $14.2 million in their latest fiscal years, 43 percent more than the male average. Bloomberg News also found that of the people who were S&P 500 CEOs in 2008, women got a 19 percent raise in 2009 while men took a 5 percent cut.[12]

Several studies of women in the legal profession reveal persistent gaps in partnership numbers at major American Law Firms. Despite the fact that women have graduated from law schools in equal numbers for over twenty years, only 16-19% of law firm partners are women.[13]

By age

Women's median weekly earnings as percentage of men's, for full-time workers, by age, 2008.[14]

The earnings difference between women and men varies with age, with younger women more closely approaching pay equity than older women.[15]

In 2011, the Bureau of Labor Statistics reported women's median weekly earnings to be 81% of men's.[2]

According to Andrew Beveridge, a Professor of Sociology at Queens College, between 2000 and 2005, young women in their twenties earned more than their male counterparts in some large urban centers, including Dallas (120%), New York (117%), Chicago, Boston, and Minneapolis. A major reason for this is that women have been graduating from college in larger numbers than men, and that many of those women seem to be gravitating toward major urban areas. In 2005, 53% of women in their 20s working in New York were college graduates, compared with only 38% of men of that age. Nationwide, the wages of that group of women averaged 89% of the average full-time pay for men between 2000 and 2005.[16]

Explaining the gender pay gap

Any given raw wage gap can be decomposed into an explained part due to differences in characteristics such as education, hours worked, work experience, and occupation, and an unexplained part which is typically attributed to discrimination.[17] The U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee shows that "as explained inequities decrease, the unexplained pay gap remains unchanged.[18] Cornell University economists Francine Blau and Lawrence Kahn stated that while the overall size of the wage gap has decreased somewhat over time, the proportion of the gap that is unexplained by human capital variables is increasing.[19]

Using Current Population Survey (CPS) data for 1979 and 1995 and controlling for education, experience, personal characteristics, parental status, city and region, occupation, industry, government employment, and part-time status, Yale University economics professor Joseph G. Altonji and the United States Secretary of Commerce Rebecca M. Blank found that only about 27% of the gender wage gap in each year is explained by differences in such characteristics.[20]

Similarly, a comprehensive study by the staff of the U.S. Government Accountability Office found that the gender wage gap can only be partially explained by human capital factors and "work patterns." The GAO study, released in 2003, was based on data from 1983 through 2000 from a representative sample of Americans between the ages of 25 and 65. The researchers controlled for "work patterns," including years of work experience, education, and hours of work per year, as well as differences in industry, occupation, race, marital status, and job tenure. With controls for these variables in place, the data showed that women earned, on average, 20% less than men during the entire period 1983 to 2000. In a subsequent study, GAO found that the Equal Employment Opportunity Commission and the Department of Labor "should better monitor their performance in enforcing anti-discrimination laws."[21][22][23]

Using CPS data, U.S. Bureau of Labor economist Stephanie Boraas and College of William & Mary economics professor William R. Rodgers III report that only 39% of the gender pay gap is explained in 1999, controlling for percent female, schooling, experience, region, Metropolitan Statistical Area size, minority status, part-time employment, marital status, union, government employment, and industry.[24] Using data from longitudinal studies conducted by the U.S. Department of Education, researchers Judy Goldberg Dey and Catherine Hill analyzed some 9,000 college graduates from 1992–93 and more than 10,000 from 1999–2000. The researchers controlled for workplace flexibility, ability to telecommute, as well as other several variables including occupation, industry, hours worked per week, whether employee worked multiple jobs, months at employer, and several education-related and "demographic and personal" factors, such as "marital status," "has children," and "volunteered in past year." The study found that wage inequities start early and worsen over time. "The portion of the pay gap that remains unexplained after all other factors are taken into account is 5 percent one year after graduation and 12 percent 10 years after graduation. These unexplained gaps are evidence of discrimination, which remains a serious problem for women in the work force."[25][26][27]

Economists Francine Blau and Lawrence Kahn took a set of human capital variables such as education, labor market experience, and race into account and additionally controlled for occupation, industry, and unionism. While the gender wage gap was considerably smaller when all variables were taken into account, a substantial portion of the pay gap (12%) remained unexplained.[28]

A study by John McDowell, Larry Singell and James Ziliak investigated faculty promotion on the economics profession and found that, controlling for quality of Ph.D. training, publishing productivity, major field of specialization, current placement in a distinguished department, age and post-Ph.D. experience, female economists were still significantly less likely to be promoted from assistant to associate and from associate to full professor—although there was also some evidence that women’s promotion opportunities from associate to full professor improved in the 1980s.[29]

Economist June O'Neill, former director of the Congressional Budget Office, found an unexplained pay gap of 8% after controlling for experience, education, and number of years on the job. Furthermore, O'Neil found that among young people who have never had a child, women's earnings approach 98 percent of men's.[30]

In a stance rejecting discrimination, a 2009 study by the CONSAD Research Corporation concluded, "it is not possible now, and doubtless will never be possible, to determine reliably whether any portion of the observed gender wage gap is not attributable to factors that compensate women and men differently on socially acceptable bases, and hence can confidently be attributed to overt discrimination against women." The conclusion was based largely on a study by Eric Solberg & Teresa Laughlin (1995), who found that "occupational selection is the primary determinant of the gender wage gap" (as opposed to discrimination) because "any measure of earnings that excludes fringe benefits may produce misleading results as to the existence magnitude, consequence, and source of market discrimination." They found that the average wage rate of females was only 87.4% of the average wage rate of males; whereas, when earnings were measured by their index of total compensation (including fringe benefits), the average value of the index for females was 96.4% of the average value for males.[31]

A 2010 study by Catalyst, a nonprofit that works to expand opportunities for women in business, of male and female MBA graduates found that after controlling for career aspirations, parental status, years of experience, industry, and other variables, male graduates are more likely to be assigned jobs of higher rank and responsibility and earn, on average, $4,600 more than women in their first post-MBA jobs.[32][33][34][35][36]

Sources of the gender gap

Hours worked

In the book Biology at Work: Rethinking Sexual Equality, Browne writes: "Because of the sex differences in hours worked, the hourly earnings gap [...] is a better indicator of the sexual disparity in earnings than the annual figure. Even the hourly earnings ratio does not completely capture the effects of sex differences in hours, however, because employees who work more hours also tend to earn more per hour."[37]

However, numerous studies indicate that variables such as hours worked account for only part of the gender pay gap and that the pay gap shrinks but does not disappear after controlling for all human capital variables known to affect pay.[20][38][22][25][28] Moreover, Gary Becker argues that the traditional division of labor in the family disadvantages women in the labor market as women devote substantially more time and effort to housework and have less time and effort available for performing market work.[39] The OECD (2002) found that women work fewer hours because in the present circumstances the "responsibilities for child-rearing and other unpaid household work are still unequally shared among partners."[40]

Occupational segregation

U.S. women's weekly earnings, employment, and percentage of men's earnings, by industry, 2009

Occupational segregation refers to the way that some jobs (such as truck driver) are dominated by men, and other jobs (such as child care worker) are dominated by women. Considerable research suggests that predominantly female occupations pay less, even controlling for individual and workplace characteristics.[41] Economists Blau and Kahn stated that women's pay compared to men's had improved because of a decrease in occupational segregation. They also argued that the gender wage difference will decline modestly and that the extent of discrimination against women in the labor market seems to be decreasing.[42]

In 2008, a group of researchers examined occupational segregation and its implications for the salaries assigned to male- and female-typed jobs. They investigated whether participants would assign different pay to 3 types of jobs wherein the actual responsibilities and duties carried out by men and women were the same, but the job was situated in either a traditionally masculine or traditionally feminine domain. The researchers found statistically significant pay differentials between jobs defined as "male" and "female," which suggest that gender-based discrimination, arising from occupational stereotyping and the devaluation of the work typically done by women, influences salary allocation. The results fit with contemporary theorizing about gender-based discrimination.[43][44]

A study showed that if a white woman in an all-male workplace moved to an all-female workplace, she would lose 7% of her wages. If a black woman did the same thing, she would lose 19% of her wages.[45] Another study calculated that if female-dominated jobs did not pay lower wages, women's median hourly pay nationwide would go up 13.2% (men's pay would go up 1.1%, due to raises for men working in "women's jobs").[46]

Numerous studies indicate that the pay gap shrinks but does not disappear after controlling for occupation and a host of other human capital variables.[20][38][22][25][28]

Workplace flexibility

It has been suggested that women choose less-paying occupations because they provide flexibility to better manage work and family.

A 2009 study of high school valedictorians in the U.S. found that female valedictorians were planning to have careers that had a median salary of $74,608, whereas male valedictorians were planning to have careers with a median salary of $97,734. As to why the females were less likely than the males to choose high paying careers such as surgeon and engineer, the New York Times article quoted the researcher as saying, "The typical reason is that they are worried about combining family and career one day in the future."[47]

However, Jerry A. Jacobs and Ronnie Steinberg, as well as Jennifer Glass separately, found that male-dominated jobs actually have more flexibility and autonomy than female-dominated jobs, thus allowing a person, for example, to more easily leave work to tend to a sick child.[48][49] Similarly, Heather Boushey stated that men actually have more access to workplace flexibility and that it is a "myth that women choose less-paying occupations because they provide flexibility to better manage work and family."[50]

Economists Blau and Kahn and Wood et al. separately argue that "free choice" factors, while significant, have been shown in studies to leave large portions of the gender earnings gap unexplained.[38][28]

Gender stereotypes

Research suggests that gender stereotypes may be the driving force behind occupational segregation because they influence men and women’s educational and career decisions.

Studies by Michael Conway et al., David Wagner and Joseph Berger, John Williams and Deborah Best, and Susan Fiske et al. found widely shared cultural beliefs that men are more socially valued and more competent than women at most things, as well as specific assumptions that men are better at some particular tasks (e.g., math, mechanical tasks) while women are better at others (e.g., nurturing tasks).[51][52][53][54] Shelley Correll, Michael Lovaglia, Margaret Shih et al., and Claude Steele show that these gender status beliefs affect the assessments people make of their own competence at career-relevant tasks.[55][56][57] Correll found that specific stereotypes (e.g., women have lower mathematical ability) affect women’s and men’s perceptions of their abilities (e.g., in math and science) such that men assess their own task ability higher than women performing at the same level. These "biased self-assessments" shape men and women’s educational and career decisions.[58][59]

Similarly, the OECD states that women's labour market behaviour "is influenced by learned cultural and social values that may be thought to discriminate against women (and sometimes against men) by stereotyping certain work and life styles as "male" or "female"." Further, the OECD argues that women's educational choices "may be dictated, at least in part, by their expectations that [certain] types of employment opportunities are not available to them, as well as by gender stereotypes that are prevalent in society."[40]

Direct discrimination

Economist David Neumark argued that discrimination by employers tends to steer women into lower-paying occupations and men into higher-paying occupations.[60]

Bias favoring men

Several authors suggest that members of low-status groups (e.g. women, racial minorities) are subject to negative stereotypes and attributes concerning their work-related competences.[61][62] Similarly, studies suggest that members of high-status groups (e.g., men, whites) are more likely to receive favorable evaluations about their competence, normality, and legitimacy.[63][64][65]

For instance, David R. Hekman and colleagues (2009) found that men receive significantly higher customer satisfaction scores than equally well-performing women. Hekman et al. (2009) found that customers who viewed videos featuring a female and a male actor playing the role of an employee helping a customer were 19% more satisfied with the male employee's performance and also were more satisfied with the store's cleanliness and appearance. This was despite the fact that the actors performed identically, read the same script, and were in exactly the same location with identical camera angles and lighting. Moreover, 38% of the customers were women, indicating that even women and minority raters are susceptible to systematic gender biases. In a second study, they found that male doctors were rated as more approachable and competent than equally well performing female doctors. They interpret their findings to suggest that customer ratings tend to be inconsistent with objective indicators of performance and should not be uncritically used to determine pay and promotion opportunities. They contend that in addition to addressing factors that cause bias in customer ratings, organizations should take steps to minimize the potential adverse impact of customer biases on female employees’ careers.[66][67][68][69][70]

Similarly, a study (2000) conducted by economic experts Claudia Goldin from Harvard University and Cecilia Rouse from Princeton University shows that when evaluators of applicants could see the applicant’s gender they were more likely to select men. When the applicants gender could not be observed, the number of women hired significantly increased.[71][72] David Neumark, a Professor of Economics at the University of California, Irvine, and colleagues (1996) found statistically significant evidence of sex discrimination against women in hiring. In an audit study, matched pairs of male and female pseudo-job seekers were given identical résumés and sent to apply for jobs as waiters and waitresses at the same set of restaurants. In high priced restaurants, a female applicant’s probability of getting an interview was 35 percentage points lower than a male’s and her probability of getting a job offer was 40 percentage points lower. Additional evidence suggests that customer biases in favor of men partly underlie the hiring discrimination. According to Neumark, these hiring patterns appear to have implications for sex differences in earnings, as informal survey evidence indicates that earnings are higher in high-price restaurants.[60]

Barriers in science

In 2006, the United States National Academy of Sciences found that women in science and engineering are hindered by bias and "outmoded institutional structures" in academia. The report Beyond Bias and Barriers says that extensive previous research showed a pattern of unconscious but pervasive bias, "arbitrary and subjective" evaluation processes and a work environment in which "anyone lacking the work and family support traditionally provided by a ‘wife’ is at a serious disadvantage."[73] Similarly, a 1999 report on faculty at MIT finds evidence of differential treatment of senior women and points out that it may encompass not simply differences in salary but also in space, awards, resources and responses to outside offers, "with women receiving less despite professional accomplishments equal to those of their male colleagues."[74]

Research finds that work by men is often subjectively seen as higher-quality than objectively equal or better work by women compared to how an actual scientific review panel measured scientific competence when deciding on research grants. The results showed that female scientists needed to be at least twice as accomplished as their male counterparts to receive equal credit[75] and that among grant applicants men have statistically significant greater odds of receiving grants than equally qualified women.[76]

Anti-female bias and perceived role incongruency

Research on competence judgments has shown a pervasive tendency to devalue women's work and, in particular, prejudice against women in male-dominated roles which are presumably incongruent for women.[77] Organizational research that investigates biases in perceptions of equivalent male and female competence has confirmed that women who enter high-status, male-dominated work settings often are evaluated more harshly and met with more hostility than equally qualified men.[78][79] The "think manager – think male" phenomenon[80] reflects gender stereotypes and status beliefs that associate greater status worthiness and competence with men than women.[81] Gender status beliefs shape men's and women's assertiveness, the attention and evaluation their performances receive, and the ability attributed to them on the basis of performance.[81] They also "evoke a gender-differentiated double standard for attributing performance to ability, which differentially biases the way men and women assess their own competence at tasks that are career relevant, controlling for actual ability."[82]

Alice H. Eagly and Steven J. Karau (2002) argue that "perceived incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles leads to two forms of prejudice: (a) perceiving women less favorably than men as potential occupants of leadership roles and (b) evaluating behavior that fulfills the prescriptions of a leader role less favorably when it is enacted by a woman. One consequence is that attitudes are less positive toward female than male leaders and potential leaders. Other consequences are that it is more difficult for women to become leaders and to achieve success in leadership roles."[83] Moreover, research suggests that when women are acknowledged to have been successful, they are less liked and more personally derogated than equivalently successful men.[84] Assertive women who display masculine, agentic traits are viewed as violating prescriptions of feminine niceness and are penalized for violating the status order.[85]

Maternity leave

The economic risk and resulting costs of a woman possibly leaving jobs for a period of time or indefinitely to nurse a baby is cited by many to be a reason why women are less common in the higher paying occupations such as CEO positions and upper management[citation needed]. It is much easier for a man to be hired in these higher prestige jobs than to risk losing a female job holder.[citation needed] Thomas Sowell argued in his 1984 book Civil Rights that most of pay gap is based on marital status, not a "glass ceiling" discrimination. Earnings for men and women of the same basic description (education, jobs, hours worked, marital status) were essentially equal. That result would not be predicted under explanatory theories of "sexism".[86] However, it can be seen as a symptom of the unequal contributions made by each partner to child raising. Cathy Young cites men's and fathers' rights activists who contend that women do not allow men to take on paternal and domestic responsibilities.[87] Many Western countries have some form of paternity leave to attempt to level the playing field in this regard. However, even in relatively gender-equal countries like Sweden, where parents are given 16 months of paid parental leave irrespective of gender, fathers take on average only 20% of the 16 months of paid parental and choose to transfer their days to their partner.[88][89] In addition to maternity leave, Walter Block and Walter E. Williams have argued that marriage in and of itself, not maternity leave, in general will leave females with more household labor than the males.[citation needed] The Bureau of Labor Statistics found that married women earn 75.5% as much as married men while women who have never married earn 94.2% of their unmarried male counterparts' earnings.[90]

One study estimated that 10% of the convergence of the gender gap in the 1980s and 30% in the 1990s can be accounted for by the increasing availability of contraceptives.[91]

Motherhood penalty and men's marriage premium

Several studies found a significant motherhood penalty on wages and evaluations of workplace performance and competence even after statistically controlling for education, work experience, race, whether an individual works full- or part-time, and a broad range of other human capital and occupational variables.[92][93][94] The OECD confirmed the existing literature, in which "a significant impact of children on women’s pay is generally found in the United Kingdom and the United States."[40] However, one study found a wage premium for women with very young children.[95]

Stanford University professor Shelley Correll and colleagues (2007) sent out more than 1,200 fictitious résumés to employers in a large Northeastern city, and found that female applicants with children were significantly less likely to get hired and if hired would be paid a lower salary than male applicants with children. This despite the fact that the qualification, workplace performances and other relevant characteristics of the fictitious job applicants were held constant and only their parental status varied. Mothers were penalized on a host of measures, including perceived competence and recommended starting salary. Men were not penalized for, and sometimes benefited from, being a parent. In a subsequent audit study, Correll et al. found that actual employers discriminate against mothers when making evaluations that affect hiring, promotion, and salary decisions, but not against fathers.[96][97][98][99][100][101] The researchers review results from other studies and argue that the motherhood role exists in tension with the cultural understandings of the "ideal worker" role and this leads evaluators to expect mothers to be less competent and less committed to their job.[102][103] Fathers do not experience these types of workplace disadvantages as understandings of what it means to be a good father are not seen as incompatible with understandings of what it means to be a good worker.[104]

Similarly, Fuegen et al. found that when evaluators rated fictitious applicants for an attorney position, female applicants with children were held to a higher standard than female applicants without children. Fathers were actually held to a significantly lower standard than male non-parents.[105][106] Cuddy, Fiske, and Glick show that describing a consultant as a mother leads evaluators to rate her as less competent than when she is described as not having children.[107]

Research has also shown there to be a "marriage premium" for men with labor economists frequently reporting that married men earn higher wages than unmarried men, and speculating that this may be attributable to one or more of the following causes: (1) more productive men marry at greater rates (attributing the marriage premium to selection bias), (2) men become more productive following marriage (due to labor market specialization by men and domestic specialization by women), or (3) employers favor married men. Lincoln (2008) found no support for the specialization hypothesis among full-time employed workers.[95] Some studies have suggested this premium is greater for men with children while others have shown fatherhood to have no effect on wages one way or the other.[95][108][108][109][110][111][112]

Gender differences in perceived pay entitlement

According to Serge Desmarais and James Curtis, the "gender gap in pay [...] is related to gender differences in perceptions of pay entitlement."[113] Similarly, Major et al. argue that gender differences in pay expectations play a role in perpetuating non-performance related pay differences between women and men.[114]

Perceptions of wage entitlement differ between women and men such that men are more likely to feel worthy of higher pay.[115][116][117][118][119][120][121] while women's sense of wage entitlement is depressed.[122][123] Women's beliefs about their relatively lower worth and their depressed wage entitlement reflects their lower social status such that when women's status is raised, their wage entitlement raises as well.[124][125] However, gender-related status manipulation has no impact on men's elevated wage entitlement. Even when men's status is lowered on a specific task (e.g., by telling them that women typically outperform men on this task), men do not reduce their self-pay and respond with elevated projections of their own competence.[126] The usual pattern whereby men assign themselves more pay than women for comparable work might explain why men tend to initiate negotiations more than women.[127]

In a study by psychologist Melissa Williams et al., published in 2010, study participants were given pairs of male and female first names, and asked to estimate their salaries. Men and to a lesser degree women estimated significantly higher salaries for men than women, replicating previous findings.[128][129][130] In a subsequent study, participants were placed in the role of employer and were asked to judge what newly hired men and women deserve to earn. The researchers found that men and to a lesser extent women assign higher salaries to men than women based on automatic stereotypic associations. The researchers argue that observations of men as higher earners than women has led to a stereotype that associates men (more than women) with wealth, and that this stereotype itself may serve to perpetuate the wage gap at both conscious and nonconscious levels. For example, a male-wealth stereotype may influence an employer’s initial salary offer to a male job candidate, or a female college graduate’s intuitive sense about what salary she can appropriately ask for at her first job.[131]

Negotiating salaries

A study of the job negotiations of graduating professional school students found that male students were eight times more likely to negotiate starting salaries and pay than female students.[132] A survey by the same researchers found that more than twice as many women than men said they felt "a great deal of apprehension" about "negotiating".[133][134] Some studies of simulated salary negotiations have also found that men on average negotiated more aggressively than women.[135][136] Other studies, however, have found no gender difference in pay negotiations.[137] Barry Gerhart and Sara Rymes (1991) investigated the salary negotiating behaviors and starting salary outcomes of graduating MBA students and found that women did not negotiate less than men. However, women did obtain lower monetary returns from negotiation. Over the course of a career, the accumulation of such differences may be substantial, according to the researchers.[138]

Situational factors which are assumed to influence salary negotiation include:

  • Knowledge of the competitive rate of pay for a task.[139][140]
  • Consciousness of gender stereotypes about negotiation.[141]
  • Negotiating on one's own behalf, versus negotiating anonymously or on behalf of someone else.[142]

Small et al. suggest that "framing situations as opportunities for negotiation is particularly intimidating to women, as this language is inconsistent with norms for politeness among low-power individuals, such as women". Their study of pay negotiations found that women were less likely than men to negotiate when the behavior was labeled as "negotiating" but equally likely when the behavior was labeled as "asking".[143]

Riley and Babcock found that women are penalized when they try to negotiate starting salaries. Male evaluators tended to rule against women who negotiated but were less likely to penalize men; female evaluators tended to penalize both men and women who negotiated, and preferred applicants who did not ask for more. The study also showed that women who applied for jobs were not as likely to be hired by male managers if they tried to ask for more money, while men who asked for a higher salary were not negatively affected.[144][145][146][147]

Danger wage premium

Warren Farrell has argued that a significant cause of the gender earnings gap is men's greater willingness to take on physically dangerous jobs (New York Times 2008).[citation needed]

The Bureau of Labor Statistics investigated job traits that are associated with wage premiums, and stated: "The duties most highly valued by the marketplace are generally cognitive or supervisory in nature. Job attributes relating to interpersonal relationships do not seem to affect wages, nor do the attributes of physically demanding or dangerous jobs."[148] Economists Peter Dorman and Paul Hagstrom (1998) state that "The theoretical case for wage compensation for risk is plausible but hardly certain. If workers have utility functions in which the expected likelihood and cost of occupational hazards enter as arguments, if they are fully informed of risks, if firms possess sufficient information on worker expectations and preferences (directly or through revealed preferences), if safety is costly to provide and not a public good, and if risk is fully transacted in anonymous, perfectly competitive labor markets, then workers will receive wage premia that exactly offset the disutility of assuming greater risk of injury or death. Of course, none of these assumptions applies in full and if one or more of them is sufficiently at variance with the real world, actual compensation may be less than utility-offsetting, nonexistent, or even negative – a combination of low pay and poor working conditions."[149]

Impact

Economy

An October 2012 study by the American Association of University Women found that over the course of a 35-year career, an American woman with a college degree will make about $1.2 million less than a man with the same education. Therefore, closing the pay gap by raising women's wages would have a stimulus effect that would grow the U.S. economy by at least 3% to 4%.[150] In contrast, the $800 billion economic stimulus bill passed by Congress in 2009 is estimated to have grown the GDP by less than 1.5%. Women who are paid more will likely spend that money to support themselves and their families, because so many women live in poverty. Women currently make up 70 percent of Medicaid recipients and 80 percent of welfare recipients. Increasing women's workplace participation from its present rate of 76% to 84%, as it is in Sweden, the U.S. could add 5.1 million women to the workforce, again, 3% to 4% of the size of the U.S. economy.[151]

Pensions

According to a report by the United States Congress Joint Economic Committee, the gender pay gap jeopardizes women's retirement security. Of the multiple sources of income Americans rely on later in life, many are directly linked to a worker’s earnings over his or her career. These include Social Security benefits, based on lifetime earnings, and defined benefit pension distributions that are typically calculated using a formula based on a worker’s tenure and salary during peak‐earnings years. The persistent gender pay gap leaves women with less income from these sources than men. For example, older women’s Social Security benefits are 71% of older men’s benefits ($11,057 for women versus $15,557 for men in 2009). Incomes from public and private pensions based on women’s own work were just 60% and 48% of men’s pension incomes, respectively.[152]

Current policy solutions

Lilly Ledbetter Fair Pay Act of 2009

In 2009, President Barack Obama signed the Lilly Ledbetter Fair Pay Act. This law extended the statute of limitations on cases where a worker found that they were receiving discriminatory pay, allowing them to sue and receive recompense more than six months after they received the pay. This was seen as a victory for those fighting against the gender wage gap, because if a woman at the end of her career found that she had been making less money than men who were doing the same work, she now had more than six months from the date of her last pay check to file a claim and possibly receive the wages that were denied.

See also

Legislation:

References

  1. ^ a b U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. Highlights of Women’s Earnings in 2009. Report 1025, June 2010.
  2. ^ a b c U.S. Department of Labor, U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, Women in the Labor Force: A Databook, December 2011 Report 1034 (accessed 4 July 2012)
  3. ^ a b c d United States Congress Joint Economic Committee. Invest in Women, Invest in America: A Comprehensive Review of Women in the U.S. Economy. Washington, DC, December 2010
  4. ^ a b c "Justice Talking: The Women's Equality Amendment / What Does It Mean and Is It Necessary?". 2007-05-28. Retrieved 2012-03-13.
  5. ^ DeNavas-Walt, Carmen, Bernadette D. Proctor, and Jessica C. Smith. Income, Poverty, and Health Insurance Coverage in the United States: 2009. U.S. Census Bureau, Current Population Reports, P60-238, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, DC, 2010, p. 7, 50.
  6. ^ a b U.S. Census Bureau. Income, Earnings, and Poverty Data From the 2007 American Community Survey. August 2008, p. 14.
  7. ^ Dougherty, Conor. Young Women's Pay Exceeds Male Peers. The Walls Street Journal, September 1, 2010.
  8. ^ a b Bureau of Labor Statistics. Women's earnings and employment by industry, 2009. Chart data, February 16, 2011.
  9. ^ Bureau of Labor Statistics. Women's earnings and employment by industry, 2009. TED article, February 16, 2011.
  10. ^ Ariane Hegewisch, Claudia Williams, and Amber Henderson. The Gender Wage Gap by Occupation. Institute for Women's Policy Research, April 2011.
  11. ^ 39 jobs where women make more than men, CNN, February 28, 2006
  12. ^ Bloomberg. Women CEOs Earn More Than Men, Get Pay Raise in 2009. Retrieved on September 7, 2010.
  13. ^ http://www.attorneyretention.org/Publications/SameGlassCeiling.pdf New Millenium: Same Glass Ceiling? The Impact of Law Firm Compensation Systems on Women, by the Project for Attorney Retention, Hastings College of Law, July, 2010; Peterson, Trond (Sept 1995). "Separate and Unequal: Occupation-Establishment Sex Segregation and the Gender Wage Gap". American Journal of Sociology. 101 (2): 329–365. Retrieved April 4, 2012. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  14. ^ Bureau of Labor Statistics. Highlights of Women’s Earnings in 2008. US Labor Department, Report 1017, July 2009, p. 9.
  15. ^ Catalyst. Women's Earnings and Income. April 2011.
  16. ^ Roberts, Sam. For Young Earners in Big City, a Gap in Women’s Favor. The New York Times, August 3, 2007.
  17. ^ Eagly, A.H., & Carli, L. L. Through the labyrinth: The truth about how women become leaders. Boston, Mass.: Harvard Business School Press, 2007, ISBN 978-1-4221-1691-3.
  18. ^ U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee. Graph: Federal Workforce - Gender Pay Gap Unchanged. December 1, 2007.
  19. ^ Blau, F. D., & Kahn, J. (2007). The gender pay gap. The Economists' Voice, Vol. 4, Iss. 4, pp. 1–6, doi:10.2202/1553-3832.1190.
  20. ^ a b c Altonji, Joseph G. and Rebecca M. Blank (1999). Race and Gender in the Labor Market, in Orley Ashenfelter and David Card (eds.), Handbook of Labor Economics, Vol. 3, Elsevier Science B.V., 1999, ISBN 978-0-444-82289-5.
  21. ^ GAO. Women's Earnings: Federal Agencies Should Better Monitor Their Performance in Enforcing Anti-Discrimination Laws. GAO-08-799, August 11, 2008.
  22. ^ a b c About.com. Why Women Still Make Less than Men. Retrieved on July 23, 2011.
  23. ^ Folbre, Nancy. Happy Equal Pay Day. New York Times, April 28, 2009.
  24. ^ Boraas, S., & Rodgers, W. M., III. (2003). How does gender play a role in the earnings gap? An update. Monthly Labor Review, Vol. 126, No. 3, pp. 9–15.
  25. ^ a b c Carman, Diane. Why do men earn more? Just because. Denver Post, April 24, 2007.
  26. ^ Arnst, Cathy. Women and the pay gap. Bloomberg Businessweek, April 27, 2007.
  27. ^ American Management Association. Bridging the Gender Pay Gap. October 17, 2007.
  28. ^ a b c d Blau, Francine D. & Lawrence M. Kahn (1997). Swimming Upstream: Trends in the Gender Wage Differential in 1980s. Journal of Labor Economics, Vol. 15, No. 1, pp. 1–42.
  29. ^ John M. McDowell, Larry D. Singell and James P. Ziliak (1999). Cracks in the Glass Ceiling: Gender and Promotion in the Economics Profession. American Economic Review, Vol. 89, Iss. 2, pp. 392–96.
  30. ^ June E. O'Neill & Dave M. O'Neill (2005). What Do Wage Differentials Tell Us about Labor Market Discrimination? NBER Working Paper No. 11240.
  31. ^ "Gender Wage Gap Final Report, 2009
  32. ^ Stark, Betsy. The Myth of the Pipeline: Inequality Still Plagues Working Women, Study Finds. ABC News, February 18, 2010.
  33. ^ Wolgemuth, Liz. Why Some Women Skirt the Wage Gap. U.S. News, May 14, 2010.
  34. ^ Ludden, Jennifer. Despite New Law, Gender Salary Gap Persists. National Public Radio, April 19, 2010.
  35. ^ Lavelle, Louis. Catalyst: Women MBAs Lag Behind Men in Jobs, Pay, Promotions. Bloomberg Businessweek, March 3, 2010.
  36. ^ Carter, Nancy M. & Christine Silver (2010). Pipeline's broken promise. Catalyst.
  37. ^ Browne, Kingsley R. (2002). Biology at Work: Rethinking Sexual Equality. Rutgers University Press. pp. 73–74. ISBN 0-8135-3053-9, 9780813530536. {{cite book}}: Check |isbn= value: invalid character (help)
  38. ^ a b c Wood, Robert G., Mary E. Corcoran, and Paul Courant. 1993. Pay Differences Among the Highly Paid: the Male-Female Earnings Gap in Lawyers’ Salaries. Journal of Labor Economics, Vol. 11, No. 3, pp. 417–41.
  39. ^ Becker, Gary S. (1985). Human capital, effort, and the sexual division of labor. Journal of Labor Economics, Vol. 3, No. 1, pp. S33–58.
  40. ^ a b c OECD (2002). Emplyoment Outlook, Chapter 2: Women at work: who are they and how are they faring? Paris: OECD 2002.
  41. ^ Blau, Francine D. & Lawrence M. Kahn (2000). Gender differences in pay. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, Vol. 14, No. 4, pp. 75–99.
  42. ^ Cicarelli, James and Julianne Cicarelli. Distinguished women economists. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 2003, pp. 36–40, ISBN 978-0-313-30331-9.
  43. ^ Alksnis, C., Desmarais, S., & Curtis, J. (2008). Workforce segregation and the gender wage gap: Is "women’s" work valued as highly as "men’s"? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 38, pp. 1416–1441, doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00354.x.
  44. ^ Vedantam, Shankar. The Wage Gap – Unconscious Bias in Judging the Value of Predominantly "Female" Professions. Psychology Today, February 18, 2010.
  45. ^ England, Paula, Lori L. Reid and Barbara S. Kilbourne (1996). The Effect of the Sex Composition of Jobs on Starting Wages in an Organization: Findings from the NLSY. Demography, Vol. 33, No. 4, pp. 511–21.
  46. ^ Figart, Deborah and June Lapidus (1996). The Impact of Comparable Worth on Earnings Inequality. Work and Occupations, Vol. 23, No. 3, pp. 297–318.
  47. ^ Do the Ambitions of High School Valedictorians Differ by Gender?, New York Times, June 1, 2009
  48. ^ Glass, Jennifer (1990). The Impact of Occupational Segregation on Working Conditions. Social Forces, 68:779–96.
  49. ^ Jacobs, J. A., & Steinberg, R. (1990). Compensating differentials and the male-female wage gap: Evidence from the New York state comparable worth study. Social Forces, 69, 439–68.
  50. ^ Center for Economic and Policy Research. Strengthening the Middle Class: Ensuring Equal Pay for Women. Testimony of Heather Boushey. Retrieved on July 23, 2011.
  51. ^ Conway, Michael, M. Teresa Pizzamiglio and Lauren Mount (1996). Status, Communality and Agency: Implications for Stereotypes of Gender and Other Groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 25–38.
  52. ^ Wagner, David G. and Joseph Berger (1997). Gender and Interpersonal Task Behaviors: Status Expectation Accounts. Sociological Perspectives, 40: 1–32.
  53. ^ Williams, John E. and Deborah L. Best (1990). Measuring Sex Stereotypes: A Multinational Study. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  54. ^ Fiske, Susan T., Amy J. C. Cuddy, Peter Glick, Jun Xu (2002). A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 82, No. 6, pp. 878–902.
  55. ^ Lovaglia, Michael J., Jeffrey W. Lucas, Jeffrey A. Houser, Shane R. Thye, and Barry Markovsky (1998). Status Processes and Mental Ability Test Scores. American Journal of Sociology, 104: 195–228.
  56. ^ Shih, Margaret, Todd L. Pittinsky and Nalini Ambady (1999). Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity, Salience and Shifts in Quantitative Performance. Psychological Science, 10: 80–3.
  57. ^ Steele, Claude M. 1997. A Threat Is in the Air: How Stereotypes Shape Intellectual Identity and Performance. American Psychologist, 52: 613–29.
  58. ^ Shelley J. Correll (2001). Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessments. American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 106, Issue 6, pp. 1691–1730.
  59. ^ Shelley J. Correll (2004). Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations. American Sociological Review, Vol 69, Issue 1, pp. 93–113.
  60. ^ a b Neumark, David, Roy J. Bank and Kyle D. Van Nort (1996). Sex discrimination in restaurant hiring: An audit study. Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 111, No. 3, pp. 915–41.
  61. ^ Fernandez, John P. Racism and sexism in corporate life. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 1981, ISBN 978-0-669-04477-5.
  62. ^ O’Leary, Virginia E., & Ickovics, Jeanette R. Cracking the glass ceiling: overcoming isolation and alienation. In U. Sekaran, & F. T. L. Leong (Eds.), Womanpower: Managing in times of demographic turbulence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1992, pp. 7–30, ISBN 978-0-8039-4106-9.
  63. ^ Aquino, Karl & William H. Bommer (2003). Preferential mistreatment: How victim status moderates the relationship between organizational citizenship behavior and workplace victimization. Organization Science, Vol. 14, No. 4, pp. 374–85.
  64. ^ Giannopoulos, Constantina, Michael Conway & Morris Mendelson (2005). The gender of status: The laypersons' perception of status groups is gender-typed. Sex Roles, Vol. 53, No. 11-12, pp. 795–806, doi:10.1007/s11199-005-8293-3.
  65. ^ Sidanius, Jim & Felicia Pratto. Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1999, ISBN 978-0-521-62290-5.
  66. ^ Bakalar, Nicholas. A Customer Bias in Favor of White Men. New York Times. June 23, 2009, page D6.
  67. ^ Vedantam, Shankar. Caveat for Employers. Washington Post, June 1, 2009, page A8.
  68. ^ Jackson, Derrick. Subtle, and stubborn, race bias. Boston Globe, July 6, 2009, page A10.
  69. ^ National Public Radio, Lake Effect
  70. ^ Hekman, David R., Karl Aquino, Brad P. Owens, Terence R. Mitchell, Pauline Schilpzand, Keith Leavitt (2010). An Examination of Whether and How Racial and Gender Biases Influence Customer Satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 53, No. 2, pp. 238–64.
  71. ^ Weiner, Joann M. No, It's Not Your Imagination; We're Biased Against Women. Politics Daily, Retrieved on July 13, 2011.
  72. ^ Goldin, Claudia, & Cecilia Rouse (2000). Orchestrating Impartiality: the Impact of Blind Auditions on Female Musicians. American Economic Review, Vol. 90, No. 4, pp. 715–42.
  73. ^ Dean, Cornelia. Bias Is Hurting Women in Science, Panel Reports. The New York Times, September 19, 2006.
  74. ^ A Study on the Status of Women Faculty in Science at MIT. The MIT Faculty Newsletter, Vol. XI, No. 4, March 1999.
  75. ^ Wenneras, Christine and Agnes Wold (1997). "Nepotism and Sexism in Peer-Review." Nature, Vol. 387, pp. 341–43.
  76. ^ Bormann, Lutz, Ruediger Mutz, Hand-Dieter Daniel (2007). Gender differences in grant peer review: A meta-analysis. Journal of Infometrics, Vol. I, Issue 3, pp. 226–38, doi:10.1016/j.joi.2007.03.001.
  77. ^ Eagly, Alice H., Mona G. Makhijani, and Bruce G. Klonsky (1992). Gender and the Evaluation of Leaders: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 111, Issue 1, pp. 3–22.
  78. ^ Collinson, David, David Knights, and Margaret Collinson. Managing to discriminate. London; New York: Routledge, 1990, ISBN 978-0-415-01817-3.
  79. ^ Heilman, Madeline E. (2001.) Description and Prescription: How Gender Stereotypes Prevent Women's Ascent Up the Organizational Ladder. Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 57, No. 4, pp. 657–74.
  80. ^ Schein, Virginia E. (2001). A Global Look at Psychological Barriers to Women's Progress in Management. Journal of Social Issues, Vol, 57, No. 4, pp. 675–88.
  81. ^ a b Ridgeway, Cecilia L. (2001). Gender, Status, and Leadership. Journal of Social Issues, Vol, 57, No. 4, pp. 637–55.
  82. ^ Correll, Shelley J. (2004). Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations. American Sociological Review, Vol. 69, No. 1, pp. 93–113.
  83. ^ Eagly, Alice H. & Steven J. Karau (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders.Psychological Review, Vol 109, No. 3, pp. 573–98, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573.
  84. ^ Heilman, Madeline E., Aaron S. Wallen, Daniella Fuchs, and Melinda M. Tamkins (2004). Penalties for Success: Reactions to Women Who Succeed at Male Gender-Typed Tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 89, No. 3, pp. 416–27.
  85. ^ Rudman, Laurie A. & Peter Glick (2001). Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes and Backlash Towards Agentic Women. Journal of Social Issues, Vol, 57, No. 4, pp. 743–62.
  86. ^ Sowell, Thomas, "Civil Rights: Rhetoric or Reality", 1984 (see Chapter 5, "The Special Case of Women") and "Markets and Minorities", 1981.
  87. ^ The mama lion at the gate – Salon.com
  88. ^ http://www.thelocal.se/10420/20080312/
  89. ^ http://www.framtidsstudier.se/filebank/files/20051201$134956$fil$U8YIJLRAaC7u4FV7gUmy.pdf
  90. ^ Wolgemuth, Liz. Young Women Closing in on Gender Wage Parity. July 31, 2009.
  91. ^ Bailey, Martha J.; Hershbein, Brad; Miller, Amalia R. (March 2012). "The opt-in revolution? Contraception and the gender gap in wages". Working paper 17922. National Bureau of Economic Research.
  92. ^ Budig, Michelle J. & Paula England. (2001). The wage penalty for motherhood. American Sociological Review, Vol. 66, No. 2, pp. 204-225.
  93. ^ Anderson, Deborah J, Melissa Binder, Kate Krause (2003).The Motherhood Wage Penalty Revisited: Experience, Heterogeneity, Work Effort, and Work-Schedule Flexibility. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, Vol. 56, No. 2, pp. 273–94.
  94. ^ Avellar, Sarah, and Smock, Pamely J. (2003). Has the price of motherhood declined over time? A cross-cohort comparison of the motherhood wage penalty. Journal of Marriage and the Family, Vol. 65, pp. 597–607.
  95. ^ a b c Lincoln, Anne E. (2008). Gender, Productivity, and the Marital Wage Premium. Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 70, pp. 806–14.
  96. ^ Folbre, Nancy. The Anti-Mommy Bias. New York Times, March 26, 2009.
  97. ^ Goodman, Ellen. A third gender in the workplace. Boston Globe, May 11, 2007.
  98. ^ Cahn, Naomi and June Carbone. Five myths about working mothers. The Washington Post, May 30, 2010.
  99. ^ Young, Lauren. The Motherhood Penalty: Working Moms Face Pay Gap Vs. Childless Peers. Bloomsberg Businessweek, June 5, 2009.
  100. ^ Correll, Shelley, Stephen Benard, In Paik (2007.) Getting a job: Is there a motherhood penalty? American Journal of Sociology, Vol 112, No. 5, pp. 1297–1338, doi:10.1086/511799.
  101. ^ News.cornell.edu. Mothers face disadvantages in getting hired. August 4, 2005.
  102. ^ Blair-Loy, Mary. Competing devotions: Career and family among women executives. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2003, ISBN 978-0-674-01089-5.
  103. ^ Ridgeway, Cecilia L., Shelley J. Correll (2004). Unpacking the gender system: A Theoretical Perspective on Gender Beliefs and Social Relations. Gender & Society, Vol. 18, No. 4, pp. 510–31, doi:10.1177/0891243204265269.
  104. ^ Townsend, Nicholas W. The package deal: Marriage, work, and fatherhood in men's lives. Philadelphia: Temple University Press, 2002, ISBN 978-1-56639-957-9.
  105. ^ Fuegen, Kathleen, Monica Biernat, Elizabeth Haines, and Kay Deaux (2004). Mothers and Fathers in the Workplace: How Gender and Parental Status Influence Judgments of Job-Related Competence. Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 60, Iss. 4, pp. 737–54, doi:10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00383.x.
  106. ^ Research News. Mothers held to stricter standards, study suggests. Retrieved on July 24, 2011.
  107. ^ Cuddy, Amy J. C., Susan T. Fiske1, Peter Glick (2004). When Professionals Become Mothers, Warmth Doesn't Cut the Ice. Vol. 60, Iss. 4, pp. 701–18, doi:10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00381.x.
  108. ^ a b Hersch, Joni & Leslie Sundt Stratton (2000). Household specialization and the male marriage wage premium. Industrial & Labor Relations Review, Vol. 54, Iss. 1, pp. 78–94.
  109. ^ Loh, Eng Seng (1996). Productivity and the Marriage Premium for White Males. Journal of Human Resources, Vol. 31, pp. 566–89.
  110. ^ Korenman, Sanders, and David Neumark (1991). Does Marriage Really Make Men More Productive? Journal of Human Resources, Vol. 26, pp. 282–307.
  111. ^ Hill, Martha (1979). The Wage Effects of Marital Status and Children. Journal of Human Resources, Vol. 14, pp. 579–94.
  112. ^ Orloff, Ann (1996.) Gender and the Welfare State. Annual Review of Sociology, Vol. 22, pp. 51–78.
  113. ^ Desmarais, S., & Curtis, J. (1997b). Gender differences in pay histories and views on pay entitlement among university students. Sex Roles, 37, 623–42.
  114. ^ Major, B., V. Vanderslice, and D. B. McFarlin. 1984. Effects of pay expected on pay received: The confirmatory nature of initial expectations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 14(5): 399–412.
  115. ^ Pelham, B. W., & Hetts, J. J. (2001). Underworked and overpaid: Elevated entitlement in men’s self-pay. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 93–103.
  116. ^ Kaman, V. S., & Hartel, C. E. J. (1994). Gender differences in anticipated pay negotiation strategies and outcomes. Journal of Business and Psychology, 9, 183–97.
  117. ^ Callahan-Levy, C. M., & Messé, L. A. (1979). Sex differences in the allocation of pay. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 433–46.
  118. ^ Jackson, L. A. (1989). Relative deprivation and the gender wage gap. Journal of Social Issues, 45, 117–33.
  119. ^ Jackson, L. A., Gardner, P. D., & Sullivan, L. A. (1992). Explaining gender differences in self-pay expectations: Social comparison standards and perceptions of fair pay. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 651–63.
  120. ^ Jost, J. T. (1997). An experimental replication of the depressed entitlement effect among women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 387–93.
  121. ^ Moore, D. (1994). Entitlement as an epistemic problem: Do women think like men? Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 9, 665–84.
  122. ^ Major, B. (1994). From social inequality to personal entitlement: The role of social comparisons, legitimacy appraisals, and group membership. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 26, pp. 293–348). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  123. ^ Major, B., McFarlin, D. B., & Gagnon, D. (1984). Overworked and underpaid: On the nature of gender differences in personal entitlement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1399–1412.
  124. ^ Major, Brenda (1994). From Social Inequality to Personal Entitlement: The Role of Social Comparisons, legitimacy Appraisals, and Group Memership. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 29, pp. 293–355.
  125. ^ Hogue, M. and J.D. Yoder (2003). "The Role of Status in Producing Depressed Entitlement in Women’s and Men’s Pay Allocations." Psychology of Women Quarterly (27) 4: 330–37.
  126. ^ Hogue, M., J.D. Yoder, et al. (2007). "The Gender Wage Gap: An Explanation of Men’s Elevated Wage Entitlement." Sex Roles 56 (9-10): 581–90.
  127. ^ Barron, L. (2003). Ask and you shall receive? Gender differences in beliefs about requests for a higher salary. Human Relations, 56, 635–63.
  128. ^ Biernat, M., Manis, M., & Nelson, T. E. (1991). Stereotypes and standards of judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 485–99.
  129. ^ Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188.
  130. ^ Morrison, T. G., Bell, E. M., Morrison, M. A., & Murray, C. A. (1994). An examination of adolescents’ salary expectations and gender-based occupational stereotyping. Youth & Society, 26, 178–93.
  131. ^ Williams, Melissa J., Elizabeth Levy Paluck, & Julia Spencer-Rogers (2010.) The Masculinity of Money: Automatic Stereotypes Predict Gender Differences in Estimated Salaries. Psychology of Women Quarterly, Vol. 34, pp. 7–20
  132. ^ Babcock, Linda. Do graduate students negotiate their job offers? Unpublished report, 2003. Cited in Babcock and Laschever, 2003. Women Don't Ask.
  133. ^ Babcock, Linda; Laschever, Sara (2003). Women Don't Ask: Negotiation and the Gender Divide. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-08940-9.
  134. ^ Womendontask.com. Women Don't Ask: Negotiation and the Gender Divide. Retrieved July 30, 2011.
  135. ^ Stevens, Cynthia K. (1993). "Gender differences in the acquisition of salary negotiation skills: The role of goals, self-efficacy, and perceived control". Journal of Applied Psychology. 78 (5): 723–35. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.5.723. PMID 8253630. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  136. ^ Kaman, Vicki S. (1994). "Gender differences in anticipated pay negotiation strategies and outcomes". Journal of Business and Psychology. 9 (2): 183–97. doi:10.1007/BF02230636. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  137. ^ Riemer, Cynthia (1982). "The success rate of personal salary negotiations: A further investigation of academic pay differentials by sex". Research in Higher Education. 16 (2): 139–54. doi:10.1007/BF00973506. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  138. ^ Gerhart, Barry (1991). "Determinants and consequences of salary negotiations by male and female MBA graduates". Journal of Applied Psychology. 76 (2): 256–62. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.76.2.256. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  139. ^ Major, Brenda (1984). "Overworked and underpaid: On the nature of gender differences in personal entitlement". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 47 (6): 1399–1412. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1399. PMID 6527220. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  140. ^ Bylsma, Wayne H. (1992). "Two routes to eliminating gender differences in personal entitlement: Social comparisons and performance evaluations". Psychology of Women Quarterly. 16 (2): 193–200. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1992.tb00249.x. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  141. ^ Kray, Lara J. (2001). "Battle of the sexes: Stereotype confirmation and reactance in negotiations". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (6): 942–58. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.942. PMID 11414376. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  142. ^ Wade, 2002. Audience and advocacy: When gender norms become salient during salary requests. Unpublished manuscript. Cited in Babcock and Laschever, 2003. Women Don't Ask.
  143. ^ Small, Deborah A. (2007). "Who goes to the bargaining table? The influence of gender and framing on the initiation of negotiation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (4): 600–13. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.600. PMID 17892334. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  144. ^ Shankar, Vedantam. Salary, Gender and the Social Cost of Haggling. The Washington Post, July 30, 2007.
  145. ^ Clark-Flory, Tracy. The costs of asking for a higher salary. Salon, July 30, 2007.
  146. ^ Montell, Gabriela. Damned if They Do. The Chronicle of Higher Education, July 31, 2007.
  147. ^ Bowles, Hannah Riley, Linda Babcock, Lei Lai (2007). Social incentives for gender differences in the propensity to initiate negotiations: Sometimes it does hurt to ask. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 103, pp. 84–103.
  148. ^ Bureau of Labor Statistics. Knowledge gets the biggest pay premium. TED article, October 5, 1999.
  149. ^ Dorman P, Hagstrom P. (1998). Wage Compensation for Dangerous Work Revisited. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, Vol. 52, pp. 116–35.
  150. ^ Christianne Corbett and Catherine Hill (October 2012) "Graduating to a Pay Gap: The Earnings of Women and Men One Year after College Graduation" (Washington, DC: American Association of University Women)
  151. ^ Laura Bassett (October 24, 2012) "Closing The Gender Wage Gap Would Create 'Huge' Economic Stimulus, Economists Say" Huffington Post
  152. ^ U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee. The Gender Wage Gap Jeopardizes Women’s Retirement Security. April 12, 2011.