Animal song
Animal song is not a well-defined term in scientific literature, and the use of the more broadly defined term 'vocalizations' is in more common use[1]. Song generally consists of repetitive vocal sounds incorporating multiple syllables. Some sources distinguish between simpler vocalizations, termed “calls”, reserving the term “song” for more complex productions[2]. Song-like productions have been identified in several groups of animals, including cetaceans (whales and dolphins), avians (birds), anurans (frogs), and humans. Social transmission of song has been found in groups including birds and cetaceans.
Anatomy of Sound Production
Mammals
Most mammalian species produce sound by passing air from the lungs across the larynx, vibrating the vocal folds[3]. Sound then enters the supralaryngeal vocal tract, which can be adjusted to produce various changes in sound output, providing refinement of vocalizations[3]. Although morphological differences between species affect sound production, neural control is thought to be more essential factor in producing the variations within human speech and song compared to those of other mammals[3].
Cetacean vocalizations are an exception to this general mechanism. Toothed whales (Odontocetes) pass air through a system of air sacs and muscular phonic lips, which vibrate to produce audible vocalizations, thus serving the function of vocal folds in other mammals[4]. Sound vibrations are conveyed to an organ in the head called the melon, which can be changed in shape to control and direct vocalizations[5]. Unlike in humans and other mammals, toothed whales are able to recycle air used in vocal production, allowing whales to sing without releasing air[4]. Some cetaceans, such as humpback whales, sing continuously for hours[6].
Anurans
Like mammals, anurans possess a larynx and vocal folds, which are used to create vibrations in sound production[3]. However, frogs also use structures called vocal sacs, elastic membranes in the base of the mouth which inflate during sound production[7]. These sacs provide both amplification and fine-tuning of sounds, and also allow air to be pushed back into the lungs during vocalizations[4]. This allows air used in sound production to be recycled, and is thought to have evolved to increase song efficiency[7]. Increased efficiency of sound production is important, as some frogs may produce calls lasting for several hours during mating seasons[7]. The New River tree frog (Trachycephalus hadroceps), for example, spends hours producing up to 38,000 calls in a single night, which is made possible through the efficient recycling of air by the vocal sac[7].
Birds
When birds inhale, air is passed from the mouth, through the trachea, which forks into two bronchii connecting to the lungs[8]. The primary vocal organ of birds is called the syrinx, which is located at the fork of the trachea, and is not present in mammals[9]. As air passes through the respiratory tract, the syrinx and the membranes within vibrate to produce sound[10]. Birds are capable of producing continuous song during both inhalation and exhalation, and may sing continuously for several minutes[11]. For example, the skylark (Alauda arvensis) is capable of producing non-stop song for up to one hour[12]. Some birds change their song characteristics during inhalation versus exhalation. The Brewer’s sparrow (Spizella breweri) alternates between rapid trilling during exhalation interspersed with lower-rate trills during short inhalations[13]. The two halves of the syrinx connect to separate lungs, and can be controlled independently, allowing some birds to produce two separate notes simultaneously[9].
Insects
Insects such as crickets (family Gryllidae) are well-known for their ability to produce loud song, however the mechanism of sound production differs greatly from most other animals. Many insects generate sound by mechanical rubbing of body structures, a mechanism known as stridulation[14]. Orthopteran insects, including crickets and katydids (family Tettigoniidae), have been especially well-studied for sound production. These insects use scraper-like structures on one wing to sweep over file-structures on an opposing wing to create vibrations, producing a variety of trilling and chirping sounds[15][16]. Locusts and other grasshoppers (suborder Caelifera) stridulate by rubbing hind legs against pegs on wing surfaces in a up and downward motion[17]. Cicadas (superfamily Cicadoidea) produce sound at much greater volumes than Orthopterans, relying on a pair of organs called tymbals on the base of the abdomen behind the wings[15]. Muscle contraction rapidly deforms the tymbal membrane, emitting several different types of sounds[15]. Insects thus produce a variety of sounds, using various mechanisms distinct from other animals.
Functions of Vocalizations
Vocalizations can play a wide variety of different roles. In groups such as anurans and birds, several distinct types of notes are incorporated to form songs, which are sung in different situations and serve distinct functions[18]. For example, many frogs may use trilling notes in mate attraction, but switch to different vocal patterns in aggressive territorial displays[18]. In some species, a single song incorporates several note types which serve different purposes, with one type of note eliciting responses from females, and another note of the same song responsible for warning competitor males of aggression[18].
Mating and courtship
Vocalizations play an important role in the mating behaviour of many animals. In many groups (birds, frogs, crickets, whales etc.), song production is more common in males of the species, and is often used to attract females[18][19][20].
Bird song is thought to have evolved through sexual selection. Female songbirds often assess potential mates using song, based on qualities such as high song output, complexity and difficulty of songs, as well as presence of local dialect[21]. Song output serves as a fitness indicator of males, since vocalizations require both energy and time to produce, and thus males capable of producing high song output for long durations may have higher fitness than less vocal males[21]. It is thought that song complexity may serve as an indicator of male fitness by providing an indication of successful brain development despite potential early-life stressors, such as lack of food[21]. Social transmission of songs allows for development of local dialects of song, and female songbirds also typically prefer to choose mates producing local song dialects[21]. One hypothesis for this phenomenon is that selecting local mates allows the female to choose genes specially adapted to suit local conditions[21].
Frog song also plays a prominent role in courtship. In túngara frogs (Engystomops pustulosus), male frogs increase the complexity of their calls, adding additional note types when greater numbers of competitor males are present, which has been found to attract greater numbers of female frogs[22]. Some species change their courtship calls when females are especially nearby. In male glass frogs (Hyalinobatachium fleichmanni), a long frequency-modulated vocalization is produced upon noticing another nearby frog, but is changed to a short chirping song when a female approaches[18]. Several species (e.g. dendrobatid frogs (Mannophryne trinitatis), ornate frogs (Cophixalus ornatus), splendid poison frogs (Dendrobates speciosus)), switch from long-range loud trilling sounds to short-range quieter chirps when females move closer, which is thought to allow mate attraction without alerting competitor males to female locations[18].
Although highly complex song-like production has been identified in whales, the function is still somewhat elusive. It is thought to be involved in courtship behaviour and sexual selection, and singing behaviour becomes more common during the breeding season[23].
Aggression and territorial defense
Another major function of song output is to indicate aggression among males during breeding seasons. Both anurans and birds use singing in territorial displays to confer aggressive intent[18][21]. For Eastern smooth frogs (Geocrinia victoriana), for example, courtship songs involve shorter notes to attract potential mates, and are followed by longer tones to repel males[18]. Frequency of sounds produced generally negatively correlates with body size both within and among species, and allows competing males to assess body size of vocalizing neighbouring frogs[24]. Male frogs typically approach higher frequency sounds more readily than lower frequencies, likely because the frog producing the sound is assessed to be a smaller, less dangerous competitor[24].
In territorial birds, males increase song production rate when neighbouring males encroach on their territory[21]. In great tits (Parus major), nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos), blackbirds (Turdus merula) and sparrows (family Passeridae), playing song recordings slows the rate at which males establish territories in an unoccupied region, suggesting these birds rely on song output in establishing territorial boundaries[21]. Experimentally muted Scott’s seaside sparrow (Ammodramus maritimus) lose control of their territories to other males[21]. Thus, territorial birds often rely on song production to repel conspecific males.
Individual recognition
Like the human voice, bird song typically contains sufficient individual variability to allow discrimination of individual vocal patterns by conspecifics[25]. Such discrimination is important to mate recognition of many monogamous species. Seabirds, for example, often use vocalization patterns to recognize their mate upon reunion during the breeding season[26]. In many colonial nesting birds, parent-offspring recognition is critical to allow parents to locate their own offspring upon return to nesting sites[27]. Cliff swallows (Petrochelidon pyrrhonota) have been demonstrated to preferentially respond to parental songs at a young age, providing a means of vocalization-based offspring recognition[27].
Social Transmission and Learning
Learned vocalizations have been identified in groups including whales, elephants, seals, and primates, however the most well-established examples of learned singing is in birds[28]. In many species, young birds learn songs from adult males of the same species, typically fathers[29]. This was first demonstrated in chaffinches (Fringilla coelabs). Chaffinches raised in social isolation develop abnormal songs, however playing recordings of chaffinch songs allows the young birds to learn their species-specific songs[30]. Song learning generally involves a sensitive learning period in early life, during which young birds must be exposed to song from tutor animals in order to develop normal singing as adults[31]. Song learning occurs in two stages: the sensory phase and the sensorimotor phase. During the sensory phase, birds memorize the song of a tutor animal, forming a template representation of the species-specific song[31]. The sensorimotor phase follows and may overlap with the sensory phase. During the sensorimotor phase, young birds initially produce variable, rambling versions of adult song, called subsong[31]. As learning progresses, the subsong is replaced with a more refined version containing elements of adult song, called plastic song. Finally, the song learning crystallizes into adult song[32]. For song learning to occur properly, young birds must be able to hear and refine their vocal productions, and birds deafened before the development of subsong do not learn to produce normal adult song[33].
The sensitive period in which birds must be exposed to song tutoring varies across species, but typically occurs within the first year of life[31]. Birds in which song learning is limited to the initial sensitive period are referred to as closed-ended learners, whereas some birds (e.g. canaries; Serinus canaria), continue to learn new songs later in life, and are called open-ended learners[31]. Some species of birds, such as the brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater), parasitize other bird species, laying their eggs in the nests of other birds such that the heterospecific bird raises the chicks[34]. Although most birds acquire song learning within the first year, brown-headed cowbirds have a delayed sensitive period, occurring approximately one year after hatching[34]. This may be an adaptation to prevent the young birds from learning the songs from the foreign bird species. Instead, the young birds have a year in which to find conspecifics, and learn their own species-specific song[34].
Birds are generally predisposed to favour learning of conspecific songs, and will typically preferentially learn the song form conspecific animals rather than heterospecifics[30]. However, song learning is not completely restricted to within-species songs. If exposed to heterospecific birds of another species in absence of same-species birds, young birds will often adopt the song of the species to which it was exposed[30]. Although birds are capable of learning song production purely from audio recordings of birdsong, tutor-student interaction may be important in some species. For example, white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) preferentially learn the songs of song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) when exposed to recordings of white-crowned sparrows and live song sparrows[35]. In other words, the interactive nature of a live tutor seems to trump the familiarity of the recordings from conspecifics.
While vertical transmission (parent-offspring) is a common element of song learning, horizontal transmission among animals of the same generation can also occur[36]. Male humpback whales produce various songs over their lifetime, which are learned from other males in the population. Males in a population conform to produce the same mating song, consisting of a highly stereotyped vocal display involved in mate attraction[20]. The cultural transmission of these songs has been found to occur across great geographic distances over years, with one study noting song transmission across the western and central South Pacific ocean populations over an 11-year period[20].
See also
- Animal communication
- Animal language
- Bird song
- Vocal learning
- Whale song
- Animals United In Song https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=kqa6vp0m9KU
External links
- Listen to Nature 400 examples of animal songs and calls
- Washington U. Mice Songs
- Cornell Animal Sound Library (over 300,000 audio recordings from various species of mammals, birds, amphibians, fish, arthropods and reptiles).
- The British Library Sound Archive has more than 150,000 recordings of 10,000 species
- Canadian Centre for Wolf Research
- International Bioacoustics Council many links to animal sound sites
- ^ Sfetcu, Nicolae (2014). The Music Sound. ISBN 1230000233979.
{{cite book}}
: Check|isbn=
value: invalid prefix (help) - ^ Arbib, Michael (2013). Language, Music, and the Brain: A Mysterious Relationship. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-01810-4.
- ^ a b c d Fitch, T (2006). "Production of Vocalizations in Mammals". Elsevier: 115–121. doi:10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/00821-X.
- ^ a b c Ladich, Friedrich; Winkler, Hans (2017). "Acoustic communication in terrestrial and aquatic vertebrates". Journal of Experimental Biology. 220: 2306–2317. doi:10.1242/jeb.132944.
- ^ Reidenberg, Joy (2017). "Terrestrial, Semiaquatic, and Fully Aquatic Mammal Sound Production Mechanisms". Acoustical Society of America. 13: 35–43.
- ^ Reynolds, John E. (1999). Biology of Marine Mammals. Melbourne, AU: Melbourne University Publishing. ISBN 978-1588342508.
- ^ a b c d Starnberger, Iris; Preininger, Doris; Hödl, Walter (2014). "The anuran vocal sac: A tool for multimodal signalling". Animal Behaviour. 97: 281–288. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.07.027.
- ^ Brown, Richard E.; Brain, Joseph D.; Wang, Ning (1997). "The avian respiratory system: A unique model for studies of respiratory toxicosis and for monitoring air quality". Environmental Health Perspectives. 105: 188–200.
- ^ a b Riede, Tobias; Goller, Franz (2010). "Peripheral mechanisms for vocal production in birds - differences and similarities to human speech and singing". Brain and Language. 115: 69–80. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.003.
- ^ Nowicki, Stephen (1987). "Vocal tract resonances in oscine bird sound production: Evidence from birdsongs in a helium atmosphere". Nature. 325: 53–55. doi:10.1038/325053a0.
- ^ Farner, Donald S.; King, James R. (1972). Avian Biology. New York: NY: Academic Press. ISBN 978-1483238616.
- ^ Geberzahn, Nicole; Aubin, Thierry (2014). "How a songbird with a continuous singing style modulates
its song when territorially challenged". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 68: 1–12. doi:10.1007/s00265-013-1616-4.
{{cite journal}}
: line feed character in|title=
at position 57 (help) - ^ Schmidt, Marc F.; Goller, Franz (2016). "Breathtaking songs: Coordinating the neural circuits for breathing and singing". American Physiological Society. 31: 442–451. doi:10.1152/physiol.00004.2016.
- ^ Stephen, R. O.; Hartley, J. C. (1995). "Sound production in crickets". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 198: 2139–2152.
- ^ a b c Luo, Changqing; Cong, Wei (2015). "Stridulatory Sound-Production and Its Function in Females of the Cicada Subpsaltria yangi". PLoS One. 10: 1–13. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0118667.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Montealegre-Z, Fernando; Jonsson, Thorin; Robert, Daniel (2011). "Sound radiation and wing mechanics in stridulating field crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae)". Journal of Experimental Biology. 214: 2105–2117. doi:10.1242/jeb.056283.
- ^ Heinrich, Ralf; Kunst, Michael; Wirmer, Andrea (2012). "Reproduction-Related Sound Production of Grasshoppers Regulated by Internal State and Actual Sensory Environment". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 6: 1–9. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00089.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ a b c d e f g h Wells, K. D. (2007). The ecology and behaviour of amphibians. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0226893341.
- ^ Odom, Karan J.; Hall, Michelle L.; Riebel, Katharina; Omland, Kevin E.; Langmore, Naomi E. (2014). "Female song is widespread and ancestral
in songbirds". Nature Communications. 5: 1–6. doi:10.1038/ncomms4379.
{{cite journal}}
: line feed character in|title=
at position 40 (help) - ^ a b c Garland, Ellen C.; Goldizen, Anne W.; Rekdahl, Melinda L.; Costantine, Rochelle; Garrigue, Claire; Hauser, Nan Daeschler; Poole, Michael; Robbins, Jooke; Noad, Michael J. (2011). "Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale". Current Biology. 21: 687–691. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.019.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Nowicki, Stephen; Searcy, William A. (2004). "Song function and the evolution of female
preferences". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1016: 704–723. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.012.
{{cite journal}}
: line feed character in|title=
at position 42 (help) - ^ Pough, F. Harvey; Janis, Christine M.; Heiser, John B. (2013). Vertebrate life. New York, NY: Pearson Education. ISBN 978-0321773364.
- ^ Janik, Vincent M. (2009). "Whale song". Cell Press. 19: 109–111. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.026.
- ^ a b Narins, Peter; Feng, Albert S.; Fay, Richard R. (2006). Hearing and sound communication in Amphibians. New York, NY: Springer. ISBN 978-0-387-47796-1.
- ^ Chew, Sek Jin; Vicario, David S.; Nottebohm, Fernando (1995). "A large-capacity memory system that recognizes the calls and songs of individual birds". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 93: 1950–1955.
- ^ Curé, C; Mathevon, N; Aubin, T (2016). "Mate vocal recognition in the Scopoli's shearwater Calonectris diomedea: Do females and males share the same acoustic code?". Behavioural Processes. 128: 96–102. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2016.04.013.
- ^ a b Beecher, Michael D.; Stoddard, Philip K.; Loesche, Patricia (1985). "Recognition of parents' voices by young cliff swallows". The Auk. 102: 600–605.
- ^ Chihiro, Mor; Kazuhiro, Wada (2015). "Songbird: a unique animal model for studying the molecular basis of disorders of vocal development and communication". Experimental Animals. 64: 221–230. doi:10.1538/expanim.15-0008.
- ^ Greig, Emma I.; Taft, Benjamin N.; Pruett-Jones, Stephen. "Sons learn songs from their social fathers in a cooperatively breeding bird". Proceedings Biological Sciences. 279: 3154–3160. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2582.
- ^ a b c Wada, Haruka (2010). "The Development of Birdsong". Nature Education Knowledge. 3: 86.
- ^ a b c d e Brenowitz, Eliot A.; Beecher, Michael D. (2005). "Song learning in birds: Diversity and
plasticity, opportunities and challenges". Trends in Neuroscience. 28: 127–132. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.004.
{{cite journal}}
: line feed character in|title=
at position 38 (help) - ^ Bell, D. A.; Trail, P. W.; Baptista, L. F. (1998). "Song learning and vocal tradition in Nuttall's white-crowned sparrows". Animal Behaviour. 55: 939–956. doi:10.1006/anbe.1997.0644.
- ^ Wiener, Linda (1986). "Song learning in birds: Possible models for human
language acquisition". Taylor & Francis Online. 37: 159–175. doi:10.1080/00437956.1986.11435775.
{{cite journal}}
: line feed character in|title=
at position 50 (help) - ^ a b c O'Loghlen, Adrian L; Rothstein, Stephen I (2002). "Ecological effects on song learning: delayed development is widespread in wild populations of brown-headed cowbirds". Animal Behaviour. 63: 475–486. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1951.
- ^ Baptista, Luis F. (1987). "Imitations of white-crowned sparrow songs by song sparrow". The Condor. 90: 489–492.
- ^ Garland, Ellen C.; Gedamke, Jason; Rekdahl, Melinda L.; Noad, Michael J.; Garrigue, Claire; Gales, Nick (2013). "Humpback whale song on the Southern ocean feeding grounds: Implications for cultural transmission". PLoS One. 8: e79422. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079422.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)