Jump to content

Magical thinking

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 208.54.14.110 (talk) at 02:14, 30 May 2009 (External links). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Magical thinking and rationality

Magical thinking in anthropology, psychology, and cognitive science is nonscientific causal reasoning that often includes such ideas as the ability of the mind to affect the physical world (see the philosophical problem of mental causation), and correlation mistaken for causation. Associative thinking may be brought into play, as well as the power of magical symbols, metaphor and metonym, and synchronicity. Since, in both theory and practice, magic does not conform to Western and modern canons of causality it is therefore appropriate to ask if it is rational to practice or believe in magic. For most theorists these questions turn on the matter of the practitioner’s thought processes, intentions, and the efficacy of their practice.

Associative thinking

Prominent Victorian theorists typically regarded practitioners of magic as irrational because they felt magic was based on incorrect notions of causality, called "associative thinking." Edward Burnett Tylor coined this term, characterizing it as pre-logical,[1] in which the "magician's folly" is in mistaking an ideal connection with a real one. The magician believes that thematically-linked items can influence one another by virtue of their similarity.[2] For example, in E. E. Evans-Pritchard’s account, amongst the Azande[3] one rubs crocodile teeth on banana plants to make them fruitful, because crocodile teeth are curved (like bananas) and grow back if they fall out. The Azande observe this similarity and want to impart this capacity of regeneration to their bananas. To them, the rubbing constitutes a means of transference.

Sir James Frazer later elaborated upon this principle by dividing magic into the categories of “contagious” and “sympathetic” magic. The former is based upon the law of contagion or contact, in which two things that were once connected retain this link and have the ability to affect their 'related' objects, such as harming a person by harming a lock of his hair. Sympathetic magic operates upon the premise that "like affects like," or that one can impart characteristics of one similar object to another. Frazer believed that ‘primitive peoples’ think the entire world functions according to these mimetic or 'homeopathic' principles.[4]

In How Natives Think, Lucien Lévy-Bruhl describes a similar notion of mystical, "collective representations." He, too, claims that magical thinking is fundamentally different from Westerner's. He asserts that in these representations, 'primitive' people's "mental activity is too little differentiated for it to be possible to consider ideas or images of objects by themselves apart from the emotions and passions which evoke those ideas or are evoked by them."[5] Lévy-Bruhl explains that natives commit the post hoc, ergo propter hoc logical fallacy, in which people observe that x is followed by y, therefore y has been caused by x.[6] He believes that this fallacy is institutionalized in native culture and is committed regularly and repeatedly.

These theories of associative thinking assert that magic is irrational. However, they are contested because they are based upon the premise that native people's notions of causality are inferior. This outlook has sparked much disagreement and has generated alternative theories of magical thinking, such as the symbolic and psychological approaches.

Other forms of magical thinking

Bronislaw Malinowski discusses another type of magical thinking is which words and sounds are thought to have the ability to directly affect the world.[7] This type of thinking can result in the avoidance of talking about certain subjects ("speak of the devil and he'll appear"), the use of euphemisms instead of certain words, or the belief that to know the "true name" of something gives one power over it, or that certain chants, prayers, or mystical phrases will bring about physical changes in the world. More generally, it is magical thinking to take a symbol to be its referent or an analogy to represent an identity.

Sigmund Freud believed that magical thinking was produced by cognitive developmental factors. He described practitioners of magic as projecting their mental states onto the world around them, similar to a common phase in child development.[8] From toddlerhood to early school age, children will often link the outside world with their internal consciousness, e.g. "It is raining because I am sad."

Symbolic approach to magic

Another theory of magical thinking is the symbolic approach. Leading thinkers of this category, including Stanley J. Tambiah, believe that magic is meant to be expressive, rather than instrumental. As opposed to the direct, mimetic thinking of Frazer, Tambiah asserts that magic utilizes abstract analogies to express a desired state, along the lines of metonymy or metaphor.[9]

An important question raised by this interpretation is how mere symbols could have material effects. One possible answer lies in John L. Austin’s concept of “performativity,” in which the act of saying something makes it true, such as in an inaugural or marital rite.[10] Other theories propose that magic is effective because symbols are able to change internal psycho-physical states. They claim that the act of expressing a certain anxiety or desire is reparative in itself.[11]

These theories can seem limited in that they do not account for the various explicitly instrumental magical practices. Many magical cultures have produced extensive rationalizations for magic’s potential lack of efficacy; this indicates that magical routines are invoked to bring about physical results. As stated by Gilbert Lewis, “if magicians performed spells for explicitly symbolic or metaphorical purposes, then we wouldn’t consider them magic at all…” [12]

Psychological functions of magic

Some scholars believe that magic is effective psychologically (see Psychological Theories of Magic). They cite the placebo effect, psychosomatic disease, etc, as prime examples of how our mental functions exert power over our bodies.[13] Similarly, Robert Horton suggests that engaging in magical practices surrounding healing can relieve anxiety, which could have a significant positive physical impact. In the absence of advanced health care, such impacts could play a relatively major role, thereby helping to explain the persistence and popularity of such practices. [14]

These theories explain that many magical practices are effective, but they do not explain why inanimate objects would be affected, such as in the Azande case of the bananas. Neither do they explain why practices involving very few of these psychological elements would be repeatedly used in a culture, if ineffective. The question of rationality remains. The effectiveness of magical practices may be psychological, but if this not be the practitioner’s intent, the practice of magic may still be considered pre-logical.

Phenomenological approach

Ariel Glucklich tries to understand magic from a subjective perspective, attempting to comprehend magic on a phenomenological, experientially-based level. Glucklich seeks to describe the attitude that magical practitioners feel which he calls "magical consciousness" or the "magical experience." He explains that it is based upon “the awareness of the interrelatedness of all things in the world by means of simple but refined sense perception,”[15] Within this mindset of interrelatedness, magic is curative in the restoration of man’s equilibrium with nature.

Another phenomenological model is Gilbert Lewis’s, which is that "habit is unthinking." He believes that those practicing magic do not think of an explanatory theory behind their actions any more than the average person tries to grasp the pharmaceutical workings of aspirin.[16] When the average person takes an aspirin, he does not know how the medicine chemically functions. He takes the pill with the premise that there is proof of efficacy. Similarly, many who avail themselves of magic do so without feeling the need to understand a causal theory behind it. Indeed, most people who use magic do so as a practical tool rather than as a philosophical question.

Idiomatic difference

Robert Horton maintains that the difference between Western and non-western people’s thinking is predominantly "idiomatic." He asserts that both cultures employ the same practical common-sense, and both science and magic are ways in which people delve into theory, into what occurs beyond that basic logic. When modern farmers desire, for example, to ensure plant growth, they would turn to scientific theory and might purchase fertilizer or consult a gardener. However, non-western cultures possess the idiom of magic or spiritual figures and would therefore turn to magical practices or an expert in that idiom. Horton sees both western and native cultures as possessing the same logic and common-sense, but simply different ontological idioms, which may contribute to seemingly illogical practices on either part. He explains, "the layman’s grounds for accepting the models propounded by the scientist is often no different from the young African villager's ground for accepting the models propounded by one of his elders."[17]

Along similar lines, Michael Brown argues that the Aguaruna of Peru, see magic as merely a type of technology, no more supernatural than their physical tools. He argues that the Aguaruna utilize magic in a very scientific manner, for example, discarding magical stones which they have observed to be ineffective. It seems, that to Brown, too, magical thinking merely differs in idiom.[18]

These theories tend to blur the lines between magic, science, and religion, asserting the similarities between magical, technical, and spiritual practices. Brown even notes that he is tempted to claim that ‘magic’ does not exist.[19] But if one maintains that magic is its own distinct category, then one might ask what makes magical thinking different from our own.

Substantive difference

One theory of substantive difference is that of the open versus closed society. Horton describes this as one of the key dissimilarities between traditional thought and Western science. He suggests that the scientific worldview is distinguished from a magical one by the scientific method and by skepticism, requiring the falsifiability of any scientific hypothesis. He notes that for native peoples “there is no developed awareness of alternatives to the established body of theoretical texts.”[20] He notes that all further differences between traditional and Western thought can be understood as a result of this factor, for example, the fact that African thought both lacks impersonal theory, or objectivity, and clings to the past as opposed to looking towards the future. Because there are no alternatives in magically-thought based societies, a theory does not need to be objectively judged to be valid, and each moment that passes draws them father away from a once undiluted relationship with the spiritual and natural world.

However, Richard Feynman suggested, in his "Cargo Cult Science" speech, that even scientists may fall prey to a form of magical thinking. When experiments are poorly controlled and not repeated, or a reporting bias dominates, scientists may "fool themselves" into believing that insignificant results are significant. If enough flawed work is done in a field — Feynman singles out psychology in particular as sloppy — then further experiments may devolve into a set of unfounded rituals.[21] In short, methods that are scientific may be used to generate results that merely seem scientific.

Further reading

  • Serban, George. The Tyranny of Magical Thinking. E. P. Dutton Inc., New York 1982. ISBN 0-525-24140-X This work discusses how and why the magical thinking of childhood can carry into adulthood, causing various maladaptions and psychopathologies.
  • Dukes, Ramsey. "SSOTBME revised, an essay on magic". TMTS, London 2002. ISBN 978-0904311082. Argues for the survival and psychological benefits of magical thinking, and that it is often better seen as post-scientific rather than pre-scientific – as in complex software where bugs are increasingly addressed via work-arounds rather than analysis.

References

  1. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), pp 32-33.
  2. ^ Evans-Pritchard, E.E. Theories of Primitive Religion. Oxford University Press (1977), p.p. 26-27.
  3. ^ Evans-Pritchard, E.E. Witchcraft, Magic, and Oracles Among the Azande, Oxford University Press (1976).
  4. ^ Frazer, James, The Golden Bough: A Study in Magic and Religion (1911-1915) Third edition. London: Macmillan.
  5. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien, How Natives Think, (1925), Knopf Publishing, p36.
  6. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien, How Natives Think, (1925), Knopf Publishing, p76.
  7. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), pp 59-61, 205-212.
  8. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), pp 53-55.
  9. ^ Brown, Michael F., Thinking About Magic, Greenwood Press, (1993) pp 5-7.
  10. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), pp 60-62.
  11. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), pp 49-53.
  12. ^ Brown F., Michael, Thinking About Magic, Greenwood Press, (1993) p8.
  13. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), pp 50-68.
  14. ^ Horton, Robert, “African Thought and Western Science,” Rationality, pp 137-140
  15. ^ Glucklich, Ariel, The End of Magic, Oxford University Press (1997), p12.
  16. ^ Lewis, Gilbert, The Look of Magic, University of Cambridge.
  17. ^ Horton, Robert, African Thought and Western Science, Rationality, p171.
  18. ^ Brown F., Michael, Tsewa’s Gift: Magic and Meaning in an Amazonian Society, The University of Alabama Press (1986).
  19. ^ Brown F., Michael, Thinking About Magic, Greenwood Press, (1993) p2.
  20. ^ Horton, Robert, African Thought and Western Science, Rationality, p153
  21. ^ Feynman, Richard (1974). "Cargo Cult Science". Engineering and Science 37:7. http://calteches.library.caltech.edu/51/02/CargoCult.pdf.