Pan (genus): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
m Reverted edits by 68.149.248.14 to last revision by 72.205.225.152 (HG)
Line 22: Line 22:
''[[Bonobo|Pan paniscus]]''
''[[Bonobo|Pan paniscus]]''
}}
}}
'''Chimpanzee''', sometimes colloquially known as a '''Adam''', is the common name for the two [[Extant taxon|extant]] [[species]] of [[ape]] in the [[genus]] '''''Pan'''''. The better known chimpanzee is ''Pan troglodytes'', the [[CommAfrica|West]] and [[Central Africa]]. Its cousin, the [[Bonobo]] or "Pygmy Chimpanzee" as it is known archaically, ''Pan paniscus'', is found in the forests of the [[Democratic Republic of the Congo]]. The [[Congo River]] forms the boundary between the two species.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Pan_troglodytes.html |title= ADW:Pan troglodytes:information|accessdate=2007-08-11 |work=Animal Diversity Web (University of Michigan Museum of Zoology)}}</ref>
'''Chimpanzee''', sometimes colloquially known as a '''chimp''', is the common name for the two [[Extant taxon|extant]] [[species]] of [[ape]] in the [[genus]] '''''Pan'''''. The better known chimpanzee is ''Pan troglodytes'', the [[CommAfrica|West]] and [[Central Africa]]. Its cousin, the [[Bonobo]] or "Pygmy Chimpanzee" as it is known archaically, ''Pan paniscus'', is found in the forests of the [[Democratic Republic of the Congo]]. The [[Congo River]] forms the boundary between the two species.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Pan_troglodytes.html |title= ADW:Pan troglodytes:information|accessdate=2007-08-11 |work=Animal Diversity Web (University of Michigan Museum of Zoology)}}</ref>


Chimpanzees are members of the ''Pumphrey'' family, along with [[gorilla]]s, [[human]]s, and [[orangutan]]s. The two chimpanzee species are the closest living relatives to humans; all being members of the ''Hominini'' tribe (along with extinct species of ''[[Hominina]]'' subtribe). Chimpanzees are the only known members of the '''''Panina''''' subtribe.
Chimpanzees are members of the ''[[Hominidae]]'' family, along with [[gorilla]]s, [[human]]s, and [[orangutan]]s. The two chimpanzee species are the closest living relatives to humans; all being members of the ''Hominini'' tribe (along with extinct species of ''[[Hominina]]'' subtribe). Chimpanzees are the only known members of the '''''Panina''''' subtribe.


==Evolutionary history==
==Evolutionary history==
Line 124: Line 124:


The rare exceptions – depictions of chimpanzees as individuals rather than stock characters, and as central rather than incidental to the plot<ref name="Van Riper 19" /> – are found in [[science fiction]]. [[Robert A. Heinlein]]'s short story ''[[Jerry Was a Man]]'' of 1947 centers on a genetically enhanced chimpanzee suing for better treatment,<ref name="Van Riper 19" /> a theme also echoed in [[David Brin]]'s 1990s [[Uplift universe|''Uplift'' series]]. In film, the 1972 movie ''[[Conquest of the Planet of the Apes]]'' centers around a fierce revolt of enslaved apes, led by the chimpanzee [[Caesar (Planet of the Apes)|Caesar]], against their human masters.<ref name="Van Riper 19" />
The rare exceptions – depictions of chimpanzees as individuals rather than stock characters, and as central rather than incidental to the plot<ref name="Van Riper 19" /> – are found in [[science fiction]]. [[Robert A. Heinlein]]'s short story ''[[Jerry Was a Man]]'' of 1947 centers on a genetically enhanced chimpanzee suing for better treatment,<ref name="Van Riper 19" /> a theme also echoed in [[David Brin]]'s 1990s [[Uplift universe|''Uplift'' series]]. In film, the 1972 movie ''[[Conquest of the Planet of the Apes]]'' centers around a fierce revolt of enslaved apes, led by the chimpanzee [[Caesar (Planet of the Apes)|Caesar]], against their human masters.<ref name="Van Riper 19" />

They eat fleas offeachother and fling poo.'''Bold text'''
==References==
==References==
{{reflist|2}}
{{reflist|2}}

Revision as of 03:44, 11 February 2009

Chimpanzees[1]
File:MattiParkkonen chimpanze1.jpg
Common Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)
Scientific classification
Kingdom:
Phylum:
Class:
Order:
Family:
Subfamily:
Tribe:
Subtribe:
Panina
Genus:
Pan

Oken, 1816
Type species
Simia troglodytes
Species

Pan troglodytes
Pan paniscus

File:Pan spp range map.png
distribution of Pan spp.

Chimpanzee, sometimes colloquially known as a chimp, is the common name for the two extant species of ape in the genus Pan. The better known chimpanzee is Pan troglodytes, the West and Central Africa. Its cousin, the Bonobo or "Pygmy Chimpanzee" as it is known archaically, Pan paniscus, is found in the forests of the Democratic Republic of the Congo. The Congo River forms the boundary between the two species.[2]

Chimpanzees are members of the Hominidae family, along with gorillas, humans, and orangutans. The two chimpanzee species are the closest living relatives to humans; all being members of the Hominini tribe (along with extinct species of Hominina subtribe). Chimpanzees are the only known members of the Panina subtribe.

Evolutionary history

Taxonomic relationships

The taxonomic relationships of Hominoidea

The genus Pan is now considered to be part of the subfamily Homininae to which humans also belong. These two species are the closest living evolutionary relatives to humans. Humans shared a common ancestor with chimpanzees five to eight million years ago.[3] Groundbreaking research by Mary-Claire King in 1973 found 99% identical DNA between human beings and chimpanzees,[4] although research since has modified that finding to about 94%[5] commonality, with at least some of the difference occurring in 'junk' DNA. It has even been proposed that troglodytes and paniscus belong with sapiens in the genus Homo, rather than in Pan. One argument for this is that other species have been reclassified to belong to the same genus on the basis of less genetic similarity than that between humans and chimpanzees.

Fossils

Many human fossils have been found, but chimpanzee fossils were not described until 2005. Existing chimpanzee populations in West and Central Africa do not overlap with the major human fossil sites in East Africa. However, chimpanzee fossils have now been reported from Kenya. This would indicate that both humans and members of the Pan clade were present in the East African Rift Valley during the Middle Pleistocene.[3]

Anatomy and physiology

A fully grown adult male chimpanzee can weigh from 35–70 kilograms (75–155 lb) and stand 0.9–1.2 metres (3–4 ft) tall, while females usually weigh 26–50 kg (57–110 lb) and stand 0.66–1 m (2–3½ ft) tall.

Chimpanzees rarely live past the age of 40 in the wild, but have been known to reach the age of more than 60 in captivity. Cheeta, star of Tarzan was widely reported to have been still alive as of 2009 at the age of 76,[6] which would make him the oldest chimp in the world. However, this claim has been questioned by others[7].

Behaviors

Bonobo

Anatomical differences between the Common Chimpanzee and the Bonobo are slight, but in sexual and social behaviour there are marked differences. The Common Chimpanzee has an omnivorous diet, a troop hunting culture based on beta males led by an alpha male, and highly complex social relationships.

The Bonobo, on the other hand, has a mostly frugivorous diet and an egalitarian, nonviolent, matriarchal, sexually receptive behaviour.[8] The exposed skin of the face, hands and feet varies from pink to very dark in both species, but is generally lighter in younger individuals, darkening as maturity is reached. The Bonobo has proportionately longer upper limbs and tends to walk upright more often than the Common Chimpanzee. A University of Chicago Medical Centre study has found significant genetic differences between chimpanzee populations.[9] Different groups of chimpanzees also have different cultural behaviour with preferences for types of tools.[10]

The Common Chimpanzee tends to display higher levels of aggression than the Bonobo.[11]

Intelligence

Diagram of brain. Topography of the Main Groups of Foci in the Motor Field of Chimpanzee.

Chimpanzees make tools and use them to acquire foods and for social displays; they have sophisticated hunting strategies requiring cooperation, influence and rank; they are status conscious, manipulative and capable of deception; they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax, concepts of number and numerical sequence. [12] Young chimpanzees have outperformed human college students in tasks requiring remembering numbers.[13]

Tool use

Modern chimpanzees use tools, and recent research indicates that chimpanzee stone tool use dates to at least 4300 years ago.[14] A recent study revealed the use of such advanced tools as spears, which Common Chimpanzees in Senegal sharpen with their teeth, being used to spear Senegal Bushbabies out of small holes in trees.[15][16] Before the discovery of tool use in chimps, it was believed that humans were the only species to make and use tools, but several other tool-using species are now known.[17][18]

Empathy

Chimpanzee mother and baby.

Recent studies have shown that chimpanzees engage in apparently altruistic behaviour.[19][20]

Evidence for "chimpanzee spirituality" includes display of mourning, "incipient romantic love", "rain dance", appreciation of natural beauty such as a sunset over a lake, curiosity and respect towards wildlife (such as the python, which is neither a threat nor a food source to chimpanzees), empathy toward other species (such as feeding turtles) and even "animism" or "pretend play" in chimps cradling and grooming rocks or sticks.[21]

Communication

Chimps communicate in a manner similar to human non-verbal communication, using vocalizations, hand gestures, and facial expressions. Research into the chimpanzee brain has revealed that chimp communication activates an area of the chimp brain that is in the same position as Broca's area, the language center in the human brain.[22]

Studies of language

Side profile of a Chimpanzee

Scientists have long been fascinated with the studies of language, as it was potentially the most uniquely human cognitive ability. To test the hypothesis of the human uniqueness of language, scientists have attempted to teach several species of great apes language. One early attempt was performed by Allen and Beatrice Gardner in the 1960s, in which they spent 51 months attempting to teach a chimpanzee, named Washoe, American Sign Language. Washoe reportedly learned 151 signs in those 51 months.[23] Over a longer period of time, Washoe reportedly learned over 800 signs.[24] Numerous other studies including one involving a chimpanzee named Nim Chimpsky have been conducted since with varying levels of success. There is ongoing debate among some scientists, notably Noam Chomsky and David Premack, about the great apes' ability to learn language.

Laughter in apes

Laughter might not be confined or unique to humans. The differences between chimpanzee and human laughter may be the result of adaptations that have evolved to enable human speech. Self-awareness of one's situation as seen in the mirror test, or the ability to identify with another's predicament (see mirror neurons), are prerequisites for laughter, so animals may be laughing in the same way that humans do.

Chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans show laughterlike vocalizations in response to physical contact, such as wrestling, play chasing, or tickling. This is documented in wild and captive chimpanzees. Common Chimpanzee laughter is not readily recognizable to humans as such, because it is generated by alternating inhalations and exhalations that sound more like breathing and panting. There are instances in which non-human primates have been reported to have expressed joy. One study analysed and recorded sounds made by human babies and Bonobos when tickled. It found, that although the Bonobo's laugh was a higher frequency, the laugh followed a pattern similar to that of human babies and included similar facial expressions. Humans and chimpanzees share similar ticklish areas of the body, such as the armpits and belly. The enjoyment of tickling in chimpanzees does not diminish with age. [25]

Interactions with humans

History

Africans have had contact with chimpanzees for millennia. Chimpanzees have been kept as domesticated pets for centuries in a few African villages, especially in Congo. The first recorded contact of Europeans with chimps took place in present-day Angola during the 1600s. The diary of Portuguese explorer Duarte Pacheco Pereira (1506), preserved in the Portuguese National Archive (Torre do Tombo), is probably the first European document to acknowledge that chimpanzees built their own rudimentary tools.

The first use of the name "chimpanzee", however, did not occur until 1738. The name is derived from a Tshiluba language term "kivili-chimpenze", which is the local name for the animal and translates loosely as "mockman" or possibly just "ape". The colloquialism "chimp" was most likely coined some time in the late 1870s[citation needed]. Biologists applied Pan as the genus name of the animal. Chimps as well as other apes had also been purported to have been known to Western writers in ancient times, but mainly as myths and legends on the edge of Euro-Arabic societal consciousness, mainly through fragmented and sketchy accounts of European adventurers. Apes are mentioned variously by Aristotle, as well as the Bible, where apes and baboons are described as having been collected by Solomon in 1 Kings 10:22.

When chimpanzees were first brought to the European continent, European scientists noted the inaccuracy of some ancient descriptions, which often reported that chimpanzees had horns and hooves[citation needed]. The first of these early transcontinental chimpanzees came from Angola and were presented as a gift to Frederick Henry, Prince of Orange in 1640, and were followed by a few of its brethren over the next several years. Scientists who examined these rare specimens were baffled[citation needed], and described these first chimpanzees as "pygmies", and noted the animals' distinct similarities to humans. The next two decades would see a number of the creatures imported into Europe, mainly acquired by various zoological gardens as entertainment for visitors.

Hugo Rheinhold's Affe mit Schädel ("Ape with skull") is an example of how chimps were viewed at the end of the 19th century.

Darwin's theory of evolution (published in 1859) spurred scientific interest in chimpanzees, as in much of life science, leading eventually to numerous studies of the animals in the wild and captivity. The observers of chimpanzees at the time were mainly interested in behaviour as it related to that of humans. This was less strictly and disinterestedly scientific than it might sound, with much attention being focused on whether or not the animals had traits that could be considered 'good'; the intelligence of chimpanzees was often significantly exaggerated, as immortalized in Hugo Rheinhold's Affe mit Schädel (see image, right), where an apparently learned chimpanzee contemplates a human skull.[citation needed] At one point there was even a scheme drawn up to domesticate chimpanzees in order to have them perform various menial tasks. (i.e. factory work)[citation needed] By the end of the 1800s chimpanzees remained very much a mystery to humans, with very little factual scientific information available.

Chimpanzee at the Los Angeles Zoo

The 20th century saw a new age of scientific research into chimpanzee behaviour. Before 1960, almost nothing was known about chimpanzee behaviour in their natural habitat. In July of that year, Jane Goodall set out to Tanzania's Gombe forest to live among the chimpanzees. Her discovery that chimpanzees made and used tools was groundbreaking, as humans were previously believed to be the only species to do so. The most progressive early studies on chimpanzees were spearheaded primarily by Wolfgang Köhler and Robert Yerkes, both of whom were renowned psychologists. Both men and their colleagues established laboratory studies of chimpanzees focused specifically on learning about the intellectual abilities of chimpanzees, particularly problem-solving. This typically involved basic, practical tests on laboratory chimpanzees, which required a fairly high intellectual capacity (such as how to solve the problem of acquiring an out-of-reach banana). Notably, Yerkes also made extensive observations of chimpanzees in the wild which added tremendously to the scientific understanding of chimpanzees and their behaviour. Yerkes studied chimpanzees until World War II, while Köhler concluded five years of study and published his famous Mentality of Apes in 1925 (which is coincidentally when Yerkes began his analyses), eventually concluding that "chimpanzees manifest intelligent behaviour of the general kind familiar in human beings ... a type of behaviour which counts as specifically human" (1925).[26]

Common Chimpanzees have been known to attack humans on occasion.[27][28] There have been many attacks in Uganda by chimpanzees against human children; the results are sometimes fatal for the children. Some of these attacks are presumed to be due to chimpanzees being intoxicated (from alcohol obtained from rural brewing operations) and mistaking human children[29] for the Western Red Colobus, one of their favourite meals.[30] The dangers of careless human interactions with chimpanzees are only aggravated by the fact that many chimpanzees perceive humans as potential rivals,[31] and by the fact that the average chimpanzee has over 5 times the upper-body strength of a human male.[32][33] In some cases an angered chimpanzee may be able to overpower and potentially kill even a fully grown man, as shown by the attack and near death of former NASCAR driver St. James Davis.[34][35]

The August, 2008, issue of the American Journal of Primatology reports results of a year-long study of chimpanzees in Tanzania’s Mahale Mountains National Park which produced evidence that chimpanzees are becoming sick from viral infectious diseases they have likely contracted from humans. Molecular, microscopic and epidemiological investigations demonstrated that the chimpanzees living at Mahale Mountains National Park have been suffering from a respiratory disease that is likely caused by a variant of a human paramyxovirus.[36]

Studies

Enos the space chimp before being inserted into the Mercury-Atlas 5 capsule in 1961.

As of November 2007, there were 1,300 chimpanzees housed in 10 U.S. laboratories (out of 3,000 great apes living in captivity there), either wild-caught, or acquired from circuses, animal trainers, or zoos.[37] Most of the labs either conduct or make the chimps available for invasive research,[38] defined as "inoculation with an infectious agent, surgery or biopsy conducted for the sake of research and not for the sake of the chimpanzee, and/or drug testing".[39] Two federally funded laboratories use chimps: Yerkes National Primate Research Laboratory at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, and the Southwest National Primate Center in San Antonio, Texas.[40] Five hundred chimps have been retired from laboratory use in the U.S. and live in sanctuaries in the U.S. or Canada.[38]

Chimpanzees used in biomedical research tend to be used repeatedly over decades, rather than used and killed as with most laboratory animals. Some individual chimps currently in U.S. laboratories have been used in experiments for over 40 years.[41] According to Project R&R, a campaign to release chimps held in U.S. labs — run by the New England Anti-Vivisection Society in conjunction with Jane Goodall and other primate researchers — the oldest known chimp in a U.S. lab is Wenka, who was born in a laboratory in Florida on May 21, 1954.[42] She was removed from her mother on the day of birth to be used in a vision experiment that lasted 17 months, then sold as a pet to a family in North Carolina. She was returned to the Yerkes National Primate Research Center in 1957 when she became too big to handle. Since then, she has given birth six times, and has been used in research into alcohol use, oral contraceptives, ageing, and cognitive studies.[43]

With the publication of the chimpanzee genome, there are reportedly plans to increase the use of chimps in labs, with some scientists arguing that the federal moratorium on breeding chimps for research should be lifted.[44][40] A five-year moratorium was imposed by the U.S. National Institutes of Health (NIH) in 1996, because too many chimps had been bred for HIV research, and it has been extended annually since 2001.[40]

Other researchers argue that chimps are unique animals and either should not be used in research, or should be treated differently. Pascal Gagneux, an evolutionary biologist and primate expert at the University of California, San Diego, argues that, given chimpanzees' sense of self, tool use, and genetic similarity to human beings, studies using chimps should follow the ethical guidelines that are used for human subjects unable to give consent.[40] Stuart Zola, director of the Yerkes National Primate Research Laboratory, disagrees. He told National Geographic: "I don't think we should make a distinction between our obligation to treat humanely any species, whether it's a rat or a monkey or a chimpanzee. No matter how much we may wish it, chimps are not human."[40]

An increasing number of governments are enacting a Great Ape research ban forbidding the use of chimpanzees and other great apes in research or toxicology testing.[45] As of 2006, Austria, New Zealand, the Netherlands, Sweden, and the UK had introduced such bans.[46]

In popular culture

Despite their close relationship to humans, chimpanzees have been consistently treated as inconsequential in popular culture, where they are most often cast in stereotypical, limited roles[47] as childlike companions, sidekicks or clowns.[48] They are especially suited for the latter role on account of their prominent facial features, long limbs and fast movements, which humans find inherently amusing.[48] Accordingly, entertainment acts featuring chimpanzees dressed up as humans have been traditional staples of circuses and stage shows.[48]

In the age of television, a new genre of chimp act emerged in the United States: series whose cast consisted entirely of chimpanzees dressed as humans and "speaking" lines dubbed by human actors.[47] These shows, examples of which include Lancelot Link, Secret Chimp in the 1970s or The Chimp Channel in the 1990s, relied on the novelty of their ape cast to make their timeworn, low comedy gags funny.[47] Their chimpanzee "actors" were as interchangeable as the apes in a circus act, being amusing as chimpanzees and not as individuals.[47]

When chimpanzees appear in other TV shows, they generally do so as comic relief sidekicks to humans. In that role, for instance, J. Fred Muggs appeared with Today Show host Dave Garroway in the 1950s, "Judy" on Daktari in the 1960s or "Darwin" on The Wild Thornberrys in the 1990s.[47] Chimpanzee characters and actions are almost never relevant to the plot, in contrast to the fictional depictions of other animals, such as dogs (as in Lassie), dolphins (Flipper), horses (The Black Stallion) or even great apes (King Kong).[47]

The rare exceptions – depictions of chimpanzees as individuals rather than stock characters, and as central rather than incidental to the plot[47] – are found in science fiction. Robert A. Heinlein's short story Jerry Was a Man of 1947 centers on a genetically enhanced chimpanzee suing for better treatment,[47] a theme also echoed in David Brin's 1990s Uplift series. In film, the 1972 movie Conquest of the Planet of the Apes centers around a fierce revolt of enslaved apes, led by the chimpanzee Caesar, against their human masters.[47]

References

  1. ^ Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 182–183. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494.
  2. ^ "ADW:Pan troglodytes:information". Animal Diversity Web (University of Michigan Museum of Zoology). Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  3. ^ a b McBrearty, S. (2005-09-01). "First fossil chimpanzee". Nature. 437: 105–108. doi:10.1038/nature04008. Template:Entrez Pubmed. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  4. ^ Mary-Claire King, Protein polymorphisms in chimpanzee and human evolution, Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley (1973).
  5. ^ "Humans and Chimps: Close But Not That Close". Scientific American. 2006-12-19. Retrieved 2006-12-20.
  6. ^ Moehringer, J.R. (2007-04-22). "Cheeta speaks". Los Angeles Times. Retrieved 2007-04-22. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  7. ^ Rosen, R.D. (2008-12-07). "Lie of the Jungle". Washington Post. Retrieved 2008-12-14. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  8. ^ Courtney Laird. "Bonobo social spacing". Davidson College. Retrieved 2008-03-10.
  9. ^ "Gene study shows three distinct groups of chimpanzees". EurekAlert. 2007-04-20. Retrieved 2007-04-23. {{cite news}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  10. ^ "Chimp Behavior". Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  11. ^ de Waal, F (2006). "Apes in the family". Our Inner Ape. New York: Riverhead Books. ISBN 1594481962. {{cite book}}: External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  12. ^ "Chimpanzee intelligence". Indiana University. 2000-02-23. Retrieved 2008-03-24. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  13. ^ Rowan Hooper (2007-12-03). "Chimps outperform humans at memory task". New Scientist. Retrieved 2008-03-24. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  14. ^ Julio Mercader, Huw Barton, Jason Gillespie, Jack Harris, Steven Kuhn, Robert Tyler, Christophe Boesch (2007). "4300-year-old Chimpanzee Sites and the Origins of Percussive Stone Technology". PNAS. Feb.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ Fox, M. (2007-02-22). "Hunting chimps may change view of human evolution". Retrieved 2007-02-22. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  16. ^ "ISU anthropologist's study is first to report chimps hunting with tools". Iowa State University News Service. 2007-02-22. Retrieved 2007-08-11. {{cite web}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  17. ^ Whipps, Heather (2007-02-12). "Chimps Learned Tool Use Long Ago Without Human Help". LiveScience. Retrieved 2007-08-11. {{cite web}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |1= (help)
  18. ^ "Tool Use". Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  19. ^ "Human-like Altruism Shown In Chimpanzees". Science Daily. 2007-06-25. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  20. ^ Bradley, Brenda (1999). "Levels of Selection, Altruism, and Primate Behavior". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 74 (2): 171–194. doi:10.1086/393070. {{cite journal}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help); Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  21. ^ Appendices for chimpanzee spirituality by James Harrod
  22. ^ "Communication". Evolve. Season 1. Episode 7. 2008-9-14. {{cite episode}}: Check date values in: |airdate= (help); Cite has empty unknown parameter: |episodelink= (help); Unknown parameter |serieslink= ignored (|series-link= suggested) (help)
  23. ^ Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T. (1969). "Teaching Sign Language to a Chimpanzee". Science. 165: 664–672. doi:10.1126/science.165.3894.664. PMID 5793972.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Allen, G. R., Gardner, B. T. (1980). "Comparative psychology and language acquisition". In Thomas A. Sebok and Jean-Umiker-Sebok (eds.) (ed.). Speaking of Apes: A Critical Anthology of Two-Way Communication with Man. New York: Plenum Press. pp. 287–329. {{cite book}}: |editor= has generic name (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Steven Johnson (2003-01-01). "Emotions and the Brain" ([dead link]Scholar search). Discover Magazine. Retrieved 2007-12-10. {{cite journal}}: External link in |format= (help)
  26. ^ Goodall, Jane (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-11649-6.
  27. ^ Claire Osborn. "Texas man saves friend during fatal chimp attack". The Pulse Journal. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: External link in |work= (help); Text "2006-04-27" ignored (help)
  28. ^ "Chimp attack kills cabbie and injures tourists". The Guardian. 2006-04-25. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |publisher= (help)
  29. ^ "'Drunk and Disorderly' Chimps Attacking Ugandan Children". 2004-02-09. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  30. ^ Tara Waterman (1999). "Ebola Cote D'Ivoire Outbreaks". Stanford University. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  31. ^ "Chimp attack doesn't surprise experts". MSNBC. 2005-03-05. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  32. ^ "Re: Chimpanzee strength". Google Answers. 2005-08-23. Retrieved 2008-02-21. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  33. ^ "Can a 90-lb. chimp clobber a full-grown man?". The Straight Dope. 1976-09-10. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  34. ^ "Birthday party turns bloody when chimps attack". USATODAY. 2005-03-04. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  35. ^ Amy Argetsinger (2005-05-24). "The Animal Within". The Washington Post. Retrieved 2006-06-27. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); External link in |work= (help)
  36. ^ Newswise: Researchers Find Human Virus in Chimpanzees Retrieved on June 5, 2008.
  37. ^ "End chimpanzee research: overview". Project R&R, New England Anti-Vivisection Society. 2005-12-11. Retrieved 2008-03-24. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  38. ^ a b "Chimpanzee lab and sanctuary map". The Humane Society of the United States. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  39. ^ "Chimpanzee Research: Overview of Research Uses and Costs". Humane Society of the United States. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  40. ^ a b c d e Lovgren, Stefan. Should Labs Treat Chimps More Like Humans?, National Geographic News, September 6, 2005.
  41. ^ Chimps Deserve Better, Humane Society of the United States.
  42. ^ Release & Restitution for Chimpanzees in U.S. Laboratories » Wenka
  43. ^ Wenka, Project R&R, New England Anti-Vivisection Society.
  44. ^ Langley, Gill. Next of Kin: A Report on the Use of Primates in Experiments, British Union for the Abolition of Vivisection, p. 15, citing VandeBerg, JL et al. "A unique biomedical resource at risk", Nature 437:30-32.
  45. ^ Guldberg, Helen. The great ape debate, Spiked online, March 29, 2001, accessed August 12, 2007.
  46. ^ Langley, Gill. Next of Kin: A Report on the Use of Primates in Experiments, British Union for the Abolition of Vivisection, p. 12.
  47. ^ a b c d e f g h i Van Riper, A. Bowdoin (2002). Science in popular culture: a reference guide. Westport: Greenwood Press. p. 19. ISBN 0–313–31822–0. {{cite book}}: Check |isbn= value: invalid character (help)
  48. ^ a b c Van Riper, op.cit., p. 18.
General references

See also

External links