Bottlenose dolphin: Difference between revisions
ScottSteiner (talk | contribs) m Reverted edits by Hedwig4Life (talk) to last revision by ClueBot NG (HG) |
Hedwig4Life (talk | contribs) |
||
Line 59: | Line 59: | ||
{{cite news|url=http://www.livescience.com/animals/ap_050415_wolphin.html|title=Whale-Dolphin Hybrid Has Baby 'Wholphin'|last=Lee|first=Jaennette|date=April 15, 2005|publisher=Associated Press|accessdate=2008-09-20}}</ref> In captivity, a bottlenose dolphin and a [[rough-toothed dolphin]] hybridized.<ref> |
{{cite news|url=http://www.livescience.com/animals/ap_050415_wolphin.html|title=Whale-Dolphin Hybrid Has Baby 'Wholphin'|last=Lee|first=Jaennette|date=April 15, 2005|publisher=Associated Press|accessdate=2008-09-20}}</ref> In captivity, a bottlenose dolphin and a [[rough-toothed dolphin]] hybridized.<ref> |
||
{{cite web|title=The Mammals of Texas – Rough-toothed Dolphin|url=http://www.nsrl.ttu.edu/tmot1/stenbred.htm|accessdate=2008-09-20}}</ref> A [[common dolphin]]-bottlenose dolphin hybrid born in captivity lives at [[SeaWorld]] California.<ref> |
{{cite web|title=The Mammals of Texas – Rough-toothed Dolphin|url=http://www.nsrl.ttu.edu/tmot1/stenbred.htm|accessdate=2008-09-20}}</ref> A [[common dolphin]]-bottlenose dolphin hybrid born in captivity lives at [[SeaWorld]] California.<ref> |
||
{{cite web|title=Robin's Island Database about captive Dolphins and Whales|url=http://www.robins-island.org/dolphins_database.php?filter=Sea_World_California|accessdate=2008-09-20}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|title=Captive-born bottlenose dolphin × common dolphin (Tursiops truncatus × Delphinus capensis) intergeneric hybrids|author=Zornetzer H.R.; Duffield D.A.|journal=Canadian Journal of Zoology|volume=81|issue=10|pages=1755–1762|date=October 1, 2003|url=http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/nrc/cjz/2003/00000081/00000010/art00014?crawler=true|publisher=NRC Research Press|doi=10.1139/z03-150}}</ref> Other hybrids live in captivity around the world and in the wild, such as a bottlenose dolphin-[[Atlantic spotted dolphin]] hybrid.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Herzing, D., Moewe, K., & Brunnick, B.|year=2003| url=http://www.trustedpartner.com/docs/library/000059/Herzing%20Moewe%20Brunnick%20final.pdf|format=PDF|title=Interspecies interactions between Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, on Great Bahama Bank, Bahamas|journal=Aquatic Mammals|volume=29|issue=3|pages=335–341|accessdate=2008-09-20|doi=10.1578/01675420360736505}}</ref> |
{{cite web|title=Robin's Island Database about captive Dolphins and Whales|url=http://www.robins-island.org/dolphins_database.php?filter=Sea_World_California|accessdate=2008-09-20}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|title=Captive-born bottlenose dolphin × common dolphin (Tursiops truncatus × Delphinus capensis) intergeneric hybrids|author=Zornetzer H.R.; Duffield D.A.|journal=Canadian Journal of Zoology|volume=81|issue=10|pages=1755–1762|date=October 1, 2003|url=http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/nrc/cjz/2003/00000081/00000010/art00014?crawler=true|publisher=NRC Research Press|doi=10.1139/z03-150}}</ref> Other hybrids live in captivity around the world and in the wild, such as a bottlenose dolphin-[[Atlantic spotted dolphin]] hybrid.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Herzing, D., Moewe, K., & Brunnick, B.|year=2003| url=http://www.trustedpartner.com/docs/library/000059/Herzing%20Moewe%20Brunnick%20final.pdf|format=PDF|title=Interspecies interactions between Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, on Great Bahama Bank,POO Bahamas|journal=Aquatic Mammals|volume=29|issue=3|pages=335–341|accessdate=2008-09-20|doi=10.1578/01675420360736505}}</ref> |
||
==Description== |
==Description== |
Revision as of 07:01, 4 May 2011
Bottlenose Dolphin | |
---|---|
Bottlenose dolphin breaching in the bow wave of a boat | |
Size compared to an average human | |
Scientific classification | |
Kingdom: | |
Phylum: | |
Class: | |
Order: | |
Family: | |
Genus: | Tursiops Gervais, 1855
|
Species | |
See text | |
Bottlenose dolphin range (in blue) |
Bottlenose dolphins, the genus Tursiops, are the most common and well-known members of the family Delphinidae, the family of oceanic dolphins.[1] Recent molecular studies show the genus contains two species, the common bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops aduncus), instead of one. They inhabit warm and temperate seas worldwide.
Bottlenose dolphins live in groups typically of 10–30 members, called pods, but group size varies from single individuals up to more than 1,000. Their diet consists mainly of forage fish. Dolphins often work as a team to harvest fish schools, but they also hunt individually. Dolphins search for prey primarily using echolocation, which is similar to sonar. They emit clicking sounds and listen for the return echo to determine the location and shape of nearby items, including potential prey. Bottlenose dolphins also use sound for communication, including squeaks and whistles emitted from the blowhole and sounds emitted through body language, such as leaping from the water and slapping their tails on the water surface.
Bottlenose dolphins are one of the most intelligent mammals on Earth.[2] Research on bottlenose dolphins has examined mimicry, use of artificial language, object categorization and self-recognition. Their considerable intelligence has driven interaction with humans. Bottlenose dolphins are popular from aquarium shows and television programs such as Flipper. They have also been trained by militaries to locate sea mines or detect and mark enemy divers. In some areas, they cooperate with local fishermen by driving fish into their nets and eating the fish that escape. Some encounters with humans are harmful to the dolphins: people hunt them for food, and dolphins are killed inadvertently as a bycatch of tuna fishing.
Taxonomy
Scientists were long aware that Tursiops dolphins might consist of more than one species. Molecular genetics allowed much greater insight into this previously intractable problem. The consensus is there are two species:[3]
- The common bottlenose dolphin (T. truncatus), is found in most tropical to temperate oceans; color is grey, with the shade of grey varying among populations; it can be bluish-grey, brownish-grey, or even nearly black, and is often darker on the back from the rostrum to behind the dorsal fin.[4] and
- The Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (T. aduncus), lives in the waters around India, northern Australia, South China, the Red Sea, and the eastern coast of Africa; the back is dark-grey and the belly is lighter grey or nearly white with grey spots.[5]
The following are sometimes recognized as subspecies of T. truncatus:
- The Pacific bottlenose dolphin (T. gillii or T. truncatus gillii), lives in the Pacific, and has a black line from the eye to the forehead.[6][7]
- The Black Sea bottlenose dolphin (T. truncatus ponticus), lives in the Black Sea.[6][8]
There are two ecotypes of the common bottlenose dolphin within the western North Atlantic.[9] These are represented by the shallower water or coastal ecotype and the more offshore ecotype.[9] Their ranges overlap, but they have been shown to be genetically distinct.[9] They are not currently described, however, as separate species or subspecies. In general, there is significant genetic variation between populations, even among nearby populations.[4] As a result of this genetic variation, it is possible there are other distinct species currently considered to be populations of common bottlenose dolphin.[4]
Old scientific data do not distinguish among the two species, making it useless for determining structural differences between them. The IUCN lists both species as data deficient on their Red List of endangered species because of this issue.[10]
Some recent genetic evidence suggests the Indo-Pacific bottlenose belongs in the genus Stenella, since it is more like the Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) than the common bottlenose.[11][12]
Dolphins belong to the suborder Odontocetae, which groups all toothed whale species, the largest of which is the sperm whale. The sister branch of Odontocetae includes all baleen-producing species (Mysticetae), the largest of which is the blue whale.
Hybrids
Bottlenose dolphins have been known to hybridize with other dolphin species. Hybrids with Risso's dolphin occur both in the wild and in captivity.[13][14] The best known is the wolphin, a false killer whale-bottlenose dolphin hybrid. The wolphin is fertile, and two currently live at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii. The first was born in 1985 to a female bottlenose. Wolphins also exist in the wild.[15] In captivity, a bottlenose dolphin and a rough-toothed dolphin hybridized.[16] A common dolphin-bottlenose dolphin hybrid born in captivity lives at SeaWorld California.[17][18] Other hybrids live in captivity around the world and in the wild, such as a bottlenose dolphin-Atlantic spotted dolphin hybrid.[19]
Description
They are grey, varying from dark grey at the top near the dorsal fin to very light grey and almost white at the underside. This countershading makes it hard to see, both from above and below, when swimming. Adults range in length between 2 and 4 metres (6.6 and 13.1 ft), and in weight between 150 and 650 kilograms (330 and 1,430 lb).[20] Males are on average slightly longer and considerably heavier than females. In most parts of the world, the adult's length is about 2.5 metres (8.2 ft), with weight ranges between 200 and 300 kilograms (440 and 660 lb).[4] Their size varies considerably with habitat. Except in the eastern Pacific, dolphins in warmer, shallower waters tend to be smaller than those in cooler pelagic waters.[1] A survey in the Moray Firth in Scotland, the world's second northernmost dolphin population, recorded an average adult length of just under 4 metres (13 ft) compared with a 2.5 metres (8.2 ft) average in a population off the coast of Florida.
Bottlenose dolphins can live for more than 40 years.[21][22] However, one study of a population off Sarasota, Florida indicated an average lifespan of 20 years or less.[23]
Anatomy
Their elongated upper and lower jaws form what is called a rostrum, or snout, which gives the animal its common name. The real, functional nose is the blowhole on top of its head; the nasal septum is visible when the blowhole is open.
Bottlenose dolphins have 18 to 28 conical teeth on each side of each jaw.[21][24]
The flukes (lobes of the tail) and dorsal fin are formed of dense connective tissue and do not contain bone or muscle. The animal propels itself by moving the flukes up and down. The pectoral flippers (at the sides of the body) are for steering; they contain bones homologous to the forelimbs of land mammals. A bottlenose dolphin was discovered in Japan that has two additional pectoral fins, or "hind legs", at the tail, about the size of a human's pair of hands. Scientists believe a mutation caused the ancient trait to reassert itself as a form of atavism.[25]
Physiology and senses
In colder waters, they have more body fat and blood, and are more suited to deeper diving. Typically, 18%–20% of their bodyweight is blubber.[26] Most research in this area has been restricted to the North Atlantic Ocean.[27] Bottlenose dolphins typically swim at 5–11 km/h (1.4–2 m/s), but are capable of bursts of up to 29–35 km/h (8-9.7 m/s). The higher speeds can only be sustained for a short time.[28][29]
Echolocation
The dolphin's search for food is aided by a form of sonar known as echolocation: they locate objects by producing sounds and listening for the echo. A broadband burst pulse of clicking sounds is emitted in a focused beam in front of the dolphin. To hear the returning echo, they have two small ear openings behind the eyes, but most sound waves are transmitted to the inner ear through the lower jaw. As the object of interest is approached, the echo grows louder, and the dolphins adjust by decreasing the intensity of the emitted sounds. (This contrasts with bats and sonar, which reduce sensitivity of the sound receptor.) The interclick interval also decreases as the animal nears the target. Evidently, the dolphin waits for each click's echo before clicking again. Echolocation details, such as signal strength, spectral qualities, and discrimination, are well-understood by researchers.[30] Bottlenose dolphins are also able to extract shape information, suggesting they are able to form an "echoic image" or sound picture of their targets.[31]
Eyesight
Dolphins have sharp eyesight. The eyes are located at the sides of the head and have a tapetum lucidum, or reflecting membrane at the back of the retina, which aids vision in dim light. Their horseshoe-shaped, double-slit pupil enables dolphins to have good vision both in air and underwater, despite the different densities of these media.[32] When underwater, the eyeball's lens serves to focus light, whereas in the in-air environment, the typically bright light serves to contract the specialized pupil, resulting in sharpness from a smaller aperture (similar to a pinhole camera).
Smell
By contrast, a bottlenose's sense of smell is poor,[33] because its blowhole, the analogue to the nose, is closed when underwater and opens only for breathing. It has no olfactory nerves or olfactory lobe in the brain.[33] Bottlenose dolphins are able to detect salty, sweet, bitter (quinine sulphate), and sour (citric acid) tastes, but this has not been well-studied.[33] Anecdotally, some animals in captivity have been noted to have preferences for food fish types, although it is not clear if taste mediates this preference.[33]
Communication
Bottlenose dolphins communicate through burst pulsed sounds, whistles, and body language. Examples of body language include leaping out of the water, snapping jaws, slapping the tail on the surface and butting heads.[34] Sounds and gestures help keep track of other dolphins in the group, and alert other dolphins to danger and nearby food. Lacking vocal cords, they produce sounds using six air sacs near their blow hole. Each animal has a uniquely-identifying, frequency-modulated narrow-band signature vocalization (signature whistle).[35]
Researchers from the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute (BDRI), based in Sardinia (Italy) have now shown whistles and burst pulsed sounds are vital to the animals' social life and mirror their behaviours.[36]
The tonal whistle sounds (the most melodious ones) allow dolphins to stay in contact with each other (above all, mothers and offspring), and to coordinate hunting strategies.[37] The burst-pulsed sounds (which are more complex and varied than the whistles) are used "to avoid physical aggression in situations of high excitement", such as when they are competing for the same piece of food, for example. The dolphins emit these strident sounds when in the presence of other individuals moving towards the same prey. The "least dominant" one soon moves away in order to avoid confrontation.[36]
Other communication uses about 30 distinguishable sounds, and although famously proposed by John Lilly in the 1950s, no "dolphin language" has been found. However, Herman, Richards, & Wolz demonstrated comprehension of an artificial language by two bottlenose dolphins (named Akeakamai and Phoenix) in the period of skepticism toward animal language following Herbert Terrace's critique.[38]
Intelligence
Cognition
Cognitive abilities investigated include concept formation, sensory skills, and mental representations. Such research has been ongoing since the 1970s. This includes:
- acoustic and behavioral mimicry,[39][40]
- comprehension of novel sequences in an artificial language,[41][42]
- memory,[43]
- monitoring of self behavior,[44]
- discrimination and matching,[43][45]
- comprehension of symbols for various body parts,[46]
- comprehension of pointing gestures and gaze (as made by dolphins or humans),[47]
- mirror self-recognition,[48][49] and
- numerical values.[50]
Tool use and culture
At least some wild bottlenose dolphins use tools. In Shark Bay, dolphins place a marine sponge on their rostrum, presumably to protect it when searching for food on the sandy sea bottom.[51] This has only been observed in this bay (first in 1997), and almost exclusively by females. Sea otters are the only other marine mammalian tool users. A 2005 study showed mothers most likely teach the behavior to their daughters, evincing culture (behavior learned from other species members).[52] Some Mauritanian dolphins cooperate with human fishermen. The dolphins drive a school of fish towards the shore, where humans await with nets. In the confusion of casting nets, the dolphins catch a large number of fish, as well. Intraspecies cooperative foraging has also been observed. These behaviors may also be transmitted via teaching. Controversially, Rendell and Whitehead have proposed a structure for the study of cetaceans culture.[53][54]
Near Adelaide, in South Australia, three bottlenose dolphins 'tail-walk', whereby they elevate the upper part of their bodies vertically out of the water, and propel themselves along the surface with powerful tail movements. Tail-walking mostly arises via human training in dolphinaria. In the 1980s, a female from the local population was kept at a local dolphinarium for three weeks, and the scientist suggests she copied the tail-walking behavior from other dolphins. Two other wild adult female dolphins have now copied it from her.[55]
Cortical neurons
Some researchers theorize mammalian intelligence correlates to the number of nerve cells (neurons) in the cortex of the brain.[56] Bottlenose dolphins have about 5.8 billion cortical neurons, placing them between chimpanzees at 6.2 billion and gorillas at 4.3 billion. (Humans have 11.5 billion.).[56] However, after Homo sapiens, the species with the highest number of cortical neurons and synapses is the elephant.[57]: 73
Life history
Respiration and sleep
The bottlenose dolphin has a single blowhole located on the dorsal surface of the head consisting of a hole and a muscular flap. The flap is closed during muscle relaxation and opens during contraction.[58] Dolphins are voluntary breathers, who must deliberately surface and open their blowholes to get air.[citation needed] They can store almost twice as much oxygen in proportion to their body weight as a human can. The dolphin can store 36 millilitres of oxygen per kilogram of body weight, compared with 20 millilitres per kilogram for humans.[59] This is an adaptation to diving.[59] The bottlenose dolphin typically rises to the surface to breathe through its blowhole two to three times per minute,[60] although it can remain submerged for up to 20 minutes.[61]
Dolphins can breathe while "half-asleep". During the sleeping cycle, one brain hemisphere remains active, while the other hemisphere shuts down. The active hemisphere handles surfacing and breathing behavior.[62] The daily sleeping cycle lasts for about 8 hours, in increments of minutes to hours. During the sleeping cycle, they remain near the surface, swimming slowly or "logging", and occasionally closing one eye.[63]
Reproduction
Both genders have genital slits on the underside of their bodies. The male can retract and conceal its penis through its slit.[64] The female's slit houses its vagina and anus. Females have two mammary slits, each housing one nipple, one on each side of the genital slit.[65][65] The ability to stow their reproductive organs (especially in males) allows for maximum hydrodynamics.
Males compete for access to females. Such competition can take the form of fighting other males or of herding females to prevent access by other males.[66][67] Mating occurs belly to belly.[67] Dolphins have been observed engaging in intercourse when the females are not in their estrus cycles and cannot produce young, as well as when they can.
Breeding season produces significant physiological changes in males. At that time, testes enlarge, enabling it to hold more sperm. Large amounts of sperm allow a male to wash away the previous suitor's sperm, while leaving some of his own for fertilization. Also, sperm concentration markedly increases. Having less sperm for out-of-season social mating means it wastes less. This suggests sperm production is energetically expensive. Males have large testes in relation to their body size.[68]
The gestation period averages 12 months.[4] Births can occur at any time of year, although peaks occur in warmer months.[1] The young are born in shallow water, sometimes assisted by a (possibly male) "midwife", and usually only a single calf is born.[69] Twins are possible but rare. Newborn bottlenose dolphins are between 0.8–1.4 m (2.6–4.6 ft) 9–30 kg (20–66 lb) kilograms, with Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin infants generally smaller than common bottlenose dolphin infants.[4]
To accelerate nursing, the mother can eject milk from her mammary glands. The calf suckles for 18 to 20 months, and continues to closely associate with its mother for several years after weaning.[21] Females sexually mature at ages 5–13, males at ages 9–14.[1] Females reproduce every two to six years.[4]
Georgetown University professor Janet Mann argues the strong personal behavior among male calves is about bond formation and benefits the species in an evolutionary context. She cites studies showing these dolphins as adults are inseparable, and that early bonds aid protection as well as in locating females.[70]
Male bottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs or larger groups to follow and/or restrict the movement of a female for weeks at a time, waiting for her to become sexually receptive.[66]
Social interaction
Adult males live mostly alone or in groups of two to three, and join pods for short periods of time. Adult female and young dolphins normally live in groups of up to 15 animals.[4] However, they live in fission-fusion societies of varying group size, within which individuals change associations, often on a daily or hourly basis.[71][72] Typically, a group of adult females and their newborns associate in a pod, while older juveniles inhabit mixed pods. Pods can join to form groups of 100 or more, and occasionally exceed 1,000.[4] The social strategies of marine mammals such as bottlenose dolphins "provide interesting parallels" with the social strategies of elephants and chimpanzees.[73]: 519
Bottlenose dolphins studied by Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute researchers off the island of Sardinia show random social behaviour while feeding, and their social behaviour does not depend on feeding activity.[74] In Sardinia, the presence of a floating marine fin-fish farm has been linked to a change in bottlenose dolphin distribution as a result of high fish density around the floating cages in the farming area.[75]
Ecology
Feeding
Its diet consists mainly of small fish, crustaceans, and squid.[1][4] Although this varies by location, many populations share an appetite for fish from the mullet family, the tuna and mackerel family, and the drum and croaker family.[1]
Its cone-like teeth serve to grasp, but do not chew food.[76][77] When they encounter a shoal of fish, they work as a team to herd them towards the shore to maximize the harvest.[4] They also hunt alone, often targeting bottom-dwelling species. The bottlenose dolphin sometimes hits a fish with its fluke, sometimes knocking it out of the water, using a strategy called "fish whacking".[22][78]
Bottlenose dolphins conflict with small-scale coastal commercial fisheries in some Mediterranean areas. Common bottlenose dolphins are probably attracted to fishing nets because they offer a concentrated food source.[79]
"Strand feeding", is an inherited feeding technique used by bottlenose dolphins near and around coastal regions of South Carolina. When a pod finds a school of fish, they will circle the school and trap the fish in a mini whirlpool. Then, the dolphins will charge at the school and push their bodies up onto a mud-flat, forcing the fish on the mud-flat as well. The dolphins then crawl around on their sides, consuming the fish they washed up on shore.
Relations with other species
Dolphins exhibit altruistic behaviour toward other sea creatures. On Mahia Beach, New Zealand on 10 March 2008,[80] two pygmy sperm whales, a female and calf, stranded on the beach. Rescuers, including Department of Conservation officer Malcolm Smith, attempted to refloat them four times. Shortly, a playful bottlenose dolphin known to local residents as Moko arrived and, after apparently communicating with the whales, led them Template:M to ft along a sandbar to the open sea, saving them from imminent euthanasia.[81]
The bottlenose dolphin can behave aggressively. Males fight for rank and access to females. During mating season, males compete vigorously with each other through displays of toughness and size, with a series of acts such as head-butting. They display aggression towards sharks, certain Killer whales, and smaller dolphin species. At least one population, off Scotland, has practiced infanticide, and also has attacked and killed harbour porpoises. University of Aberdeen researchers say the dolphins do not eat their victims, but are simply competing for food.[82] Similar behaviour has been observed in Ireland.[83]
The bottlenose dolphin sometimes forms mixed species groups with other species from the dolphin family, particularly larger species such as the short-finned pilot whale, the false killer whale and Risso's dolphin.[66][84][85] They also interact with smaller species, such as the Atlantic spotted dolphin and the rough-toothed dolphin.[66][86] While interactions with smaller species are sometimes affiliative, they can also be hostile.[66]
Predators
Some large shark species, such as the tiger shark, the dusky shark, the great white shark and the bull shark, prey on the bottlenose dolphin, especially calves.[67][87][88][89][90] The bottlenose dolphin is capable of defending itself by charging the predator; dolphin 'mobbing' behavior of sharks can occasionally prove fatal for the shark.[87] Targeting a single adult dolphin can be dangerous for a shark of similar size. Certain (but not all) killer whale (orca) populations may also prey on dolphins, but this seems rare,[87] and other orcas may swim with dolphins.
Swimming in pods allows dolphins to better defend themselves against predators. Bottlenose dolphins either use complex evasive strategies to outswim their predators, or mobbing techniques to batter the predator to death or force it to flee.
Relation to humans
Interaction
The species sometimes shows curiosity towards humans in or near water. Occasionally, they rescue injured divers by raising them to the surface. They also do this to help injured members of their own species.[87] In November 2004, a dramatic report of dolphin intervention came from New Zealand. Four lifeguards, swimming Template:M to ft off the coast near Whangarei, were approached by a shark (reportedly a great white shark). Bottlenose dolphins herded the swimmers together and surrounded them for forty minutes, preventing the shark from attacking, as they slowly swam to shore.[91]
In coastal regions, dolphins run the risk of colliding with boats. Researchers of the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute first quantified data about solitary bottlenose dolphin diving behavior in the presence and absence of boats.[92] Dolphins responded more to tourist than fishing vessels. Driving behavior, speed, engine type and separation distance all affect dolphin safety.
Bottlenose dolphins perform in many aquaria, generating much controversy. Some animal welfare activists claim the dolphins do not have adequate space or receive adequate care or stimulation. However, others counter that the dolphins are properly cared for and enjoy interacting with humans.[87][93]
Eight bottlenose dolphins that lived at the Marine Life Aquarium in Gulfport, Mississippi were swept away from their aquarium pool during Hurricane Katrina. They were later rescued in the Gulf of Mexico.[94]
Therapies for handicapped children can include interactions with bottlenose dolphins.[87]
The military of the United States and Russia train bottlenose dolphins as military dolphins for wartime tasks, such as locating sea mines and detecting enemy divers.[95][96] The USA's program is the U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program, located in San Diego, California.[97]
Commercial 'dolphin encounter' enterprises and tours operate in many countries. In addition to such endeavors, the animals swim with and surface near surfers at the beach.[98]
In the town of Laguna in south Brazil, a pod of bottlenose dolphins drives fish towards fishermen who stand at the beach in shallow waters. One dolphin then rolls over, after which the fishermen throw out their nets. The dolphins feed on the escaping fish. The dolphins were not trained for this behavior; the collaboration began before 1847. Similar cooperative fisheries also exist in Mauritania, Africa.[99]
Culture
The popular television show Flipper, created by Ivan Tors, portrayed a bottlenose dolphin[100] in a friendly relationship with two boys, Sandy and Bud. A seagoing Lassie, Flipper understood English and was a hero: "Go tell Dad we're in trouble, Flipper! Hurry!" The show's theme song contains the lyric no one you see / is smarter than he. The television show was based on a 1963 film, and was remade as a feature film in 1996, starring Elijah Wood and Paul Hogan, as well as a second series running from 1995–2000, starring Jessica Alba.[101]
Dolphin Tale, directed by Charles Martin Smith, starring Nathan Gamble, Ashley Judd, Harry Connick, Jr., Morgan Freeman, Kris Kristofferson and Winter, herself. The movie will be in theatres on September 23, 2011.[citation needed]
Other television appearances by bottlenose dolphins include Dolphin Cove, seaQuest DSV, the HBO movie Zeus and Roxanne, in which a female bottlenose dolphin befriends a male dog, and in a 1996 made for TV movie, Bermuda Triangle, in which Annie (played by Lisa Jakub) swim with dolphins, some human and dolphin interaction segments featured on Sesame Street and a Halloween episode of The Simpsons.[citation needed]
Bottlenose dolphins have appeared in novels. In The Hitchhiker's Guide to the Galaxy and one of its sequels, So Long, and Thanks For All the Fish, the dolphins try to warn humans of Earth's impending destruction, but their behavior was misinterpreted as playful acrobatics. Bottlenose dolphins are also central to David Brin's series of Uplift Universe novels, particularly Startide Rising, where they are one of the two Earth species (along with chimpanzees) to have been 'uplifted' to sentience. Bottlenose dolphins are primary characters in Anne McCaffrey's Dragonriders of Pern series, especially The Dolphins of Pern. Bottlenose dolphins are incorporated into the science fiction video game series Ecco the Dolphin. Delphineus, a dolphin, is also featured in the video game EcoQuest: The Search for Cetus, helping the boy, Adam, to find the sea king Cetus (a sperm whale), as well as assisting in cleaning up the underwater environment where he lives. Two dolphins also appeared in the film Jaws 3-D protecting Kay Morgan, the marine biologist, from shark attacks.[citation needed]
The Miami Dolphins NFL franchise uses the bottlenose dolphin as its mascot and team logo.
Factual descriptions of the dolphins date back into antiquity – the writings of Aristotle, Oppian and Pliny the Elder all mention the species.[1][102]
Threats
Bottlenose dolphins are still killed in dolphin drive hunts for their meat or to eliminate competition for fish.[103] Bottlenose dolphins (and several other dolphin species) often travel with tuna, and can get caught in tuna nets, which can kill the dolphins.[24][104] Boycotts of tuna products led to the concept of "dolphin-safe" labeling for fishing methods that avoid endangering dolphins.[104]
The man-made chemical perfluorooctanesulfonic acid (PFOS) may be compromising the immune system of bottlenose dolphins.[105] PFOS affects the immune system of male mice at a concentration of 91.5 ppb,[106] while PFOS has been reported in bottlenose dolphins in excess of 1 ppm.[107]
High levels of metal contaminants have been measured in tissues in many areas of the globe. A recent study found high levels of cadmium and mercury in bottlenose dolphins from South Australia,[108] levels which were later found to be associated with kidney malformations, indicating possible health effects of high metal concentrations in dolphins.[109]
Conservation
Bottlenose dolphins are not endangered. Their future is stable because of their abundance and adaptability. However, specific populations are threatened due to various environmental changes. The population in the Moray Firth in Scotland is estimated to consist of around 150 animals, and is declining by around 6% per year from harassment, traumatic injury, water pollution and reduction in food availability. Likewise, an isolated population in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand, is in decline due to calf loss coincident to an increase in warm freshwater discharge into the fjord.[110] Less local climate change, such as increasing water temperature may also play a role but has never been shown to be the case.[111]
In U.S. waters, hunting and harassing of marine mammals is forbidden in almost all circumstances.
See also
References
- ^ a b c d e f g
Wells, R. and Scott, M. (2002). "Bottlenose Dolphins". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 122–127. ISBN 0-12-551340-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ [1]
- ^ Rice, Dale W (1998). Marine mammals of the world: systematics and distribution (Special Publication). Society of Marine Mammalogy. ISBN 1-891276-03-4.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k
Shirihai, H. and Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. pp. 155–161. ISBN 0-691-12757-3.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Shirihai, H. and Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. pp. 159–161. ISBN 0-691-12757-3.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Mead, J. G.; Brownell, R. L. Jr. (2005). "Order Cetacea". In Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 723–743. ISBN 978-0-8018-8221-0. OCLC 62265494. Cite error: The named reference "MSW3" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ "Catalog of Living Whales". Retrieved 2008-10-01.
- ^
"Convention of International Trade in Endanged Species of Wild Flora and Fauna" (PDF). 2000. Retrieved 2008-10-01.
{{cite web}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ a b c "Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Western North Atlantic Offshore Stock" (PDF). Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ "Tursiops truncatus: Species Information". IUCN. Retrieved 2006-11-03.
- ^ LeDuc R.G., Perrin W.F. and Dizon A.E. (1999). "Phylogenetic relationships among the delphinids cetaceans based on full cyctochrome b sequences". Marine Mammal Science. 15: 619–648. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1999.tb00833.x.
- ^
Leduc, R., Perrin, W. & Dizon, E. (August 18, 1998). "Phylogenetic Relationships among the Delphinid Cetaceans Based on Full Cytochrome B Sequences". Marine Mammal Science. 15 (3): 619–648. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1999.tb00833.x. Retrieved 2008-10-05.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. Knopf. p. 422. ISBN 0-375-41141-0.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ "Risso's Dolphin – American Cetacean Society". Retrieved 2008-09-20.
- ^ Lee, Jaennette (April 15, 2005). "Whale-Dolphin Hybrid Has Baby 'Wholphin'". Associated Press. Retrieved 2008-09-20.
- ^ "The Mammals of Texas – Rough-toothed Dolphin". Retrieved 2008-09-20.
- ^ "Robin's Island Database about captive Dolphins and Whales". Retrieved 2008-09-20.
- ^ Zornetzer H.R.; Duffield D.A. (October 1, 2003). "Captive-born bottlenose dolphin × common dolphin (Tursiops truncatus × Delphinus capensis) intergeneric hybrids". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 81 (10). NRC Research Press: 1755–1762. doi:10.1139/z03-150.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Herzing, D., Moewe, K., & Brunnick, B. (2003). "Interspecies interactions between Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, on Great Bahama Bank,POO Bahamas" (PDF). Aquatic Mammals. 29 (3): 335–341. doi:10.1578/01675420360736505. Retrieved 2008-09-20.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ American Cetacean Society Fact Sheet – Bottlenose Dolphin
- ^ a b c
Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). National Audubon Society Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A. A. Knopf. pp. 362–365. ISBN 0-375-41141-0.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b "Office of Protected Resources – Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ "Bottlenose Dolphins – Longevity and Causes of Death". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ a b "Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus at MarineBio.org". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ "Dolphin reveals an extra set of 'legs'". Retrieved 2007-07-05.
- ^ "Bottlenose Dolphins: Adaptations for an Aquatic Environment".
- ^
Hersh, Sandra L. (1990). "Distinction Between Northwest Atlantic Offshore and Coastal Bottlenose Dolphins Based on Hemoglobin Profile and Morphometry". In Stephen Leatherwood and Randall R. Reeves (ed.). The Bottlenose Dolphin. San Diego: Academic Press. pp. 129–139. ISBN 0-12-440280-1.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Goforth, Harold W., Jr. "Ergometry (Exercise Testing) of the Bottlenose Dolphin." In The Bottlenose Dolphin, edited by Stephen Leatherwood and Randall R. Reeves, pp. 559-574. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc., 1990.
- ^ FISH, F. E. AND HUI, C. A.(1991). Dolphin swimming – a review. Mamm. Rev. 21, 181–195.
- ^ Au, Whitlow (1993). The Sonar of Dolphins. New York: Springer-Verlag. ISBN 3-540-97835-6.
- ^
Pack AA, Herman LM (1995). "Sensory integration in the bottlenosed dolphin: immediate recognition of complex shapes across the senses of echolocation and vision". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 98 (2 Pt 1): 722–33. doi:10.1121/1.413566. PMID 7642811.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^
Herman, L. M. (1975). "Bottlenosed dolphin: Double-slit pupil yields equivalent aerial and underwater diurnal acuity". Science. 139 (4203): 650–652. doi:10.1126/science.1162351. PMID 1162351.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b c d "Dolphin Characteristics". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^ "Bottlenose Dolphins: Animal information, pictures, map". National Geographic. Archived from the original on October 23, 2006. Retrieved 2006-11-03.
- ^
Janik VM, Slater PJB (1998). "Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls". Animal Behaviour. 56 (4): 829–838. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0881. PMID 9790693.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ a b
Díaz López, B. and Shirai, J.A.B., 2009 (2009). "MEDITERRANEAN COMMON BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN'S REPERTOIRE AND COMMUNICATION USE". Dolphins: Anatomy, Behavior and Threats: 129–148. ISBN 978-1-60876-849-3.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link) - ^
Díaz López (2010). "Whistle characteristicsinfree-rangingbottlenosedolphins(Tursiops truncatus) in the Mediterranean Sea: Influence of behaviour". Mammalian Biology. doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2010.06.006.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|month=
(help) - ^
Herman LM, Richards DG, Wolz JP (1984). "Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins". Cognition. 16 (2): 129–219. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(84)90003-9. PMID 6540652.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Reiss D, McCowan B (1993). "Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): evidence for vocal learning". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107 (3): 301–12. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.107.3.301. PMID 8375147.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ "The Dolphin Institute – Behavioral Mimicry". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^
Herman, L. (2002). "Language Learning". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 685–689. ISBN 0-12-551340-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link) - ^
"The Dolphin Institute – Understanding Language". Retrieved 2008-09-31.
{{cite web}}
: Check date values in:|accessdate=
(help) - ^ a b
Herman, L., Pack, A. & Wood, A. (August 26, 2006). "Bottlenose Dolphins Can Generalize Rules and Develop Abstract Concepts". Marine Mammal Science. 10 (1): 70–80. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1994.tb00390.x.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ "The Dolphin Institute – Awareness of One's Own Recent Behaviors". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^
Herman LM, Gordon JA (1974). "Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 21 (1): 19–26. doi:10.1901/jeab.1974.21-19. PMC 1333166. PMID 4204143.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ "The Dolphin Institute – Awareness of One's Own Body Parts". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^ "The Dolphin Institute – Pointing Gestures". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^ "Intelligence and Humans". Retrieved 2008-08-11.
- ^
Marten, K. & Psarakos, S. (1995). "Evidence of self-awareness in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". In Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. & Boccia, M. (ed.). Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. pp. 361–379. ISBN 978-0-521-44108-7. Retrieved 2008-10-04.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ A dolphin was shown two panels with varying numbers of dots of different size and position. The dolphin was able to touch the panel with the greater number. "Can Dolphins Count?". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^
Smolker, R.A.; et al. (1997). "Sponge Carrying by Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.): A Foraging Specialization Involving Tool Use?". Ethology. 103 (6): 454–465. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1997.tb00160.x.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^
Krutzen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (25): 8939–8943. doi:10.1073/pnas.0500232102. PMC 1157020. PMID 15947077.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Rendell, L. (2001). "Culture in whales and dolphins". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (2): 309–382. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0100396X.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Premack, D. & Hauser, M. "A whale of a tale: Calling it culture doesn't help" (PDF): 1–3. Retrieved 2008-09-30.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ "Wild dolphins teaches others tail walking tricks". WDCS, the Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society.
- ^ a b
Dicke, U. and Roth G. (August/September 2008). "Intelligence Evolved". Scientific American Mind. pp. 75–77.
{{cite news}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help) - ^ Roth, Gerhard (2002). "Is the human brain unique?". Mirror Neurons and the Evolution of Brain and Language. John Benjamins Publishing. pp. 63–76. ISBN 978-90-272-5166-4.
{{cite book}}
: External link in
(help); Unknown parameter|chapterurl=
|chapterurl=
ignored (|chapter-url=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ "Bottlenose dolphins". Seaworld. Retrieved 2008-03-14.
- ^ a b
Kooyman, G. (2002). "Diving Physiology". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 339–341. ISBN 0-12-551340-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link) - ^ "Adaptations for an aquatic environment". Busch Gardens. Retrieved 2008-03-14.
- ^ "Dolphin Q&A". Mia Research Foundation. Archived from the original on 2008-03-18. Retrieved 2008-03-13.
- ^ "Do whales and dolphins sleep". howstuffworks. Retrieved 2008-03-13.
- ^ "Dolphin Q&A". Mia Research Foundation. Archived from the original on 2008-03-18. Retrieved 2008-03-13.
- ^
Atkinson, S. (2002). "Male Reproductive Systems". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 700. ISBN 0-12-551340-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link) - ^ a b
Stewart, R. (2002). "Female Reproductive Systems". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 422–428. ISBN 0-12-551340-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link) - ^ a b c d e
Connor, R.; Wells, R.; Mann, J.; Read, A. (2000). "The Bottlenose Dolphin". In Mann, J.; Connor, R.; Tyack, P.; Whitehead, H. (ed.). Cetacean Societies. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. p. 102. ISBN 0-226-50341-0.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c "Bottlenose Dolphins of Sarasota Bay". Retrieved 2008-10-02.
- ^
Connor Richard C., Peterson, Dawn M. (1994). The Lives of Whales and Dolphins. New York.: Henry Holt and Company. ISBN 978-0-8050-1950-6.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Connor, R.; Wells, R.; Mann, J.; Read, A. (2000). "The Bottlenose Dolphin". In Mann, J.; Connor, R.; Tyack, P.; Whitehead, H. (ed.). Cetacean Societies. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. p. 95. ISBN 0-226-50341-0.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Mann, J. Vasey, P.; Sommer, V. (eds.). "Homosexual Behaviour in Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective". Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-86446-6.
{{cite journal}}
:|chapter=
ignored (help); Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Connor, Richards (2000). Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- ^
Díaz López, Bruno (2007). "Marine aquaculture and bottlenose dolphins' (Tursiops truncatus) social structure". Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 62: 887. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0512-1.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Acevedo-Gutiérrez, Alejandro (2008). "Group behavior". Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals (2 ed.). United States: Academic Press. pp. 511–520. ISBN 012373553X.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Díaz López, Bruno (2008). "Marine aquaculture off Sardinia Island (Italy):ecosystem effects evaluated through a trophic mass-balance model". Ecological Modelling 212: 292–303. 212: 292. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2007.10.028.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Díaz López, Bruno (2006). "Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) presence and incidental capture in a marine fish farm on the north-eastern coast of Sardinia (Italy)". Journal of Marine Biological Ass. UK. 87: 113–117. doi:10.1017/S0025315407054215.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ "Dolphins – Skeletal Structure". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ "Bottlenose Dolphins – Physical Characteristics". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ "Comparative Mammalian Brain Collections – Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ Díaz López, Bruno (2006). "Interaction between bottlenose dolphins and fisheries off Sardinia". ICES Journal of Marine Science. 63: 946–951. doi:10.1016/j.icesjms.2005.06.012.
- ^ "Dolphin rescues stranded whales". CNN. 2008-03-12. Archived from the original on May 12, 2008.
- ^ Brooks, David (2008-03-12). "Dolphin rescues beached whales". Herald Sun.
- ^ Read, Andrew (1999). Porpoises. Stillwater, MN, USA: Voyageur Press. ISBN 0-89658-420-8.
- ^ Ryan, Conor (2008). "Attack on Harbour Poropise (Phocoena phocoena (L. 1758) by Bottle-nosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus (Montagu 1821)". Irish Naturalists' Journal. 29 (2): 130.
- ^
Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. Knopf. p. 434. ISBN 0-375-41141-0.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ "Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): California/Oregon/Washington Offshore Stock" (PDF). Retrieved 2008-09-21.
- ^
Shirihai, H. and Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. p. 166. ISBN 0-691-12757-3.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f "Bottlenose Dolphin at Animal Corner". Retrieved 2008-09-16.
- ^
Heithaus, M. & Dill, L. (2002). "Food Availability and Tiger Shark Predation Risk Influence Bottlenose Dolphin Habitat Use". Ecology. 83 (2): 480–491. doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2002)083[0480:FAATSP]2.0.CO;2.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Gannon, D., Barros, N., Nowacek, D., Read, A., Waples, D. & Wells, R. (2004). "Prey detection by bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: an experimental test of the passive listening hypothesis". Animal Behavior. 69 (3): 709. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.020. Retrieved 2008-09-30.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Eisfeld, S. "The social affiliation and group composition of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the outer southern Moray Firth, NE Scotland" (PDF). p. 42. Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ Thomson, Ainsley (2004-11-25). "Dolphins saved us from shark, lifeguards say". New Zealand Herald.
- ^ Diaz Lopez, B.; Shirai, J.A.; Bilbao Prieto, A.; Méndez Fernández, P. (2008). "Diving activity of a solitary wild free ranging bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)" (PDF). Journal of Marine Biological Association U.K. 88 (6): 1153–1157. doi:10.1017/S0025315408000921.
- ^ "Animal rights groups rap Pasig dolphin show". Retrieved 2008-09-16.
- ^ "Katrina dolphin rescue launched". BBC. 15 September 2005. Retrieved 2008-02-14.
- ^ "Dolphins Deployed as Undersea Agents in Iraq". National Geographic. Retrieved 2009-01-18.
- ^ Rehn, KW; Riggs, PK (2002). "Non-Lethal Swimmer Neutralization Study". U.S. Space and Naval Warfare Systems Center Technical Report. Document Number 3138. Retrieved 2008-09-25.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
"U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program Web Site". U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program. Retrieved 2000-01-18.
{{cite web}}
: Check date values in:|accessdate=
(help) - ^ Dolphins and porspoises Australian Government: Dept. of the Environment, Water, Heritage and the Arts, Accessed Oct. 24, 2008
- ^ "Bottlenose Dolphin". Archived from the original on April 21, 2008. Retrieved 2008-08-11.
- ^ "American Cetacean Society – Bottlenose Dolphin". Retrieved 2008-08-31.
- ^ "Flipper (1995)". IMDb. Retrieved 2006-11-03.
- ^
Constantine, R. (2002). "Folklore and Legends". In Perrin, W., Würsig, B. & Thewissen, J. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 448. ISBN 0-12-551340-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link) - ^ Kenyon, P. (2004-11-08). "Dining With The Dolphin Hunters". BBC News. Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ a b "The Dolphin Institute – Threats to the Bottlenose Dolphin and Other Marine Mammals". Retrieved 2008-09-30.
- ^ Betts KS (2007). "Perfluoroalkyl Acids: What Is the Evidence Telling Us?". Environmental Health Perspectives. 115 (5). doi:10.1289/ehp.115-a250. PMC 1867999. PMID 17520044. Retrieved October 25, 2008.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^
Betts KS (2008). "Not immune to PFOS effects?". Environ. Health Perspect. 116 (7): A290. doi:10.1289/ehp.116-a290a. PMC 2453185. PMID 18629339.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^
Houde M, Martin JW, Letcher RJ, Solomon KR, Muir DC (2006). "Biological monitoring of polyfluoroalkyl substances: A review". Environmental Science & Technology. 40 (11): 3463–73. doi:10.1021/es052580b. PMID 16786681.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Lavery TJ, Butterfield N, Kemper CM, Reid RJ, Sanderson K (2008). "Metals and selenium in the liver and bone of three dolphin species from South Australia, 1988–2004". The Science of the Total Environment. 390 (1): 77–85. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2007.09.016. PMID 18006044.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Lavery TJ, Kemper CM, Sanderson K; et al. (2009). "Heavy metal toxicity of kidney and bone tissues in South Australian adult bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus)". Marine Environmental Research. 67 (1): 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.marenvres.2008.09.005. PMID 19012959.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Currey RJC, Dawson SM, Slooten E, Schneider K, Lusseau D, Boisseau OJ, Haase P, Williams JA (25 Nov 2008). "Survival rates for a declining population of bottlenose dolphins in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand: an information theoretic approach to assessing the role of human impacts". Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems. 19 (6): 658–670. doi:10.1002/aqc.1015.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Curran, S., Wilson, B. and Thompson, P (1996). "Recommendations for the sustainable management of the bottlenose dolphin population in the Moray Firth". Scottish Natural Heritage Review. 56.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
- General references
- Hale, P.T., Barreto, A.S. and Ross, G.J.B (2000). "Comparative morphology and distribution of the aduncus and truncatus forms of bottlenose dolphin Tursiops in the Indian and Western Pacific Oceans" (PDF). Aquatic Mammals. 26 (2): 101–110.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) — Discusses distinguishing features between Bottlenose Dolphin species - Reiss D, Marino L (2001). "Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (10): 5937–5942. doi:10.1073/pnas.101086398. PMC 33317. PMID 11331768.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)
Further reading
- Berrow, S.D. (2009). "Winter distribution of Bottle-nosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus (Montagu)) in the inner Shannon Estuary" (PDF). Irish Naturalists' Journal. 30 (1): 35–39.