Jump to content

Insular cortex

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Rjwilmsi (talk | contribs) at 13:52, 20 May 2012 (Journal cites (journal names):, using AWB (8060)). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Insular cortex
The insula of the left side, exposed by
removing the opercula.
Coronal section of brain immediately in
front of pons. (Insula labeled at upper right.)
Details
ArteryMiddle cerebral
Identifiers
LatinCortex insularis
MeSHD000087623
NeuroNames111
NeuroLex IDbirnlex_1117
TA98A14.1.09.149
A12.2.07.053
TA25502
FMA67329
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy

In each hemisphere of the mammalian brain the insular cortex (often called insula, insulary cortex or insular lobe) is a portion of the cerebral cortex folded deep within the lateral sulcus between the temporal lobe and the frontal lobe.

The insulae are believed to be involved in consciousness and play a role in diverse functions usually linked to emotion or the regulation of the body's homeostasis. These functions include perception, motor control, self-awareness, cognitive functioning, and interpersonal experience. In relation to these it is involved in psychopathology.

The insular cortex is divided into two parts: the larger anterior insula and the smaller posterior insula in which more than a dozen field areas have been identified. The cortical area overlying the insula towards the lateral surface of the brain is the operculum (meaning "lid"). The opercula are formed from parts of the enclosing frontal, temporal and parietal lobes.

The insula was first described by Johann Christian Reil while describing cranial and spinal nerves and plexi.[1] Henry Gray in Gray's Anatomy is responsible for it being known as the Island of Reil.[1]

Functions

Interoceptive awareness

The right anterior insula aids interoceptive awareness of body states, such as the ability to time one's own heart beat. Moreover, greater right anterior insular gray matter volume correlates with increased accuracy in this subjective sense of the inner body, and with negative emotional experience.[2] It is also involved in the control of blood pressure,[3] particularly during and after exercise.[3] It is also activated when the brain perceives greater exertion.[4][5]

The insular cortex also is where the sensation of pain is judged as to its degree.[6] Further, the insula is where a person imagines pain when looking at images of painful events while thinking about them happening to one's own body.[7] Those with irritable bowel syndrome have abnormal processing of visceral pain in the insular cortex related to dysfunctional inhibition of pain within the brain.[8]

Another perception of the right anterior insula is the degree of nonpainful warmth[9] or nonpainful coldness[10] of a skin sensation. Other internal sensations processed by the insula include stomach or gastric distension.[11][12] A full bladder also activates the insular cortex.[13]

One brain imaging study suggest that the unpleasantness of subjectively perceived dyspnea is processed in the right human anterior insula and amygdala.[14]

The cerebral cortex processing vestibular sensations extends into the insula[15] with small lesions in the anterior insular cortex being able to cause loss of balance and vertigo.[16]

Other noninteroceptive perceptions include passive listening to music,[17] laughter and crying,[18] empathy and compassion,[19] and language.[20]

Motor control

In motor control it contributes to hand and eye motor movement,[21][22] swallowing,[23] gastric motility,[24] and speech articulation.[25][26] It has been identified as a "central command” centre that ensures that heart rate and blood pressure increase at the onset of exercise.[27] Research upon conversation links it to the capacity for long and complex spoken sentences.[28] It is also involved in motor learning[29] and has been identified as playing a role in the motor recovery from stroke.[30]

Homeostasis

In homeostasis it controls autonomic functions through the regulation of the sympathetic and parasympathetic systems.[31][32] It has a role in regulating the immune system.[33][34][35]

Self

It has been identified as playing a role in the experience of bodily self-awareness,[36][37] sense of agency[38] and sense body ownership.[39]

Social emotions

The anterior insular processes a person's sense of disgust both to smells[40] and to the sight of contamination and mutilation[41] — even when just imagining the experience.[42] This associates with a mirror neuron like link between external and internal experience.

In social experience it is involved in the processing of norm violations,[43] emotional processing,[44] empathy,[45] and orgasms.[46]

Emotions

The insular cortex, in particular its most anterior portion, is considered a limbic-related cortex. The insula has increasingly become the focus of attention for its role in body representation and subjective emotional experience. In particular, Antonio Damasio has proposed that this region plays a role in mapping visceral states that are associated with emotional experience, giving rise to conscious feelings. This is in essence a neurobiological formulation of the ideas of William James, who first proposed that subjective emotional experience (i.e. feelings) arise from our brain's interpretation of bodily states that are elicited by emotional events. This is an example of embodied cognition.

Functionally speaking, the insula is believed to process convergent information to produce an emotionally relevant context for sensory experience. More specifically, the anterior insula is related more to olfactory, gustatory, vicero-autonomic, and limbic function, while the posterior insula is related more to auditory-somesthetic-skeletomotor function. Functional imaging experiments have revealed that the insula has an important role in pain experience and the experience of a number of basic emotions, including anger, fear, disgust, happiness and sadness.

Functional imaging studies have also implicated the insula in conscious desires, such as food craving and drug craving. What is common to all of these emotional states is that they each change the body in some way and are associated with highly salient subjective qualities. The insula is well situated for the integration of information relating to bodily states into higher-order cognitive and emotional processes. The insula receives information from "homeostatic afferent" sensory pathways via the thalamus and sends output to a number of other limbic-related structures, such as the amygdala, the ventral striatum and the orbitofrontal cortex, as well as to motor cortices.[47]

A study using magnetic resonance imaging found that the right anterior insula was significantly thicker in people who meditate.[48]

Another study using voxel-based morphometry and MRI on experienced Vipassana meditators was done to extend the findings of Lazar et al., which found increased grey matter concentrations in this and other areas of the brain in experienced meditators.[49]

Salience

Functional imaging research suggests the insula is involved in two types of salience. Interoceptive information processing that links interoception with emotional salience to generate a subjective representation of the body. This involves the anterior insular cortex with the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann area 33) and the anterior and posterior mid-cingulate cortices. Second, a general salience system concerned with environmental monitoring, response selection, and skeletomotor body orientation that involves all of the insular cortex and the mid-cingulate cortex.[50]

An alternative or perhaps complementary proposal is that right anterior insular regulates the interaction between the salience of the selective attention created to achieve a task (the dorsal attention system) and the salience of arousal created to keep focused upon the relevant part of the environment (ventral attention system).[51] This regulation of salience might be particularly important during challenging tasks where attention might fatigue and so cause careless mistakes but if there is too much arousal it risks creating poor performance by turning into anxiety.[51]

Neuroanatomy

Connections

The anterior insula receives a direct projection from the basal part of the ventral medial nucleus (VMb) of the thalamus and a particularly large input from the central nucleus of the amygdala. Additionally, the anterior insula itself projects to the amygdala.

One study on rhesus monkeys revealed widespread reciprocal connections between the insular cortex and almost all subnuclei of the amygdaloid complex. The posterior insula projects predominantly to the dorsal aspect of the lateral and to the central amygdaloid nuclei. In contrast, the anterior insula projects to the anterior amygdaloid area as well as the medial, the cortical, the accessory basal magnocellular, the medial basal and the lateral amygdaloid nuclei.[52]

The posterior insula connects reciprocally with the secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) and receives input from spinothalamically activated ventral posterior inferior (VPI) thalamic nuclei. More recent work by Bud Craig and his colleagues has shown that this region receives inputs from the ventromedial nucleus (posterior part) of the thalamus that are highly specialized to convey emotional/homeostatic information such as pain, temperature, itch, local oxygen status and sensual touch.

A human neuroimaging study using diffusion tensor imaging revealed that the anterior insula is interconnected to regions in the temporal and occipital lobe, opercular and orbitofrontal cortex, triangular and opercular parts of the inferior frontal gyrus. The same study revealed differences in the anatomical connection patterns between the left and right hemisphere.[53]

Cytoarchitecture

The insular cortex has regions of variable cell structure or cytoarchitecture, changing from granular in the posterior portion to agranular in the anterior portion. The insula also receives differential cortical and thalamic input along its length.

Origins

The insular cortex is considered a separate lobe of the telencephalon by some authorities.[54] Other sources see the insula as a part of the temporal lobe.[55] It is also sometimes grouped with limbic structures deep in the brain into a limbic lobe.[citation needed]

As a paralimbic cortex, the insular cortex is considered to be a relatively old structure. It plays a role in a variety of highly conserved functions that are related to basic survival needs, such as taste, visceral sensation and autonomic control (so-called homeostatic functions). There is evidence that in addition to its more conserved functions, the insula may play a role in certain "higher" functions that operate only in humans and other great apes. John Allman and his colleagues have shown that the anterior insular cortex contains a population of neurons, called spindle neurons. These neurons are also found in the anterior cingulate cortex, which is another region that has reached a high level of specialization in great apes. Spindle neurons are found at a higher density in the right insular cortex. It has been speculated that these neurons are involved in cognitive-emotional processes that are specific to great apes, such as empathy and self-aware emotional feelings. This is supported by functional imaging results showing that the structure and function of the right anterior insula are correlated with the ability to feel one's own heartbeat, or to empathize with the pain of others. It is thought that these functions are not distinct from the "lower" functions of the insula, but rather arise as a consequence of the role of the insula in conveying homeostatic information to consciousness.[56][57]

Clinical

Progressive non-fluent aphasia

Progressive non-fluent aphasia is the deterioration of normal language function which causes individuals to lose the ability to communicate fluently while being still being able to comprehend single words and intact other non-linguistic cognition. It is found in a variety of degenerative neurological conditions including Pick's disease, motor neuron disease, corticobasal degeneration, frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. It is associated with hypometabolism[58] and atrophy of the left anterior insular cortex.[59]

Addiction

A number of functional brain imaging studies have shown that the insular cortex is activated when drug abusers are exposed to environmental cues that trigger cravings. This has been shown for a variety of drugs, including cocaine, alcohol, opiates and nicotine. Despite these findings, the insula has been ignored within the drug addiction literature, perhaps because it is not known to be a direct target of the mesotelencephalic dopamine system which is central to current dopamine reward theories of addiction. Recent research[60] has shown that cigarette smokers who suffer damage to the insular cortex, from a stroke for instance, have their addiction to cigarettes practically eliminated. However, the study was conducted on average eight years after the strokes, and the author admits recall bias could affect the results[61]

These individuals were found to be up to 136 times more likely to undergo a disruption of smoking addiction than smokers with damage in other areas. Disruption of addiction was evidenced by self-reported behavior changes such as quitting smoking less than one day after the brain injury, quitting smoking with great ease, not smoking again after quitting, and having no urge to resume smoking since quitting. This suggests a significant role for the insular cortex in the neurological mechanisms underlying addiction to nicotine and other drugs, and would make this area of the brain a possible target for novel anti-addiction medication. In addition, this finding suggests that functions mediated by the insula, especially conscious feelings, may be particularly important for maintaining drug addiction, although this view is not represented in any modern research or reviews of the subject.[62]

A recent study in rats by Contreras et al.[63] corroborates these findings by showing that reversible inactivation of the insula disrupts amphetamine conditioned place preference, an animal model of cue-induced drug craving. In this study, insula inactivation also disrupted "malaise" responses to lithium chloride injection, suggesting that the representation of negative interoceptive states by the insula plays a role in addiction. However, in this same study, the conditioned place preference took place immediately after the injection of amphetamine, suggesting that it was the immediate, pleasurable interoceptive effects of amphetamine administration, rather than the delayed, aversive effects of amphetamine withdrawal that are represented within the insula.

A model proposed by Naqvi et al. (see above) is that the insula stores a representation of the pleasurable interoceptive effects of drug use (e.g. the airway sensory effects of nicotine, the cardiovascular effects of amphetamine), and that this representation is activated by exposure to cues that have previously been associated with drug use. A number of functional imaging studies have shown the insula to be activated during the administration of drugs of abuse. Several functional imaging studies have also shown that the insula is activated when drug users are exposed to drug cues, and that this activity is correlated with subjective urges. In the cue-exposure studies, insula activity is elicited when there is no actual change in the level of drug in the body. Therefore, rather than merely representing the interoceptive effects of drug use as it occurs, the insula may play a role in memory for the pleasurable interoceptive effects of past drug use, anticipation of these effects in the future, or both. Such a representation may give rise to conscious urges that feel as if they arise from within the body. This may make addicts feel as if their bodies need to use a drug, and may result in persons with lesions in the insula reporting that their bodies have forgotten the urge to use, according to this study.

Other clinical conditions

The insular cortex has been suggested to have a role in anxiety disorders,[64] and emotion dysregulation.[65]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Binder DK, Schaller K, Clusmann H (2007). "The seminal contributions of Johann-Christian Reil to anatomy, physiology, and psychiatry". Neurosurgery. 61 (5): 1091–6, discussion 1096. doi:10.1227/01.neu.0000303205.15489.23. PMID 18091285. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ (2004). "Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness". Nat. Neurosci. 7 (2): 189–95. doi:10.1038/nn1176. PMID 14730305. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  3. ^ a b Lamb K, Gallagher K, McColl R, Mathews D, Querry R, Williamson JW (2007). "Exercise-induced decrease in insular cortex rCBF during postexercise hypotension". Med Sci Sports Exerc. 39 (4): 672–9. doi:10.1249/mss.0b013e31802f04e0. PMID 17414805. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. ^ Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, Mitchell JH, Raven PB, Morgan WP (2001). "Hypnotic manipulation of effort sense during dynamic exercise: cardiovascular responses and brain activation". J. Appl. Physiol. 90 (4): 1392–9. PMID 11247939. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  5. ^ Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, Ginsburg M, Mitchell JH (1999). "Activation of the insular cortex is affected by the intensity of exercise". J. Appl. Physiol. 87 (3): 1213–9. PMID 10484598. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ Baliki MN, Geha PY, Apkarian AV (2009). "Parsing pain perception between nociceptive representation and magnitude estimation". J. Neurophysiol. 101 (2): 875–87. doi:10.1152/jn.91100.2008. PMID 19073802. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ Ogino Y, Nemoto H, Inui K, Saito S, Kakigi R, Goto F (2007). "Inner experience of pain: imagination of pain while viewing images showing painful events forms subjective pain representation in human brain". Cereb. Cortex. 17 (5): 1139–46. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl023. PMID 16855007. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Song GH, Venkatraman V, Ho KY, Chee MW, Yeoh KG, Wilder-Smith CH (2006). "Cortical effects of anticipation and endogenous modulation of visceral pain assessed by functional brain MRI in irritable bowel syndrome patients and healthy controls". Pain. 126 (1–3): 79–90. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.06.017. PMID 16846694. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  9. ^ Olausson H, Charron J, Marchand S, Villemure C, Strigo IA, Bushnell MC (2005). "Feelings of warmth correlate with neural activity in right anterior insular cortex". Neurosci. Lett. 389 (1): 1–5. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.065. PMID 16051437. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  10. ^ Craig AD, Chen K, Bandy D, Reiman EM (2000). "Thermosensory activation of insular cortex". Nat. Neurosci. 3 (2): 184–90. doi:10.1038/72131. PMID 10649575. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ Ladabaum U, Minoshima S, Hasler WL, Cross D, Chey WD, Owyang C (2001). "Gastric distention correlates with activation of multiple cortical and subcortical regions". Gastroenterology. 120 (2): 369–76. doi:10.1053/gast.2001.21201. PMID 11159877. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ Hamaguchi T, Kano M, Rikimaru H; et al. (2004). "Brain activity during distention of the descending colon in humans". Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 16 (3): 299–309. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2982.2004.00498.x. PMID 15198652. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  13. ^ Matsuura S, Kakizaki H, Mitsui T, Shiga T, Tamaki N, Koyanagi T (2002). "Human brain region response to distention or cold stimulation of the bladder: a positron emission tomography study". J. Urol. 168 (5): 2035–9. doi:10.1097/01.ju.0000027600.26331.11. PMID 12394703. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  14. ^ von Leupoldt, A. (24 January 2008). "The Unpleasantness of Perceived Dyspnea Is Processed in the Anterior Insula and Amygdala". American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine. 177 (9): 1026–1032. doi:10.1164/rccm.200712-1821OC. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  15. ^ Kikuchi M, Naito Y, Senda M; et al. (2009). "Cortical activation during optokinetic stimulation — an fMRI study". Acta Otolaryngol. 129 (4): 440–3. doi:10.1080/00016480802610226. PMID 19116795. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |unused_data= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ Papathanasiou ES, Papacostas SS, Charalambous M, Eracleous E, Thodi C, Pantzaris M (2006). "Vertigo and imbalance caused by a small lesion in the anterior insula". Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol. 46 (3): 185–92. PMID 16918202.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ Brown S, Martinez MJ, Parsons LM (2004). "Passive music listening spontaneously engages limbic and paralimbic systems". NeuroReport. 15 (13): 2033–7. doi:10.1097/00001756-200409150-00008. PMID 15486477. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  18. ^ Sander K, Scheich H (2005). "Left auditory cortex and amygdala, but right insula dominance for human laughing and crying". J Cogn Neurosci. 17 (10): 1519–31. doi:10.1162/089892905774597227. PMID 16269094. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  19. ^ http://ccare.stanford.edu/node/89
  20. ^ Bamiou DE, Musiek FE, Luxon LM (2003). "The insula (Island of Reil) and its role in auditory processing. Literature review". Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42 (2): 143–54. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00172-3. PMID 12738055. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ Anderson TJ, Jenkins IH, Brooks DJ, Hawken MB, Frackowiak RS, Kennard C (1994). "Cortical control of saccades and fixation in man. A PET study". Brain. 117 (Pt 5): 1073–84. doi:10.1093/brain/117.5.1073. PMID 7953589. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  22. ^ Fink GR, Frackowiak RS, Pietrzyk U, Passingham RE (1997). "Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex". J. Neurophysiol. 77 (4): 2164–74. PMID 9114263. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Sörös P, Inamoto Y, Martin RE (2009). "Functional brain imaging of swallowing: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis". Hum Brain Mapp. 30 (8): 2426–39. doi:10.1002/hbm.20680. PMID 19107749. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Penfield W, Faulk ME (1955). "The insula; further observations on its function". Brain. 78 (4): 445–70. PMID 13293263.
  25. ^ Dronkers NF (1996). "A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation". Nature. 384 (6605): 159–61. doi:10.1038/384159a0. PMID 8906789. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  26. ^ Ackermann H, Riecker A (2004). "The contribution of the insula to motor aspects of speech production: a review and a hypothesis". Brain Lang. 89 (2): 320–8. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00347-X. PMID 15068914. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  27. ^ Nowak M, Holm S, Biering-Sørensen F, Secher NH, Friberg L (2005). ""Central command" and insular activation during attempted foot lifting in paraplegic humans". Hum Brain Mapp. 25 (2): 259–65. doi:10.1002/hbm.20097. PMID 15849712. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  28. ^ Borovsky A, Saygin AP, Bates E, Dronkers N (2007). "Lesion correlates of conversational speech production deficits". Neuropsychologia. 45 (11): 2525–33. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.023. PMID 17499317. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  29. ^ Mutschler I, Schulze-Bonhage A, Glauche V, Demandt E, Speck O, Ball T (2007). Fitch, Tecumseh (ed.). "A rapid sound-action association effect in human insular cortex". PLoS ONE. 2 (2): e259. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000259. PMC 1800344. PMID 17327919.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  30. ^ Weiller C, Ramsay SC, Wise RJ, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS (1993). "Individual patterns of functional reorganization in the human cerebral cortex after capsular infarction". Ann. Neurol. 33 (2): 181–9. doi:10.1002/ana.410330208. PMID 8434880. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  31. ^ Oppenheimer SM, Gelb A, Girvin JP, Hachinski VC (1992). "Cardiovascular effects of human insular cortex stimulation". Neurology. 42 (9): 1727–32. PMID 1513461. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  32. ^ Critchley HD (2005). "Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration". J. Comp. Neurol. 493 (1): 154–66. doi:10.1002/cne.20749. PMID 16254997. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  33. ^ Pacheco-López G, Niemi MB, Kou W, Härting M, Fandrey J, Schedlowski M (2005). "Neural substrates for behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression in the rat". J. Neurosci. 25 (9): 2330–7. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4230-04.2005. PMID 15745959. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  34. ^ Ramírez-Amaya V, Alvarez-Borda B, Ormsby CE, Martínez RD, Pérez-Montfort R, Bermúdez-Rattoni F (1996). "Insular cortex lesions impair the acquisition of conditioned immunosuppression". Brain Behav. Immun. 10 (2): 103–14. doi:10.1006/brbi.1996.0011. PMID 8811934. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  35. ^ Ramírez-Amaya V, Bermúdez-Rattoni F (1999). "Conditioned enhancement of antibody production is disrupted by insular cortex and amygdala but not hippocampal lesions". Brain Behav. Immun. 13 (1): 46–60. doi:10.1006/brbi.1998.0547. PMID 10371677. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  36. ^ Karnath HO, Baier B, Nägele T (2005). "Awareness of the functioning of one's own limbs mediated by the insular cortex?". J. Neurosci. 25 (31): 7134–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-05.2005. PMID 16079395. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  37. ^ Craig AD (2009). "How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 (1): 59–70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555. PMID 19096369. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  38. ^ Farrer C, Frith CD (2002). "Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action: the neural correlates of the experience of agency". Neuroimage. 15 (3): 596–603. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1009. PMID 11848702. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  39. ^ Tsakiris M, Hesse MD, Boy C, Haggard P, Fink GR (2007). "Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness". Cereb. Cortex. 17 (10): 2235–44. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl131. PMID 17138596. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  40. ^ Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2003). "Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–64. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  41. ^ Wright P, He G, Shapira NA, Goodman WK, Liu Y (2004). "Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination". NeuroReport. 15 (15): 2347–51. doi:10.1097/00001756-200410250-00009. PMID 15640753. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  42. ^ Jabbi M, Bastiaansen J, Keysers C (2008). Lauwereyns, Jan (ed.). "A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways". PLoS ONE. 3 (8): e2939. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002939. PMC 2491556. PMID 18698355.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  43. ^ Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (2003). "The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game". Science. 300 (5626): 1755–8. doi:10.1126/science.1082976. PMID 12805551. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  44. ^ Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I (2002). "Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI". Neuroimage. 16 (2): 331–48. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. PMID 12030820. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  45. ^ Singer T (2006). "The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of literature and implications for future research". Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 30 (6): 855–63. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.011. PMID 16904182.
  46. ^ Ortigue S, Grafton ST, Bianchi-Demicheli F (2007). "Correlation between insula activation and self-reported quality of orgasm in women". Neuroimage. 37 (2): 551–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.026. PMID 17601749. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  47. ^ Craig, A. D. (Bud) (2002). "A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion" (PDF). Trends in Neuroscience. 26 (6): 303–307.
  48. ^ Sara W. Lazar, Catherine E. Kerr, Rachel H. Wasserman, Jeremy R. Gray, Douglas N. Greve, Michael T. Treadway, Metta McGarvey, Brian T. Quinn, Jeffery A. Dusek, Herbert Benson, Scott L. Rauch, Christopher I. Moore, and Bruce Fischl (2005). "Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness". NeuroReport. 16 (17): 1893–7. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. PMC 1361002. PMID 16272874. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |month= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  49. ^ http://scan.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/3/1/55
  50. ^ Taylor KS, Seminowicz DA, Davis KD (2009). "Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingulate cortex". Hum Brain Mapp. 30 (9): 2731–45. doi:10.1002/hbm.20705. PMID 19072897. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  51. ^ a b Eckert MA, Menon V, Walczak A, Ahlstrom J, Denslow S, Horwitz A, Dubno JR. (2009). At the heart of the ventral attention system: the right anterior insula. Hum Brain Mapp. 30(8):2530-41. doi:10.1002/hbm.20688 PMID 19072895
  52. ^ MUFSON, E (1 July 1981). "Insular interconnections with the amygdala in the rhesus monkey" (PDF). Neuroscience. 6 (7): 1231–1248. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(81)90184-6. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  53. ^ JAKAB, A (1 oct2011). "Connectivity-based parcellation reveals interhemispheric differences in the insula". Brain Topography. doi:10.1007/s10548-011-0205-y. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  54. ^ Brain, MSN Encarta. Archived 2009-10-31.
  55. ^ Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2003). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology (5th ed.). [New York]: Worth. ISBN 0-7167-5300-6.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  56. ^ Benedetto De Martino, Dharshan Kumaran, Ben Seymour, and Raymond J. Dolan (2006). "Frames, Biases, and Rational Decision-Making in the Human Brain". Science. 313: 684–687. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |Issue= ignored (|issue= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  57. ^ Gui Xue, Zhonglin Lu, Irwin P. Levin d, Antoine Bechara (2010). "The impact of prior risk experiences on subsequent risky decision-making: The role of the insula". NeuroImage. 50: 709–716.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  58. ^ Nestor PJ, Graham NL, Fryer TD, Williams GB, Patterson K, Hodges JR (2003). "Progressive non-fluent aphasia is associated with hypometabolism centred on the left anterior insula". Brain. 126 (Pt 11): 2406–18. doi:10.1093/brain/awg240. PMID 12902311. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  59. ^ Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP; et al. (2004). "Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia". Ann. Neurol. 55 (3): 335–46. doi:10.1002/ana.10825. PMC 2362399. PMID 14991811. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  60. ^ Nasir H. Naqvi, David Rudrauf, Hanna Damasio, Antoine Bechara. (2007). "Damage to the Insula Disrupts Addiction to Cigarette Smoking" (abstract). Science. 315 (5811): 531–4. doi:10.1126/science.1135926. PMID 17255515. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  61. ^ Vorel SR, Bisaga A, McKhann G, Kleber HD (2007). "Insula damage and quitting smoking". Science. 317 (5836): 318–9, author reply 318–9. doi:10.1126/science.317.5836.318c. PMID 17641181. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  62. ^ Hyman, Steven E. (2005-08-01). "Addiction: A Disease of Learning and Memory" (abstract). Am J Psychiatry. 162 (8): 1414–22. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1414. PMID 16055762.
  63. ^ Marco Contreras, Francisco Ceric, Fernando Torrealba (2007). "Inactivation of the Interoceptive Insula Disrupts Drug Craving and Malaise Induced by Lithium" (abstract). Science. 318 (5850): 655–8. doi:10.1126/science.1145590. PMID 17962567. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  64. ^ Paulus MP, Stein MB (2006). "An insular view of anxiety". Biol. Psychiatry. 60 (4): 383–7. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042. PMID 16780813. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  65. ^ Thayer JF, Lane RD (2000). "A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation". J Affect Disord. 61 (3): 201–16. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4. PMID 11163422. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)