Absolute pitch
Absolute pitch (AP), often called perfect pitch, is a rare ability of a person to identify or re-create a given musical note without the benefit of a reference tone.[1][2] AP may be demonstrated using linguistic labeling ("naming" a note), associating mental imagery with the note, or sensorimotor responses. For example, an AP possessor can accurately reproduce a heard tone on a musical instrument without "hunting" for the correct pitch.[3][4]
The frequency of AP in the general population is not known. The assumed occurrence of less than 1 in 10,000 is widely reported, but it is not supported by evidence.[5] However, a 2019 review of more recent and international studies indicates prevalence of at least 4% amongst music students.[6]
Generally, absolute pitch implies some or all of the following abilities, achieved without a reference tone:[7]
- Identify by name individual pitches (e.g. F♯, A, G, C) played on various instruments.
- Name the key of a given piece of tonal music.
- Identify and name all the tones of a given chord or other tonal mass.
- Accurately sing a named pitch.
- Name the pitches of common everyday sounds such as car horns and alarms.
People may have absolute pitch along with the ability of relative pitch, and relative and absolute pitch work together in actual musical listening and practice, but strategies in using each skill vary.[8]
Adults who possess relative pitch but do not already have absolute pitch can learn "pseudo-absolute pitch" and become able to identify notes in a way that superficially resembles absolute pitch.[9] Certain people who train to name notes may indeed become able to identify all 12 notes of the scale with 90% accuracy or above,[10] and valproate, a medication used to treat epilepsy and severe depression, may re-open the "critical period" of learning, making the acquisition of absolute pitch, as well as languages, potentially as efficient for adults as for children.[11][12] Even so, this form of pitch training can require considerable motivation, time, and effort, and it is not retained without constant practice and reinforcement.[13]
Scientific studies
History of study and terminologies
Scientific study of absolute pitch appears to have commenced in the 19th century, focusing on the phenomenon of musical pitch and methods of measuring it.[14] It would have been difficult for any notion of absolute pitch to have formed earlier because pitch references were not consistent. For example, the note now known as 'A' varied in different local or national musical traditions between what would now be considered as G sharp and B flat before the standardisation of the late 19th century. While the term absolute pitch, or absolute ear, was in use by the late 19th century by both British[15] and German researchers,[16] its application was not universal; other terms such as musical ear,[14] absolute tone consciousness,[17] or positive pitch[18] were also used to refer to the ability. The skill is not exclusively musical, or limited to human perception; absolute pitch has been demonstrated in animals such as bats, wolves, gerbils, and birds, for whom specific pitches facilitate identification of mates or meals.[19]
Difference in cognition, not elementary sensation
Physically and functionally, the auditory system of an absolute listener does not appear to be different from that of a non-absolute listener.[20] Rather, "it reflects a particular ability to analyze frequency information, presumably involving high-level cortical processing."[21] Absolute pitch is an act of cognition, needing memory of the frequency, a label for the frequency (such as "B-flat"), and exposure to the range of sound encompassed by that categorical label. Absolute pitch may be directly analogous to recognizing colors, phonemes (speech sounds), or other categorical perception of sensory stimuli. Just as most people have learned to recognize and name the color blue by the range of frequencies of the electromagnetic radiation that are perceived as light, it is possible that those who have been exposed to musical notes together with their names early in life will be more likely to identify, for example, the note C.[22] Although it was once thought that it "might be nothing more than a general human capacity whose expression is strongly biased by the level and type of exposure to music that people experience in a given culture",[23] absolute pitch may have contributions from genetic variations, possibly an autosomal dominant genetic trait.[24][25][26][27][28]
Influence by music experience
Absolute pitch sense appears to be influenced by cultural exposure to music, especially in the familiarization of the equal-tempered C-major scale. Most of the absolute listeners that were tested in this respect identified the C-major tones more reliably and, except for B, more quickly than the five "black key" tones,[29] which corresponds to the higher prevalence of these tones in ordinary musical experience. One study of Dutch non-musicians also demonstrated a bias toward using C-major tones in ordinary speech, especially on syllables related to emphasis.[30]
Linguistics
Absolute pitch is more common among speakers of tonal languages, such as most dialects of Chinese or Vietnamese, which often depend on pitch variation as the means of distinguishing words that otherwise sound the same—e.g., Mandarin with four possible tonal variations, Cantonese with six, Southern Min with seven or eight (depending on dialect), and Vietnamese with six.[31][32] Speakers of Sino-Tibetan languages have been reported to speak a word in the same absolute pitch (within a quarter-tone) on different days; it has therefore been suggested that absolute pitch may be acquired by infants when they learn to speak a tonal language[33] (and possibly also by infants when they learn to speak a pitch-accent language). However, the brains of tonal-language speakers do not naturally process musical sound as language;[34] perhaps such speakers are more likely to acquire absolute pitch for musical tones when they later receive musical training. Many native speakers of a tone language, even those with little musical training, are observed to sing a given song consistently with regard to pitch. Among music students of East Asian ethnic heritage, those who speak a tone language very fluently have a much higher prevalence of absolute pitch than those who do not speak a tone language.[35][36][37]
It is possible that African level-tone languages—such as Yoruba,[38] with three pitch levels, and Mambila,[39] with four—may be better suited to study the role of absolute pitch in speech than the pitch and contour tone languages of East Asia.
Speakers of European languages have been found to make subconscious use of an absolute pitch memory when speaking.[40]
Perception
Absolute pitch is the ability to perceive pitch class and to mentally categorize sounds according to perceived pitch class.[41] A pitch class is the set of all pitches that are a whole number of octaves apart. While the boundaries of musical pitch categories vary among human cultures, the recognition of octave relationships is a natural characteristic of the mammalian auditory system.[42][43][44][45][46][47] Accordingly, absolute pitch is not the ability to estimate a pitch value from the dimension of pitch-evoking frequency (30–5000 Hz),[22] but to identify a pitch class category within the dimension of pitch class (e.g., C-C♯-D ... B-C).
An absolute listener's sense of hearing is typically no keener than that of a non-absolute ("normal") listener.[48] Absolute pitch does not depend upon a refined ability to perceive and discriminate gradations of sound frequencies,[49] but upon detecting and categorizing a subjective perceptual quality typically referred to as "chroma".[50][clarification needed] The two tasks— of identification (recognizing and naming a pitch) and discrimination (detecting changes or differences in rate of vibration)— are accomplished with different brain mechanisms.[51]
Special populations
The prevalence of absolute pitch is higher among those who are blind from birth as a result of optic nerve hypoplasia.
Absolute pitch is considerably more common among those whose early childhood was spent in East Asia.[52][53][54][55] This might seem to be a genetic difference;[56] however, people of East Asian ancestry who are reared in North America are significantly less likely to develop absolute pitch than those raised in East Asia,[55] so the difference is more probably explained by experience. The language that is spoken may be an important factor; many East Asians speak tonal languages such as Mandarin and Cantonese, while others (such as those in Japan and certain provinces of Korea) speak pitch-accent languages, and the prevalence of absolute pitch may be partly explained by exposure to pitches together with meaningful musical labels very early in life.[53][54][55][57]
Absolute pitch ability has higher prevalence among those with Williams syndrome[58] and those with an autism spectrum disorder, with rates as high as 30% claimed, stating that the rate among musicians in general is far lower.[clarification needed][59][60][61] A non-verbal piano-matching method resulted in a correlation of 97% between[clarification needed] autism and absolute pitch, with a 53% correlation in non-autistic observers[clarification needed].[62] However, the converse is not indicated by research which found no difference between those with AP and those without on measures of social and communication skills, which are core deficits in autistic spectrum disorders. Additionally, the AP group's autism-spectrum quotient was "way below clinical thresholds".[63]
Nature vs. nurture
Absolute pitch might be achievable by any human being during a critical period of auditory development,[64][65] after which period cognitive strategies favor global and relational processing. Proponents of the critical-period theory agree that the presence of absolute pitch ability is dependent on learning, but there is disagreement about whether training causes absolute skills to occur[66][67][68][69] or lack of training causes absolute perception to be overwhelmed and obliterated by relative perception of musical intervals.[70][71]
One or more genetic loci could affect absolute pitch ability, a predisposition for learning the ability or signal the likelihood of its spontaneous occurrence.[26][28][27]
Researchers have been trying to teach absolute pitch ability in laboratory settings for more than a century,[72] and various commercial absolute-pitch training courses have been offered to the public since the early 1900s.[73] In 2013, experimenters reported that adult men who took the antiseizure drug valproate (VPA) "learned to identify pitch significantly better than those taking placebo—evidence that VPA facilitated critical-period learning in the adult human brain".[74] However, no adult has ever been documented to have acquired absolute listening ability,[75] because all adults who have been formally tested after AP training have failed to demonstrate "an unqualified level of accuracy... comparable to that of AP possessors".[76]
Pitch memory related to musical context
While very few people have the ability to name a pitch with no external reference, pitch memory can be activated by repeated exposure.[77] People who are not skilled singers will often sing popular songs in the correct key,[78] and can usually recognize when TV themes have been shifted into the wrong key.[79] Members of the Venda culture in South Africa also sing familiar children's songs in the key in which the songs were learned.[80]
This phenomenon is apparently unrelated to musical training. The skill may be associated more closely with vocal production. Violin students learning the Suzuki method are required to memorize each composition in a fixed key and play it from memory on their instrument, but they are not required to sing. When tested, these students did not succeed in singing the memorized Suzuki songs in the original, fixed key.[81]
Possible problems
Musicians with absolute perception may experience difficulties which do not exist for other musicians. Because absolute listeners are capable of recognizing that a musical composition has been transposed from its original key, or that a pitch is being produced at a nonstandard frequency (either sharp or flat), a musician with absolute pitch may become distressed upon perceiving tones believed to be "wrong" or hearing a piece of music "in the wrong key". This can especially apply to Baroque music that is recorded in Baroque tuning (usually A = 415 Hz as opposed to 440 Hz, i.e., roughly a half step or semitone lower than standard concert pitch).[59] An absolute listener may also use absolute strategies for tasks which are more efficiently accomplished with relative strategies, such as transposition[82] or producing harmony for tones whose frequencies do not match standard equal temperament.[83] It is also possible for some musicians to have displaced absolute pitch, where all notes are slightly flat or slightly sharp of their respective pitch as defined by a given convention.[citation needed] This may arise from learning the pitch names from an instrument that was tuned to a concert pitch convention other than the one in use, e.g., A = 435 Hz (the Paris Opera convention of the late 19th and early 20th centuries) as opposed to the Anglo-American modern standard A = 440 Hz. When playing in groups with other musicians, this may lead to playing in a tonality that is slightly different from that of the rest of the group.[citation needed]
Synesthesia
Absolute pitch shows a genetic overlap with music-related and non-music-related synesthesia/ideasthesia.[28] They may associate certain notes or keys with different colors, enabling them to tell what any note or key is. In this study, about 20% of people with perfect pitch are also synesthetes.
Correlations
There is evidence of a higher rate of absolute pitch in the autistic population.[84]
Correlation with musical talent
Absolute pitch is not a prerequisite for skilled musical performance or composition. However, musicians with absolute pitch tend to perform better on musical transcription tasks (controlling for age of onset and amount of musical training) compared to those without absolute pitch.[85] It was previously argued that musicians with absolute pitch perform worse than those without absolute pitch on recognition of musical intervals,[86] however experiments on which this conclusion was based contained an artifact, and when this artifact was removed, absolute pitch possessors were found to perform better than nonpossessors on recognition of musical intervals.[87]
Notable cases
Owing to uncertainty in the historical record, it is often impossible to determine whether notable composers and musicians of the past had absolute pitch. Since absolute pitch is rare in European musical culture,[54] claims that any particular musician possessed it are difficult to evaluate. Among composers of the Baroque and Classical eras, evidence is available only for Mozart, who is documented to have demonstrated the ability at age 7.[88] Experts have only surmised that Beethoven had it, as indicated from some excerpts from his letters. By the 19th century, it became more common for the presence of absolute pitch to be recorded, identifying the ability to be present in musicians such as Camille Saint-Saëns and John Philip Sousa.[89]
Notable recent musicians with absolute pitch include Charlie Puth,[90][91] Kofi Burbridge,[92] Eddy Chen of TwoSet Violin,[93] and Dahyun of Korean girl group TWICE.[94]
See also
References
- ^ Deutsch, D. (2013). "Absolute pitch" (PDF). In D. Deutsch (ed.). The Psychology of Music (3rd ed.). pp. 141–182. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-381460-9.00005-5. ISBN 9780123814609.
- ^ Ward, W.D. (1998). "Absolute Pitch". In D. Deutsch (ed.). The Psychology of Music (Second ed.). San Diego: Academic Press. pp. 265–298. ISBN 0-12-213564-4.
- ^ Zatorre, Robert; Beckett, Christine (1989). "Multiple coding strategies in the retention of musical tones by possessors of absolute pitch". Memory & Cognition. 17 (5): 582–589. doi:10.3758/BF03197081. PMID 2796743.
- ^ Zatorre, Robert (July 2003). "Absolute pitch: a model for understanding the influence of genes and development on neural and cognitive function". Nature Neuroscience. 6 (7): 692–695. doi:10.1038/nn1085. PMID 12830161.
- ^ Bachem, A. (November 1955). "Absolute Pitch". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 27 (6): 1180. Bibcode:1955ASAJ...27.1180B. doi:10.1121/1.1908155.
- ^ Carden, Jill; Cline, Tony (July 9, 2019). "Absolute pitch: Myths, evidence and relevance to music education and performance". Psychology of Music. 47 (6): 890–901. doi:10.1177/0305735619856098. ISSN 0305-7356.
- ^ Parncutt, R.; Levitin, D. J. (2001). "Absolute Pitch". In Sadie, S. (ed.). The New Grove Dictionary of Music and Musicians. London: Macmillan. ISBN 1-56159-239-0.
- ^ Miyazaki, Ken'ichi (June 2004). "How well do we understand absolute pitch?" (PDF). Acoustical Science and Technology. 25 (6): 270–282. doi:10.1250/ast.25.426. Archived from the original (PDF) on August 16, 2007.
- ^ Levitin, D.J. (2008). "Absolute pitch: Both a curse and a blessing". In Clockars, M.; Peltomaa, M. (eds.). Music Meets Medicine, Proceedings of the Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation. Helsinki, Finland: Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation. pp. 124–132.
- ^ Wong, Alan C.-N.; Yip, Ken H. M.; Lui, Kelvin F. H.; Wong, Yetta Kwailing (January 28, 2019). "Is it impossible to acquire absolute pitch in adulthood?". bioRxiv 10.1101/355933.
- ^ Jann Ingmire (May 28, 2015). "Acquiring 'perfect' pitch may be possible for some adults". University of Chicago News.
- ^ Gervain J, Vines BW, Chen LM, Seo RJ, Hensch TK, Werker JF, Young AH (2013). "Valproate reopens critical-period learning of absolute pitch". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 7 (102): 1–11. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00102. PMC 3848041. PMID 24348349.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Meyer, M (1956). "On memorizing absolute pitch". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 28 (4): 718–719. Bibcode:1956ASAJ...28..718M. doi:10.1121/1.1908465.
- ^ a b Ellis, Alexander J (November 6, 1876). "On the Sensitiveness of the Ear to Pitch and Change of Pitch in Music" (PDF). Proceedings of the Musical Association. 3 (1): 1–32. doi:10.1093/jrma/3.1.1. Retrieved August 24, 2010.
- ^ Bosanquet, R.H.M (1876). "An Elementary Treaties on Musical Intervals and Temperament". with an Account of an Enharmonic Harmonium Exhibited in the Loan Collection of Scientific Instruments. London: Macmillan and Co: xiv (n17 in electronic page field). Retrieved August 24, 2010.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ von Kries, J (1892). "Über das absolute Gehör" [About the perfect pitch]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie (in German). 3: 257–79. Retrieved August 24, 2010. Translation by Christopher Aruffo, www.acousticlearning.com
- ^ Abraham, O (1907). "Das absolute Tonbewußtsein und die Musik" [Absolute tone consciousness and music]. Sammelbände der Internationalen Musikgesellschaft (in German). 8: 486–91. Retrieved August 25, 2010. Translation by Christopher Aruffo, www.acousticlearning.com
- ^ Copp, E.F. (1916). "Musical Ability". Journal of Heredity. 7 (7): 297–305. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a110728.
- ^ Brown, Kathryn (December 4, 1999). "Striking the Right Note". Nature. 164 (2215): 38–41. Bibcode:1999Natur.400..308L. doi:10.1038/22411.
- ^ Sergeant, D. (1969). "Experimental investigation of absolute pitch". Journal of Research in Music Education. 17 (1). Journal of Research in Music Education, Vol. 17, No. 1: 135–143. doi:10.2307/3344200. JSTOR 3344200.
- ^ Gregersen, P. K. (1998). "Instant Recognition: The Genetics of Pitch Perception". The American Journal of Human Genetics. 62 (2): 221–223. doi:10.1086/301734. PMC 1376907. PMID 9463341.
- ^ a b Takeuchi, A. H.; Hulse, S. H. (1993). "Absolute pitch". Psychological Bulletin. 113 (2): 345–361. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.345. PMID 8451339.
- ^ Wallin, N.L.; Merker, B.; Brown, S. (2000). The Origins of Music. A. Bradford. p. 13. ISBN 9780262731430. Retrieved August 16, 2015.
- ^ Profita, J.; Bidder, T. G. (1988). "Perfect pitch". American Journal of Medical Genetics. 29 (4): 763–771. doi:10.1002/ajmg.1320290405. PMID 3400722.
- ^ Baharloo, S.; Johnston, P. A.; Service, S. K.; Gitschier, J.; Freimer, N. B. (1998). "Absolute pitch: An approach for identification of genetic and nongenetic components" (PDF). The American Journal of Human Genetics. 62 (2): 224–231. doi:10.1086/301704. PMC 1376881. PMID 9463312.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Theusch, E.; Basu, A.; Gitschier, J. (2009). "Genome-wide Study of Families with Absolute Pitch Reveals Linkage to 8q24.21 and Locus Heterogeneity". The American Journal of Human Genetics. 85 (1): 112–119. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.06.010. PMC 2706961. PMID 19576568.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Drayna, D.; Manichaikul, A.; DeLange, M.; Snieder, H.; Spector, T. (2001). "Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in humans". Science. 291 (5510): 1969–1972. Bibcode:2001Sci...291.1969D. doi:10.1126/science.291.5510.1969. PMID 11239158.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b c Gregersen, P.K.; Kowalsky, E.; Lee, A.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Fisher, S.E.; Asher, J.E.; Ballard, D.; Freudenberg, J.; Li, W. (2013). "Absolute pitch exhibits phenotypic and genetic overlap with synesthesia". Human Molecular Genetics. 22 (10): 2097–104. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddt059. PMC 4707203. PMID 23406871.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Miyazaki, K. (1990). "The speed of musical pitch identification by absolute-pitch possessors". Music Perception. 8 (2): 177–188. doi:10.2307/40285495. JSTOR 40285495.
- ^ Braun, M. (2002). "Absolute pitch in emphasized speech". Acoustics Research Letters Online. 3 (2): 77–82. doi:10.1121/1.1472336.
- ^ Deutsch, D.; Henthorn, T.; Dolson, M. (1999). "Tone Language Speakers Possess Absolute Pitch". Acoustical Society of America Journal. 106 (4): 2267. Bibcode:1999ASAJ..106.2267D. doi:10.1121/1.427738.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|lay-url=
ignored (help); Unknown parameter|laysource=
ignored (help) - ^ Deutsch, D.; Henthorn, T.; Marvin, E.; Xu, H. (2005). "Perfect Pitch in Tone Language Speakers Carries Over to Music". Acoustical Society of America Journal. 116 (4): 2580. Bibcode:2004ASAJ..116.2580D. doi:10.1121/1.4808626.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|lay-url=
ignored (help); Unknown parameter|laysource=
ignored (help) - ^ Deutsch, D.; Henthorn T.; Dolson, M. (2004). "Absolute pitch, speech, and tone language: Some experiments and a proposed framework" (PDF). Music Perception. 21 (3): 339–356. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.339.
- ^ Gandour, J; Wong, D.; Hutchins, G. (1998). "Pitch processing in the human brain is influenced by language experience" (PDF). NeuroReport. 9 (9): 2115–2119. doi:10.1097/00001756-199806220-00038. PMID 9674604. Archived from the original (PDF) on September 12, 2006.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Deutsch, D.; Henthorn, T.; Marvin, E.; Xu H-S (2006). "Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences, and evidence for a speech-related critical period" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 119 (2): 719–722. Bibcode:2006ASAJ..119..719D. doi:10.1121/1.2151799. PMID 16521731.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Deutsch, D.; Dooley, K.; Henthorn, T.; Head, B. (2009). "Absolute pitch among students in an American music conservatory: Association with tone language fluency" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 125 (4): 2398–2403. Bibcode:2009ASAJ..125.2398D. doi:10.1121/1.3081389. PMID 19354413.
- ^ Deutsch, D.; Li, X.; Shen, J. (2013). "Absolute pitch among students at the Shanghai Conservatory of Music: A large-scale direct-test study" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 134 (5): 3853–3859. Bibcode:2013ASAJ..134.3853D. doi:10.1121/1.4824450. PMID 24180794.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Connell, B.; Ladd, D. R. (1990). "Aspects of pitch realization in Yoruba". Phonology. 7: 1–29. doi:10.1017/S095267570000110X.
- ^ Connell, B. (2000). "The perception of lexical tone in Mambila". Language and Speech. 43 (2): 163–182. doi:10.1177/00238309000430020201. PMID 11064955.
- ^ Braun, M. (2001). "Speech mirrors norm-tones: Absolute pitch as a normal but precognitive trait" (PDF). Acoustics Research Letters Online. 2 (3): 85–90. doi:10.1121/1.1376728.[permanent dead link]
- ^ Rakowski, A. (1993). "Categorical perception in absolute pitch". Archives of Acoustics Quarterly. 18: 515–523.
- ^ Morest, D.K. (1965). "The laminar structure of the medial geniculate body of the cat". Journal of Anatomy. 99: 143–160. PMC 1261468. PMID 14245341.
- ^ Cetas, J.S.; Price, R.O.; Crowe, J.J.; Velenovsky, D.S.; McMullen, N.T. (2003). "Dendritic orientation and laminar architecture in the rabbit auditory thalamus". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 458 (3): 307–317. doi:10.1002/cne.10595. PMID 12619083.
- ^ Imig, T.J.; Morel, A. (1985). "Tonotopic organization in ventral nucleus of medial geniculate body in the cat". Journal of Neurophysiology. 53 (1): 309–340. doi:10.1152/jn.1985.53.1.309. PMID 3973661.
- ^ Cetas, J.S.; Velenovsky, D.S.; Price, R.O.; Sinex, D.G.; McMullen, N.T. (2001). "Frequency organization and cellular lamination in the medial geniculate body of the rabbit". Hearing Research. 155 (1–2): 113–123. doi:10.1016/S0378-5955(01)00257-X. PMID 11335081.
- ^ Wright, A.A.; Rivera, J.J.; Hulse, S.H.; Shyan, M.; Neiworth, J.J. (2000). "Music perception and octave generalization in rhesus monkeys" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 129 (3): 291–307. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.291. PMID 11006902.
- ^ Braun, M.; Chaloupka, V. (2005). "Carbamazepine induced pitch shift and octave space representation". Hearing Research. 210 (1–2): 85–92. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2005.05.015. PMID 16181754.
- ^ Fujisaki, W.; Kashino, M. (2002). "The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors". Acoustical Science and Technology. 23 (2): 77–83. doi:10.1250/ast.23.77.
- ^ Oakes, W. F. (1955). "An experimental study of pitch naming and pitch discrimination reactions". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 86 (2): 237–259. doi:10.1080/00221325.1955.10532197. PMID 13263528.
- ^ Bachem, A. (1937). "Various types of absolute pitch". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 9 (2): 146–151. Bibcode:1937ASAJ....9..146B. doi:10.1121/1.1915919.
- ^ Tervaniemi, M.; Alho, K.; Paavilainen, P.; Sams, M.; Näätänen, R. (1993). "Absolute pitch and event-related brain potentials". Music Perception. 10 (3): 305–316. doi:10.2307/40285572. JSTOR 40285572.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ P. K. Gregersen; E. Kowalsky; N. Kohn; E. Marvin (2000). "Early childhood music education and predisposition to absolute pitch". American Journal of Medical Genetics. 98 (3): 280–282. doi:10.1002/1096-8628(20010122)98:3<280::AID-AJMG1083>3.0.CO;2-6. PMID 11169569.
- ^ a b D. Deutsch; T. Henthorn; E. W. Marvin; H.-S. Xu (2006). "Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences, and evidence for a speech-related critical period" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 119 (2): 719–722. Bibcode:2006ASAJ..119..719D. doi:10.1121/1.2151799. PMID 16521731.
- ^ a b c Deutsch, D (2006). "The enigma of absolute pitch" (PDF). Acoustics Today. 2 (4): 11–19. doi:10.1121/1.2961141.
- ^ a b c Henthorn, T.; Deutsch, D. (2007). "Ethnicity versus Early Environment: Comment on 'Early childhood music education and predisposition to absolute pitch: Teasing apart genes and environment' by Peter K. Gregersen, Elena Kowalsky, Nina Kohn, and Elizabeth West Marvin (2000)" (PDF). American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A. 143A (1): 102–103. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.31596. PMID 17163519.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Zatorre, R. (2003). "Absolute Pitch: A model for understanding the influence of genes and development on neural and cognitive function" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience. 6 (7): 692–5. doi:10.1038/nn1085. PMID 12830161. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 7, 2008. Retrieved July 22, 2007.
- ^ Deutsch, D.; Henthorn, T.; Dolson, M. (2004). "Absolute pitch, speech, and tone language: Some experiments and a proposed framework" (PDF). Music Perception. 21 (3): 339–356. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.339.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Lenhoff, H. M.; Perales, O.; Hickok, G. (2001). "Absolute pitch in Williams syndrome". Music Perception. 18 (4): 491–503. doi:10.1525/mp.2001.18.4.491.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Sacks, O. (2007). Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain. New York: Knopf. ISBN 978-1-4000-4081-0.
- ^ Heaton, P.; Hermelin, B.; Pring, L. (1998). "Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability". Music Perception. 15 (3): 291–305. doi:10.2307/40285769. JSTOR 40285769.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Frith, U. How Cognitive Theories Can Help Us Explain Autism. Speech. UC Davis Mind Institute. Video available, 4/5 down the list: http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/videos/video_autism.html Archived November 4, 2011, at the Wayback Machine
- ^ Kupferstein, H.; Walsh, B. (2014). "Non-Verbal Paradigm for Assessing Individuals for Absolute Pitch". World Futures. 72 (7–8): 390–405. doi:10.1080/02604027.2014.989780.
- ^ Dohn, Anders; Garza-Villarreal, Eduardo A.; Heaton, Pamela; Vuust, Peter (May 30, 2012). Krueger, Frank (ed.). "Do Musicians with Perfect Pitch Have More Autism Traits than Musicians without Perfect Pitch? An Empirical Study". PLOS ONE. 7 (5): e37961. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...737961D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037961. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3364198. PMID 22666425.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Sakakibara, A. (2004). "Why Are People Able to Acquire Absolute Pitch Only During Early Childhood?". The Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology. 52 (4): 485–496. doi:10.5926/jjep1953.52.4_485.
- ^ Chin, C. (2003). "The Development of Absolute Pitch: A Theory Concerning the Roles of Music Training at an Early Developmental Age and Individual Cognitive Style". Psychology of Music. 31 (2): 155–171. doi:10.1177/0305735603031002292.
- ^ Oura, Y.; Eguchi, K. (1982). "Absolute pitch training program for children". Music Education Research. 32: 162–171.
- ^ Sakakibara, A. (1999). "A longitudinal study of a process for acquiring absolute pitch". The Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology. 47: 19–27. doi:10.5926/jjep1953.47.1_19.
- ^ Miyazaki, K.; Ogawa, Yoko (2006). "Learning of absolute pitch by children". Music Perception. 24 (1): 63. doi:10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.63.
- ^ Lau, C.K. (2004). "The acquisition of absolute pitch for the mainstreamed, special educational needs and academically talented under Lau Chiu Kay Music Educatherapy". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 116 (4): 2580. Bibcode:2004ASAJ..116.2580L. doi:10.1121/1.4785301.[1][permanent dead link] Abstract
- ^ Abraham, O. (1901). "Das absolute tonbewußtsein". Sammelbände der Internationalen Musikgesellschaft. 3: 1–86.Full text Full text (English)
- ^ Saffran, J. R.; Griepentrog, G. J. (2001). "Absolute pitch in infant auditory learning: Evidence for developmental reorganization" (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 37 (1): 74–85. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.1.74. PMID 11206435.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help)[permanent dead link] - ^ Meyer, M. (1899). "Is the memory of absolute pitch capable of development by training?". Psychological Review. 6 (5): 514–516. doi:10.1037/h0069034.
- ^ Maryon, E. (1924). The Science of Tone-Color (PDF). Boston: C. C. Birchard & Co. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 6, 2006. Retrieved September 5, 2006.
- ^ Gervain, Judit; Vines, Bradley W.; Chen, Lawrence M.; Seo, Rubo J.; Hensch, Takao K.; Werker, Janet F.; Young, Allan H. (2013). "Valproate reopens critical-period learning of absolute pitch". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 7. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00102. PMC 3848041. PMID 24348349.
- ^ Levitin, D. J.; Rogers, S. E. (2005). "Absolute pitch: Perception, coding, and controversies" (PDF). Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (1): 26–33. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.007. PMID 15639438. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 22, 2006. Retrieved June 11, 2006.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Takeuchi, A. H.; Hulse, S. H. (1993). "Absolute pitch". Psychological Bulletin. 113 (2): 345–61. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.345. PMID 8451339.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) - ^ Ben-Haim, Moshe Shay; Eitan, Zohar; Chajut, Eran (February 2014). "Pitch memory and exposure effects". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 40 (1): 24–32. doi:10.1037/a0033583. PMID 23875573.
- ^ Levitin, D. (1994). "Absolute memory for musical pitch: evidence from production of learned melodies" (PDF). Perception & Psychophysics. 56 (4): 414–423. doi:10.3758/BF03206733. PMID 7984397. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 6, 2011.
- ^ Schellenberg, E. Glenn; Trehub, Sandra E. (2003). "Good pitch memory is widespread". Psychological Science. 14 (3): 262–266. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.03432. PMID 12741751.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help)Full text[permanent dead link] - ^ Blacking, John (1995). "Music and Historical Process in Vendaland". In Reginald Byron (ed.). Music Culture and Experience. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p. 136. ISBN 0-226-08829-4.
- ^ Saah, Victoria; Marvin, Elizabeth West (2004). "Absolute memory of learned melodies in children trained by the Suzuki violin method" (PDF). Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition. pp. 736–739. Archived from the original (PDF) on August 10, 2011.
- ^ Miyazaki, K. (1993). "Absolute pitch as an inability: Identification of musical intervals in a tonal context". Music Perception. 11 (1): 55–72. doi:10.2307/40285599. JSTOR 40285599.
- ^ Harris, G. B. (1974). Categorical perception and absolute pitch. Ontario: University of Western Ontario.
- ^ Mottron, Laurent; Bouvet, Lucie; Bonnel, Anna; Samson, Fabienne; Burack, Jacob A.; Dawson, Michelle; Heaton, Pamela (2013). "Veridical mapping in the development of exceptional autistic abilities". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 37 (2): 209–228. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.016. ISSN 1873-7528. PMID 23219745.
- ^ Dooley, K.; Deutsch, D. (2010). "Absolute pitch correlates with high performance on musical dictation". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 128 (2): 890–3. Bibcode:2010ASAJ..128..890D. doi:10.1121/1.3458848. PMID 20707458. S2CID 13958792.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|last-author-amp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help) PDF Document - ^ Miyazaki K. (1995). "Perception of relative pitch with different references: Some absolute-pitch listeners can't tell musical interval names". Perception & Psychophysics. 57 (7): 962–970. doi:10.3758/bf03205455. PMID 8532499. PDF Document
- ^ Dooley, K.; Deutsch, D. (2011). "Absolute pitch correlates with high performance on interval naming tasks". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 130 (6): 4097–4104. Bibcode:2011ASAJ..130.4097D. doi:10.1121/1.3652861. PMID 22225064. S2CID 2840110. PDF Document
- ^ Deutsch, D. (2006). "The Enigma of Absolute Pitch". Acoustics Today. 2 (4): 11–18. doi:10.1121/1.2961141. S2CID 110140289. PDF Document
- ^ Naxos. (2018). "John Philip Sousa." Retrieved from https://www.naxos.com/person/John_Philip_Sousa_24864/24864.htm
- ^ "Charlie Puth, Rumson's Pop Prodigy". New Jersey Monthly. January 12, 2016. Retrieved April 10, 2019.
- ^ "Perfectly Pitched For Success – Charlie Puth | eDirectory". livinginmedia.com. June 1, 2015. Archived from the original on February 22, 2017. Retrieved April 10, 2019.
- ^ Alan Bryson (May 29, 2019). "Remembering Kofi Burbridge: 1961–2019". All About Jazz.
- ^ "5 Annoying Things People with Perfect Pitch Do". youtube.com. Retrieved January 11, 2020.
- ^ "[V Report Plus] Twice's Dahyun boasts perfect pitch". The Korea Herald. April 6, 2018. Retrieved April 28, 2020.
External links
- Comprehensive historical bibliography of absolute pitch research, 1876–present
- Another bibliography of absolute pitch, with +300 papers