Neveʻei language
Neve’ei | |
---|---|
Vinmavis | |
Native to | Vanuatu |
Region | Central Malekula |
Ethnicity | spoken by 70% (no date)[1] |
Native speakers | 500 (2007)[2] |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | vnm |
Glottolog | vinm1237 |
ELP | Vinmavis |
Neve’ei (IPA: [neveʔei]), also known as Vinmavis, is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu.[M 1] There are around 500 primary speakers of Neve’ei and about 750 speakers in total.[M 1]
Neve’ei was described for the first time by Jill Musgrave, in a monograph published in 2007: A grammar of Neve’ei, Vanuatu. This book is the source for the data and analyses below.
Name of the language
The traditional name of the language, which is recognized by older speakers, is Neveʻei.[M 1] However, the majority of younger speakers of Neve’ei do not use the traditional name and some are not even aware of it.[M 1] Native speakers commonly refer to their language as Nabusian teget which literally means “our language” and in Bislama the language(Neveʻei) is called Lanwis Vinmavis “the language of Vinmavis”.[M 1] Neveʻei is the traditional name for the language; Vinmavis is the name of one of the villages in which the language is spoken in(Lynch and Crowley 2001:83).[3][M 1]
Phoneme inventory
Neve’ei contains 5 vowels and 20 consonants:
Vowel phonemes[M 2] | |||
---|---|---|---|
Front | Mid | Back | |
High | i | u | |
Mid | e | o | |
Low | a |
Consonant phonemes[M 3] | |||||
Labio-velar | Labial | Alveolar | Velar | Glottal | |
Voiceless stop | t | k | ʔ | ||
Voiced stop | bʷ | b | d | g | |
Nasal | mʷ | m | n | ŋ | |
Fricative | vʷ | v | s | x | h |
Lateral | l | ||||
Flap | ɾ | ||||
Glide | w | j |
By comparison with other languages of the world,[4] such a phoneme inventory classifies Neve’ei in the average range regarding vowels (5-6) as well as regarding consonants (22 ± 3). The consonant-vowel ratio classifies Neve’ei in the low band.
Phoneme properties
The table above indicates that the segments /t/, /k/ and /ʔ/ can be understood as voiceless stops at the alveolar, velar and glottal positions of articulation (Musgrave, 2007, p. 6). The segment /t/ can be understood as a voiceless alveolar stop in each position: initial, medial and final. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 6):
/tabin/ | [taᵐbɪn] | 'grandfather’ |
/nebat/ | [neᵐbat] | ‘vine variety’ |
/atl/ | [atl] | ‘third’ |
The segment /k/ can be understood as a voiceless velar stop (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7). It rarely appears with non-borrowed lexical root forms and is only found within the root /niaɾekaʔa-n/ ‘fin-CONST’ (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7).
The segment /ʔ/ can be understood as a glottal stop and appears in all positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7):
/ʔaʔan/ | [ʔaʔan] | ‘eat’ |
/nibiʔiŋ/ | [nɪᵐbiʔɪŋ] | ‘giant turban snail’ |
/bʷebaʔ/ | [ᵐbʷeᵐbaʔ] | ‘hide’ |
The segments /bʷ/, /b/, /d/ and /g/ are voiced stops at the labio-velar, bilabial, alveolar and velar positions of articulation (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7). The labio-velar stop /bʷ/ can be understood as a prenasalised bilabial stop that is voiced, involving the rounding of lips and an audible labio-velar, semi-vowel offset (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7). It can be identified in initial as well as medial positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7):
/bʷi-/ | [ᵐbʷi-] | ‘3SG.IRR (verbal prefix)’ |
/abʷit-/ | [aᵐbʷɪt-] | '2/3PL.IRR (verbal prefix)’ |
/nebʷelegen/ | [neᵐbʷeleᵑgen] | ‘thigh’ |
The bilabial stop /b/ can be understood as a prenasalised stop that is voiced [ᵐb] found in initial and medial positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7):
/baxah/ | [ᵐbaxah ~ ᵐbaɣah] | ‘clean’ |
/nubuah/ | [nuᵐbu(w)ah] | ‘pig’ |
The segment /d/ is an alveolar stop that differs from other prenasalised stops in that it can be found in all positions (initial, medial and final) (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7). In initial and medial positions, it appears as a prenasalised voiced stop [ⁿd]. Examples include (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7):
/doŋon/ | [ⁿdoŋon] | ‘count’ |
/dedan/ | [ⁿdeⁿdan] | ‘dive, swim underwater’ |
The phonetic variations of [ⁿd], [ⁿt], [ndɾ] and [nɾ] can be found when /d/ occurs in final position (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7). Examples include (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7):
/minsed/ | [mɪntʃeⁿd ~ mɪntʃeⁿt ~ mɪntʃeⁿdɾ ~ mɪntʃenɾ] |
/netarbad/ | [netaɾᵐbaⁿd ~ netaɾᵐbaⁿt ~ netaɾᵐbaⁿdɾ ~ netaɾᵐbanɾ] |
Finally, the velar stop /g/ can be understood as a voiced homorganic prenasalised stop [ᵑg] which can be found in initial and medial positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7-8):
/gis/ | [ᵑgɪs] | ‘squash, squeeze’ |
/gegeɾah/ | [ᵑgeᵑgeɾah] | ‘drag, pull’ |
The following phonemes /mʷ/, /m/, /n/ and /ŋ/ are voiced nasals occurring at the labio-velar, bilabial, alveolar and velar positions of articulation (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8). The phoneme /mʷ/ appears in initial and medial positions, involving the rounding of lips and an audible labio-velar, semi-vowel offset. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8):
/mʷaʔam/ | [mʷaʔam] | ‘sit’ |
/mamʷe/ | [mamʷe] | ‘father’ |
/nemʷen/ | [nemʷen] | ‘man’ |
The segment /m/ can be understood as a voiced bilabial nasal in each position, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8):
/mesemah/ | [mesemah] | ‘dry’ |
/nomomox/ | [nomomox] | ‘woman’ |
/dam/ | [ⁿdam] | ‘shout’ |
The segment /n/ can be understood as a voiced alveolar nasal in each position, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8):
/nabuŋ/ | [naᵐbuŋ] | ‘time, occasion, day’ |
/vanili/ | [fanili] | ‘different, strange, unusual’ |
/dan/ | [ⁿdan] | ‘go down, sink’ |
Finally, the segment /ŋ/ can be understood as a voiced velar nasal in each position, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8):
/ŋaŋ/ | [ŋaŋ] | ‘laugh’ |
/doŋon/ | [ⁿdoŋon] | ‘count’ |
/naʔaibuŋ/ | [naʔaiᵐbuŋ] | ‘my grandchild’ |
The phonemes /vʷ/, /v/, /s/, /x/ and /h/ can be understood as fricatives at the labio-velar, labio-dental, alveolar, velar and glottal positions of articulation (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8). The segment /vʷ/ is a labio-velar fricative involving the rounding of lips and an audible labio-velar, semi-vowel offset (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8). In initial position it is voiceless but can be optionally voiced intervocalically and does not appear in final position. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 8-9):
/vʷelem/ | [fʷelem] | ‘come’ |
/nevʷen/ | [nefʷen ~ nevʷen] | ‘fruit’ |
The segment /v/ can be understood as a labio-dental fricative (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). In initial and final positions it is voiceless however, it can be voiced intervocalically. In some cases such as in syllable-final positions before /m/ and /b/, /v/ can be understood as [p] while in other syllable-final positions, there is variation between [f] and [p] (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9):
/vavu/ | [fafu ~ favu] | ‘walk’ |
/navmolto/ | [napmolto] | ‘kind of bird’ |
/tenev/ | [tenef ~ tenep] | ‘yesterday’ |
The segment /s/ can be understood as a voiceless alveolar grooved fricative (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). If /s/ follows /n/ it is understood as the affricate [tʃ] in initial and medial positions but is understood as [s] in final positions. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9):
/sido/ | [sɪⁿdo] | ‘remember’ |
/bus/ | [ᵐbus] | ‘speak, talk’ |
/nsev/ | [ntʃef] | ‘cough’ |
The segment /x/ can be understood as a velar fricative that is voiceless in initial and final positions but can be optionally voiced intervocalically. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9):
/xus/ | [xus] | ‘hit, kill’ |
/buxut/ | [ᵐbuxut ~ ᵐbuɣut] | ‘inside’ |
/maʔabux/ | [maʔaᵐbux] | ‘short’ |
The segment /h/ can be understood as a glottal fricative that is voiceless (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). It is attested in final positions with the exception of /ahau/ (which means ‘no’ in answer to an affirmative question or ‘yes’ in answer to a negative question) (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). It is attested in medial positions such as /eheʔ/ ‘no’ (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9):
/vuluh/ | [fuluh] | ‘bake in earth oven’ |
/ahau/ | [ahau] | ‘no, yes (depending on the question)’ |
/eheʔ/ | [eheʔ] | ‘no’ |
The segment /l/ can be understood as an alveolar lateral that is voiced (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9). It can be identified in all positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 9-10):
/lax/ | [lax] | ‘hang’ |
/bʷeli/ | [ᵐbʷeli] | ‘very, a lot’ |
/namul/ | [namul] | ‘million’ |
The segment /ɾ/ can be understood as an alveolar flap that is voiced (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10). It can be identified in all positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10):
/ɾaʔ/ | [ɾaʔ] | ‘work (in garden)’ |
/viviɾoŋ/ | [fifiɾoŋ ~ fiviɾoŋ] | ‘listen’ |
/boɾ/ | [ᵐboɾ] | ‘deaf’ |
The glides /w/ and /j/ can only be found in syllable-initial positions (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10). The segment /w/ is s a semi-vowel that can be understood as a labio-velar approximate [w] that is voiced (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10):
/wal/ | [wal] | ‘because’ |
/muluwul/ | [muluwul] | ‘round’ |
/lowi/ | [lowi] | ‘out, away’ |
The segment /j/ is a semi-vowel that can be understood as a palatal approximate that is voiced (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10):
/jaʔai/ | [jaʔai] | ‘this person’ |
/joxjox/ | [joxjox] | ‘vomit’ |
/nijim/ | [nijɪm] | ‘house’ |
Consonant phoneme contrasts
The consonants that are phonetically similar in Neve’ei can be contrasted in the following ways (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10-11):
/b/ | /beɾ/ | ‘long, tall, deep’ |
/bʷ/ | /bʷeɾ/ | ‘possibly, maybe’ |
/m/ | /melemal/ | ‘have cramp’ |
/mʷ/ | /mʷelemal/ | ‘straight, correct’ |
/v/ | /veɾi/ | ‘outside’ |
/vʷ/ | /vʷeɾi/ | ‘saying (something), tell (story)’ |
/ʔ/ | /meɾaʔ/ | ‘get up, fly, jump up’ |
/h/ | /meɾah/ | ‘light (in weight)’ |
/h/ | /veh/ | ‘carry’ |
/∅/ | /ve∅/ | ‘what’ |
/ʔ/ | /saʔ/ | ‘go up’ |
/x/ | /sax/ | ‘not’ |
/d/ | /madl/ | ‘in three days’ time’ |
/t/ | /matl/ | ‘thick, incorrect, uncircumcised’ |
/g/ | /gu/ | ‘you (pronoun)’ |
/k/ | /ku-/ | ‘2SG.IRR (verbal prefix)’ |
/ŋ/ | /ŋe/ | ‘the (demonstrative)’ |
/g/ | /ge/ | ‘the very one’ |
/l/ | /nebal/ | ‘hawk’ |
/ɾ/ | /nebaɾ/ | ‘blind person’ |
/n/ | /neban/ | ‘woman’s headdress’ |
/w/ | /wah/ | ‘look’ |
/u/ | /uah/ | ‘day after tomorrow’ |
Neutralisation of labial consonants
Contrasts between plain labials /m/, /v/ and /b/ and labio-velars /mʷ/, /vʷ/ and /bʷ/ are neutralised in circumstances such as before front vowels /i/ and /e/. Examples include (Musgrave, 2007, p. 11):
/nimins/ | ‘wooden stirring stick’ |
/nimʷinsi/ | ‘star’ |
/meli/ | ‘wilt’ |
/mʷelam/ | ‘marked, spotted’ |
/visvis/ | ‘teach’ |
/vʷisi/ | ‘train yam vine’ |
/veh/ | ‘carry’ |
/vʷelem/ | ‘come’ |
/bʷiaŋ/ | ‘all right’ |
/bial/ | ‘all over the place, all sorts of things’ |
/bʷeɾ/ | ‘maybe’ |
/beɾ/ | ‘long, tall’ |
When these consonants precede rounded vowels such as /o/ and /u/ there is no contrast and only plain labials, examples include (Musgrave, 2007, p. 11):
/moŋ/ | ‘proud’ |
/numuɾ/ | ‘person’ |
/vov/ | ‘rain’ |
/navusmo/ | ‘white flying fox’ |
/boɾ/ | ‘deaf’ |
/bubut/ | ‘do quietly’ |
Vowel phonemes
The table above (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12) indicates that the vowel /i/ can be understood as a tense high front unrounded vowel (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12). When the vowel is followed by a nasal or a prenasalised consonant and in closed syllables that end in a non-liquid alveolar consonant, it can be understood as the lax high front vowel [ɪ] (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12):
/ivah/ | [ifah ~ ivah] | ‘four’ |
/nimin/ | [nɪmɪn] | ‘bird’ |
/gis/ | [ᵑgɪs] | ‘squash, squeeze’ |
The vowel /u/ can be understood as a high back rounded vowel. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13):
/utne/ | [utne] | ‘here’ |
/lueh/ | [lu(w)eh] | ‘tired’ |
/manuanu/ | [manu(w)anu] | ‘rainbow’ |
The vowel /e/ can be understood as a mid front unrounded vowel. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13):
/etnaŋ/ | [etnaŋ] | ‘that thing there’ |
/dedan/ | [ⁿdeⁿdan] | ‘dive, swim underwater’ |
/mamʷe/ | [mamʷe] | ‘father’ |
The vowel /o/ can be understood as a mid back rounded vowel. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13):
/oʔo/ | [oʔo] | ‘yes’ |
/nomomox/ | [nomomox] | ‘woman’ |
/noto/ | [noto] | ‘chicken’ |
The vowel /a/ can be understood as a low front unrounded vowel. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13):
/alimin/ | [alɪmɪn] | ‘fifth’ |
/mavis/ | [mafɪs ~ mavɪs] | ‘white’ |
/naʔanian/ | [naʔani(j)an] | ‘food’ |
Vowel phoneme contrasts
The forms listed below provide contrast between the five vowel phonemes in the Neve’ei language (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13):
/i/ | /bir/ | ‘fart audibly’ |
/u/ | /bur-/ | ‘1DL.IRR (verbal prefix)’ |
/e/ | /ber/ | ‘long, tall, deep’ |
/o/ | /bor/ | ‘deaf’ |
/a/ | /bar/ | ‘blind’ |
Phonotactics
Syllable Structure
The table below shows the various syllable structures attested in Neve’ei (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13-14):
V(vowel), C(consonant) |
Neveʻei | English |
---|---|---|
V | /u-/
/i-/ |
'2SG.REAL(verbal prefix)'
'3SG.REAL(verbal prefix)' |
VC | /im/
/aɾ/ |
'only, just'
'they, them (pronoun)' |
VCC | /itl/ | 'three' |
CV | /vi/ | 'do, make' |
CC | /tn/ | 'roast' |
CCV | /nsu/
/tno/ |
'remove bark of'
'my (pronoun)' |
CCVC | /nsev/
/tlel/ |
'cough'
'secret' |
CVC | /dan/
/veh/ |
'go down, sink'
'carry' |
CVCC | /matl/
/bans/ |
'thick'
'wander' |
CCVCC | /nsutl/ | 'eight' |
Syllables that consist of two vowels are also present, but are only used for specific reasons (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13). While most two non-like vowels can be used to form a morpheme, there are certain exceptions that must be noted (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13).
Impermissible vowel sequences[M 4] | |
---|---|
eo, oe | mid vowels cannot be followed by another mid vowel |
ea | mid front vowel /e/ cannot be followed by /a/ |
iu, eu | front vowels /i/ and /e/ cannot be followed by /u/ |
ae, ao | low vowel /a/ cannot be followed by a mid vowel |
ui | back vowel /u/ cannot be followed by /i/ |
Intramorphemic possibilities
Phonotactic patterns display various possibilities within morphemes (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14).
Single segments
In root-initial forms, any of the vowels are possible but initial vowels are not common in lexical items. Initial vowels, as displayed below, are found mostly in grammatical items, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14):
/i/ | ‘he/she/it’ |
/en/ | ‘instrumental’ |
/aɾ/ | ‘they, them’ |
/oʔo/ | ‘yes’ |
/utnaŋ/ | ‘there’ |
With the exception of /h/, many lexical root forms begin with any consonant (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14). In the final root position of the lexical item the following vowels are permitted: /i/, /e/, /o/ and /u/ (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14). For example, /viviɾi/ 'spit', /mamʷe/ 'father', /noto/ 'chicken' and /manuanu/ 'rainbow' (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12-13). The vowel /a/ does not occur in the root-final position unless there is an absence of a final nasal form. An example of this can be found within prepositions that end in /n/; /len/, /sakhan/ and /nsensan/, these often appear in spoken language without the final nasal when a noun phrase follows. In saying this however, when the preposition exists without the noun phrase following, the final nasal is essential (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14-15). Any consonant can appear in the root-final position of the lexical item with the exception of the prenasalised voiced stops /bw/, /b/ and /g/ and labio-velars /vw/ and /mw/ (Musgrave, 2007, p. 15). The glottal fricative /h/ is most commonly found in the root-final position for example /bʷebaʔ/ 'hide' (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7).
Stress
In Neve'ei, stress is predictable in which the primary stress lies on the penultimate syllable where the last two syllables contain single vowels, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 21):
/ˈvenox/ | ‘steal’ |
/meˈtabux/ | ‘morning’ |
/ˈmoxot/ | ‘torn, ripped’ |
/ˈnowi/ | ‘water’ |
Neve’ei does not have a traditional orthography. Like elsewhere in Vanuatu, most speakers of Neve’ei write using either Bislama, English or sometimes French. However, Neve’ei is sometimes used in writing for certain special occasions, such as the composition of hymns, recording details of family history, and land ownership.[M 5] Due to the lack of stable spelling conventions for Neve’ei, individuals differ in the choice of orthographical methods when transcribing unusual phonemes. In the past efforts had been made to create a standard orthography, but the current situation is unknown.[M 5]
Pronouns
Independent pronouns
In Neve’ei, independent pronouns can function as verbal subjects, as verbal and prepositional objects, and as pronominal possessors following directly possessed nouns. Independent pronouns inflect according to person and number (singular, dual, or non-singular), but not according to case or grammatical gender. First person pronouns also distinguish between inclusive and exclusive in the dual and non-singular forms.[M 6]
Independent pronouns[M 6] | |||
---|---|---|---|
Singular |
Dual |
Non-singular | |
1st inclusive |
no |
getdru |
get |
1st exclusive |
gememru |
gemem | |
2nd |
gu |
gemru |
gem |
3rd |
i |
ardru |
ar |
Although Neve’ei has a distinction between dual and plural, the forms listed as non-singular can be used to refer to both dual and plural subjects. In these cases, even when the non-singular independent pronoun is used, the dual verbal prefix will be used.[M 7]
Example:[M 8]
gemem |
bwera-vu |
ran |
nourour |
toro |
1NONSG.EXCL |
1DL.IRR-go |
GOAL |
island |
big |
"We (two) will go to the mainland." |
gemem |
bwit-tovu |
ran |
nourour |
toro |
1NONSG.EXCL |
1PL.IRR-go |
GOAL |
island |
big |
"We (all) will go to the mainland." |
The pronominal forms listed as dual in the table are instead used in cases where there is a pragmatic contrast with singular or plural reference, or where two participants are acting together rather than separately.[M 9]
Example:[M 9]
ar |
ar-vwelem |
3NONSG |
3DL.REAL-come |
"They (two) came." |
ardru |
ar-vwelem |
3DL |
3DL.REAL-come |
"They (two) came together." |
Possessive pronouns
Neve’ei contains a set of possessive postmodifiers which are used as possessive adjuncts with indirectly possessed nouns. The possessive pronouns (used when the possessed noun is not overtly expressed) are derived from these possessive postmodifiers by adding the prefix ti-, although this may be omitted in some forms. Like the independent pronouns, these pronominal forms indicate person and number, and the non-singular forms may be used for dual referents. These pronominal forms also have an inclusive/exclusive distinction. Additionally, possessive postmodifiers can take singular, dual and non-singular forms, similar to independent pronouns, in which case the non-singular form is often employed for both dual and plural referents (Musgrave, 2007).
Possessive postmodifiers[M 10] | |||
---|---|---|---|
Singular |
Dual |
Non-singular | |
1st inclusive |
tno |
tegetdru |
teget |
1st excusive |
tegememru |
tegemem | |
2nd |
tugu |
tegemru |
tegem |
3rd |
ti |
terdru |
ter |
Possessive pronouns[M 10] | |||
---|---|---|---|
Singular |
Dual |
Non-singular | |
1st inclusive |
titno |
(ti)tegetdru |
(ti)teget |
1st excusive |
(ti)tegememru |
(ti)tegemem | |
2nd |
(ti)tugu |
tegemru |
(ti)tegem |
3rd |
titi |
titerdru |
titer |
Example:[M 11]
gu |
ke-takh |
natitimwen |
teget |
bwe-sevakh |
2SG |
2SG.IRR-take |
boy |
1NONSG.INCL.POSS |
3SG.IRR-one |
"You will take one of our boys." |
get |
tuan |
ba-khal |
titi |
1NONSG.INCL |
INDEF |
3SG.IRR-dig |
3SG.POSS |
"Each of us will dig his own." |
Indefinite pronouns
Neve’ei also contains a set of indefinite pronouns which function as both verbals subjects and objects, with separate forms in fast and slow speech.[M 12]
Slow speech | Fast speech | |
---|---|---|
numuruan | numurwan | "somebody, someone" |
nusutuan | nusutwan | "something" |
nisituan | nisitwan | "something" |
nei mang (mi) | "who, whoever" | |
Neve’ei mang (mi) | "what, whatever" |
Example:[M 13]
nisit | nganga' | i-leh | nusutwan |
thing | little | 3SG.REAL-see | something |
"The child saw something" |
Nouns
In Neveʻei, similar to most Oceanic Languages nouns do not change.[M 14] The inflection of nouns by numbers does not occur and instead postmodifiers are used.[M 14] Only two types of possession exist in Neveʻei.[M 14] These are direct and indirect. For a directly possessed noun, a suffix is attached directly to the noun itself and for an indirectly possessed noun, a possessive postmodifier appears before the noun.
Direct possession[M 15] | Indirect possession |
---|---|
netal-ung | noang tno |
leg-1SG | canoe 1SG.POSS |
my leg | my canoe |
Most often, directly possessed nouns have stems ending in vowels, however there are also instances in which stems ending in non-nasal alveolar consonants t, s, l and r occur (Musgrave, 2007). The following table provides examples of both instances (Musgrave, 2007 p.33):
Vowel Endings | Consonant Endings |
---|---|
khava - 'brother, friend' | nagalel - 'rib, side (of body) |
ninsibi - 'finger, toe claw (of bird/chicken)' | netal - 'foot, leg, behind leg (of animal)' |
nobologo - 'bone' | nibis - 'tail' |
(More on directly possessed nouns below: 5.1 Directly possessed nouns)
Noun Derivation
In Neveʻei complex nouns can be formed by compounding or affixation.[M 16] Compounding involves combining a noun root with either a noun or verb root. And affixation involves deriving nouns from verbs, a process in which a simulfix is added to the verb stem.[M 16]
Numerals
Cardinal Numerals (numerals that denote quantity)
Neveʻei uses a decimal (base 10) system for numbers, however many young people are only familiar with one to ten, so higher value numerals are often expressed using methods derived from Bislama.[M 17]
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
sevakh | iru | itl | ivah | ilim | nsouh | nsuru | nsutl | nsavah | nangavil (sevakh) / vungavil |
10 is an interesting number in Neveʻei because it can be either nangavil or nangavil sevkh.[M 18] Also note that nangavil is used for the actual number 10 when counting and vungavil is used to modify a phrase or noun.[M 18] An example would be noang vungavil which translates to "10 canoes" in English.[M 18]
Person markers
In Neve’ei, subject verbal prefixes are obligatory and indicate the person and number of the subject. There are two complete sets of prefixes: one for realis mood and the other for irrealis. Unlike the pronoun systems, there is no distinction between inclusive and exclusive first person, and dual prefixes are always used with dual referents. There is no set of affixes encoding the person and number of objects. Vowels in some of these prefixes may change according to the first vowel in the verb stem; this is a process of progressive assimilation at a distance.[M 19]
Realis prefixes[M 20]
Singular |
Dual |
Plural | |
---|---|---|---|
1st |
nV- |
er- |
it- |
2nd |
u- |
ar- |
at- |
3rd |
i-/∅- |
Irrealis prefixes[M 20]
Singular |
Dual |
Plural | |
---|---|---|---|
1st |
nVbwV- |
bwVr- |
bwit- |
2nd |
kV- |
abwVr- |
abwit- |
3rd |
bwV- |
Example:[M 21]
bwer-ngang |
1DL.IRR-laugh |
"we (both) will laugh" |
er-ngang |
1DL.REAL-laugh |
"we (both) laugh" |
ar-ngang |
2/3DL.REAL-laugh |
"you/they (both) laugh" |
abwer-ngang |
2/3DL.IRR-laugh |
"you/they (both) will laugh" |
The third person singular realis prefix is sometimes realised as ∅- in various environments. This is more common with certain verbs, especially verbs where the stem begins with s.
Example:[M 22]
numur |
bweradang |
∅-sakh |
person |
real |
3SG.REAL-is.not |
"...not a real person." |
Verbal prefixes ending in t also often lose the t before verb roots beginning with s.
Example:[M 22]
i-sav |
1PL.REAL-dance |
"We (all) dance" |
Reduplication in Neveʻei
Reduplication is most commonly used in Neveʻei to indicate things such as intensity, prohibition, reciprocity and habitual aspects and is also sometimes used in order to differentiate between transitive and intransitive forms of a verb.[M 23] The most common method of reduplication in Neveʻei is the repeating of the first syllable in the verb stem.[M 23]
Example[M 23]
I-bans-bans do. |
3SG.REAL-RED-wander. about a. little |
(S)he wandered about a bit. |
This example shows reduplication to express a habitual aspect.
Example[M 23]
Abur-su-khus-khus-si ∅ |
2DL.IRR-NEG1-RED-kill-NEG2 3SG |
Do not kill it. |
This example shows reduplication to express prohibition.
Example[M 23]
Ar are-te-teri ar. |
---|
2NONSG 3DL.REAL-RED-cut 3NONSG |
They cut each other. |
This example shows reduplication to express reciprocity.
Example[M 23]
Ar ar-yang-yangwal ar. |
3NONSG 3DL.REAL-RED-like 3NONSG |
They like each other. |
This example shows reduplication to express intensity.
Transitive suffix
In Neveʻei transitive and intransitive verbs are represented by a technique in which intransitive verbs become transitive through the addition of a suffix -V(vowel)n to the end of an intransitive verb stem.[M 24]
The suffix to make verbs transitive has three allomorphs. They are:
-on ~ -en (used for verb stems ending in oC(consonant)).[M 24]
-an ~ -en (used for verb stems ending in aC(consonant) where the consonant is also glottal or velar).[M 24]
-en (used elsewhere).[M 24]
Intransitive[M 24] | Transitive |
---|---|
nonong "finish" | nonong-on ~ nonong-en "finish" |
ngang "laugh" | ngang-an ~ ngang-en "laugh at" |
dedan "dive" | dedan-en "dive for" |
Apart from addition of the suffix, there are two other methods to differentiate between intransitive and transitive verbs.
One other way is for both transitive and intransitive verbs to have very different forms of expression.[M 24]
Intransitive[M 24] | Transitive | |
---|---|---|
"eat" | ʻaʻan | khan |
"steal" | vevenaʻ | venokh |
"burn" | lililmin | vang do |
The final way for intransitive and transitive verbs to differentiate from each other is through reduplication.[M 25]
Transitive[M 26] | Intransitive | |
---|---|---|
"turn around" | vilih | vil-vilih |
"buy" | vul | vul-vul |
"drink" | min | min-min |
"copulate (with)" | ʻav | ʻav-ʻav |
Negation
In Neve'ei negation is marked by the simulfixes sV- ... -si 'not' and sV- ... -vang(an) 'not yet'. The prefixed element of the negative marker directly follows the subject prefix and precedes the verb stem while the suffixed element is always the final morpheme of the verb.[M 27] The prefixed element sV- is used in both negative simulfixes with the realisation of V- depending on a process of vowel harmony. The suffixed element -si remains the same following both transitive and intransitive verbs, however the -vang(an) element from the other simulfix is -vang following intransitive verbs and -vang-an (here -an can be described as a realisation of the transitive suffix -Vn) after transitive verbs.[M 28]
The following examples[M 29] show how the negative simulfixes are used with the intransitive verb nonong 'finish' and the transitive equivalent nonong-on 'finish (something)'.
No-so-nonong-si. |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-finish-NEG2 |
'I haven't finished.' |
No-so-nonong-vang. |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-finish-NEG2 |
'I haven't finished yet.' |
No-so-nonong-on-si | nemagarian | tno. |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-finish-TRANS-NEG2 | work | 1SG.POSS |
'I haven't finished my work.' |
No-so-nonong-on-vang-an | nemagarian | tno. |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-finish-TRANS-NEG2-TRANS | work | 1SG.POSS |
'I haven't finished my work yet.' |
When a sentence containing a verb with the completive suffix -i is negated the simulfix usually replaces the completive suffix as in the following example where mah-i becomes mah-vang-an in the negative version.[M 30]
Ne-vweri | mah-i | ∅ |
1SG.REAL-say | finish.doing-COMP | 3SG |
'I've already said it.' |
Ne-se-vweri | mah-vang-an | ∅ |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-say | finish.doing-NEG2-TRANS | 3SG |
'I haven't said it yet.' |
Complex Verbs
Neve'ei allows for inflected verb stems to consist of a sequence of two or more stems. These verbs in sequence are negated as a single unit with the negative prefix preceding the first verb stem and the suffix following the final verb stem in the sequence.[M 31]
For example:[M 32]
I-si-yel | totovung-si. |
3SG.REAL-NEG1-sing | try-NEG2 |
'(S)he didn't try to sing.' |
I-so-rogulel | lieh | mo-si | bwe-sa` |
3SG.REAL-NEG1-able | again | do.any.more-NEG2 | 3SG.IRR-go.up |
'he couldn't go up any more' |
Auxiliaries
The following verbs in Neve'ei are attested as both as independent verbs and as serial verbs expressing aspect.[M 33]
magar(-en) | 'progressive' |
mera` | 'inceptive' |
tokh | 'habitual' |
vwer | 'actual' |
When constructions containing the aspectual auxiliaries (with the exception of vwer for which there are no examples of negated constructions) are negated the main verb carries the negative simulfix while the auxiliary appears in the affirmative.[M 35] Illustrated in the following example containing the auxiliary mera` 'inceptive' and the verb stem rong 'listen'.[M 36]
U-mera` | u-son-rong | do-si. |
2SG.REAL-INCEP | 2SG.REAL-NEG1-listen | a.little-NEG2 |
'Then you didn't listen at all.' |
Neve'ei also contains the following verbs that are attested as both independent verbs and serial verbs expressing modality.[M 37]
rogulel | 'abilitative' |
yangwal | 'desiderative' |
sisi | 'anti-desiderative' |
vwer | 'intentional' |
In contrast with the negation pattern for the aspectual auxiliaries the modal auxiliaries carry the negative simulfix while the main verb has affirmative polarity as shown in the following examples. [M 39]
No-so-rogulel-si | nebwe-lahk. |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-ABIL-NEG2 | 1SG.IRR-hang |
'I can't hang.' |
Ni-si-yangwal-si | nebwe-vweri | nabusian | nene. |
1SG.REAL-NEG1-DESID-NEG2 | 1SG.IRR-say | story | DEM |
'I don't want to tell this story.' |
Even though yangwai 'desiderative' can be negated as seen in the previous example there is also the separate verb sisi that expresses anti-desiderative modality. As is the case when it appears as an aspectual auxiliary vwer also cannot be negated when used to express intentional modality instead sisi is used to express negative intention.[M 40]
Directional serial verbs
To negate directional serial constructions only the first verb carries the simulfix as in the following example where vavu 'walk' carries the negation and the directional verb vu 'thither' appears in the affirmative.[M 41]
I-sa-vavu-si | (i-)vu | len | niyim. |
3SG.REAL-NEG1-walk-NEG2 | 3SG.REAL-thither | GOAL | house |
'He didn't walk there to the house.' |
Adnominal and Pronominal Possession
Directly possessed nouns
Extending on directly possessed noun stems in section 4.2. Nouns, when these directly possessed nouns appear to have a nominal possessor that is when the directly possessed nouns do not refer to a particular possessor, a suffix which is identical to the person singular pronominal suffix –n, known as a construct suffix (Musgrave, 2007). The construct suffixes are depicted in the table below:
After consonant | After vowels | |
---|---|---|
1 | -ung/-ing | -ng |
2 | -um/im | -m |
3 | -n | -n |
The construct suffix –ing/im is attached to –iC (where C is consonant) root stems, whereas –ung/um suffixes follow noun stem roots ending in all other consonants. Additionally, noun roots that end in a vowel have the suffix –ng/-m attached.
Some examples from Musgrave's (2007, p.34) grammar include:
nemelnibis-ing – ‘my saliva’
nemelnibis-im – ‘your saliva’
nemelnibis-n – ‘his/her saliva’
Unlike many other languages closely related to Neve’ei, the patterns for directly suffixed nouns are not as complicated. As many of the closely related languages have distinct pronominal suffixes that correspond to both singular and non-singular pronouns independent pronouns, Neve’ei is moving away from a rigid difference between how pronominal and nominal possession is demonstrated (Musgrave, 2007).
According to Musgrave's (2007) grammar findings, when found without a reference to a specific possessor, directly possessed nouns are displayed in their construct form, and the third person non-singular independent pronoun –ar and the plural postmodifier –ar are identical, their semantics must be worked out in context. For instance, depending on the context nat-n ar could be translated to ‘the children’, with the plural postmodifier, or ‘their children’, possession (Musgrave, 2007, p.35).
E.g.:
... ar-sido | etenen | ar-vweri | en | nat-n | ar. |
3DL.REAL-remember | what | 3DL.REAL-say | GOAL | child-CONST | PL/3NONSG.POSS |
‘… they remembered what they said to the children/their children’ |
However, determining the differences between the uses of the singular pronominal possessor constructions, it has been found that pronominal suffix formations are used more often for third person singular possessors and for first person singular possessors as a term of address, in narratives (Musgrave, 2007).
Nominal possession
Nominal possessive pronouns
Extending on aforementioned possessive pronoun information, when the nominal phrase head is taken by a possessive pronoun, the postmodifiers used are determiners and plural postmodifiers. This is explained in the following formula:
POSSESSIVE PRONOUN + (DETERMINER) + (PLURAL POSTMODIFIER)
For example, the possessive pronouns (ti)tugu precedes the determiner nge:
Uten | gu | u-sa-khal-si | tugu | nge |
2SG | 2SG.REAL-NEG1-dig-NEG2 | 2NONSG.POSS | DEM | |
‘If you don’t dig yours…’ |
When indefinite pronouns inhabit the nominal phrase head, possessive postmodifiers, determiners and plural postmodifiers are the only postmodifiers employed. As such, they follow the following formula:
INDEFINITE PRONOUN + (POSSESSIVE POSTMODIFIER) + (DETERMINER) + (PLURAL POSTMODIFIER)
Musgrave (2007, p.62) demonstrates an example of this construction as follows: the indefinite pronoun nusutwan precedes the possessive postmodifier ter and the plural postmodifier ar.
Ara-vu | ar-takh | nusutwan | ter | ar |
3DL.REAL-go | 3DL.REAL-take | something | 3NONSG.POSS | PL |
‘They a went and took their things’ |
Nominal possessive postmodifiers
Indirectly possessed nouns are unable to take a possessive suffix, and therefore adopt possessive modifier forms instead. An example of this is found in Musgrave's (2007) grammar.
I-leh | yokhoi | nang | ti | i-vanili | mang |
3SG.REAL-see | that.person | mother | SG.POSS | 3SG.REAL-different | very.much |
‘He saw that person’s mother was very different’ |
Complex possessive constructions
Musgrave's (2007) grammar book identifies that Neve’ei, like most languages, has two main types of possession, direct and indirect possession. The constructions of possession show how the language has deviated from typical rules of grammar from Proto Oceanic language system and other languages of Vanuatu. A main difference identified in Musgrave's (2007) grammar is that Neve’ei does not juxtapose different indirect possessive subtypes and its direct possessive forms are reduced to singular pronominal possessors, as opposed to the Proto Oceanic system.
Direct possessive constructions
As previously discussed, if a directly possessed noun has a nominal possessor, in its construct form, a possessor follows the directly possessed nouns. These directly possessed nouns have a very close semantic relationship between the referent of the possessed and possessor nouns, referred to as inalienable possession (Musgrave, 2007). An inalienable relationship describes a situation when the possessed noun does not exist separately from the possessor. This notion is exhibited by the following formula:
POSSESSED NOUN + POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSSESSOR NOUN MODIFIERS) + (POSSESSED NOUN POSTMODIFIERS)
Consequently, no postmodifiers belonging to the directly possessed noun can come between it and the nominal possessor.
Most often the categories of alienable possessive constructions relate to kin/relationship terms, parts of a plant, body parts of humans and animals etc. Examples from Musgrave (2007, p.71) are listed below.
Neve'ei | English | |
---|---|---|
Kinship | na’aibi-n
nat-n tabi-n |
‘grandchild’
‘child' ‘grandfather |
Animal/human body parts | na’adle-n
na-avera-n neleme-n |
'egg (of bird or turtle, roe (of fish)’
‘wing’ ‘tongue’ |
Plant parts | na’ansemwe-n
nokhora-n netevi-n |
'stem (of leaf, fruit)
‘root’ ‘shoot (of a plant)’ |
Bodily products | nemakha-n
nemwetebwe-n |
'urine’
‘excrement’ |
A possessor noun can be moved to the beginning of the possessive construction in order to show the prominence of the noun. Additionally, the presence or absence of a postmodifier behind the possessor noun depends on the level of animacy, consequently highly animate possessors (such as animals and humans) allow the directly possessed noun to follow with or without intervening postmodifiers. On the contrary, trees and plants etc. are considered less animate and therefore a postmodifier must follow the possessor noun and precede the possessed noun. Two formula's describe these animate constructions below (Musgrave, 2007, p.72):
- Highly animate: POSSESSOR NOUN+ (POSSESSOR NOUN POSTMODIFIERS) + POSSESSED NOUN + (POSSESSED NOUN POSTMODIFIERS)
nemwat | ar | nebat-n | at-met. |
snake | PL | head-CONST | 3PL.REAL-black |
'The snakes' heads are black.' |
- Less animate: [POSSESSOR NOUN + POSSESSOR POSTMODIFIERS + POSSESSED NOUN + (POSSESSED NOUN POSTMODIFIERS)
nakhankhan | nge | nevwene-n | sevakh |
pawpaw | DEM | fruit-CONST | one |
'one fruit of the pawpaw' |
Indirect possessive constructions
Similarly to the inalienable relationship of directly suffixed possessed nouns, indirectly possessed nouns can also establish an inalienable relationship. This fact would suggest there is some overlap between the semantic features of alienable and inalienable relationships.
The following examples from Musgrave (2007, p.73) demonstrate indirectly possessed nouns that may be expected to establish a close semantic or inalienable relationship with their noun possessors.
Mamwe – ‘father, father’s sister’s son’
Mamwilam – ‘father’s eldest brother’
Nang – ‘mother, mother’s sister, father’s sister’s son’s wife’
Nang tokhtokh – ‘mother’s elder sister’
When an indirectly possessed noun has a pronominal possessor, it precedes a possessive postmodifier. However, there are two outcomes for a possessive construction when an indirectly possessed noun takes a nominal possessor.
In the first construction the possessive postmodifier agrees with the number of the possessor noun, establishing a semantic relationship that is distant or alienable, as the possessed noun exists independently of the possessor (adnominal possession). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p.73):
POSSESSED NOUN + POSSESSIVE POSTMODIFIER + POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSTMODIFIERS)
libakh | ti | matoro |
dog | 3NONSNG.POSS | old.man |
‘the old man’s dog.’ |
POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSTMODIFIERS) + POSSESSED NOUN + POSSESSIVE POSTMODIFIER
matoro | libakh | ti |
old.man | dog | 3SG.POSS |
‘the old man’s dog.’ |
Another indirect possessive construction includes a possessive marker nen, which acts similarly to a relativiser. The formula demonstrates that nen is found in between the possessed nominal and the possessor nominal.
POSSESSED NOUN + NEN + POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSSESSOR POSTMODIFIERS) + (POSSESSED POSTMODIFIERS)
These constructions often pertain to a certain semantic category: purposive relationships, habitual relations and possession of body parts. In the following example from Musgrave (2007, p.75), the possessed noun is a person and the possessor noun in an occupation:
numur | nen | navisvisian |
person | POSS | teaching |
'teacher' |
While some body parts are directly possessed nouns, some body parts (such as internal organs, bodily exudations etc.) are indirectly possessed nouns and most often pertain to an alienable semantic relationship.
nidri | nen | libakh |
blood | POSS | dog |
'the dog's blood' |
nowi | nen | netal-n |
water | POSS | leg-CONST |
'leg vains' |
References
- Consonant Inventories. (2013). In World Atlas of Language Structures. Retrieved from http://wals.info/chapter/1
- Lynch & Crowley, 2001, Languages of Vanuatu: A New Survey and Bibliography. Australian National University.
- Musgrave, J. (2007). A grammar of Neve’ei, Vanuatu. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
Notes
- ^ Neveʻei language at Ethnologue (15th ed., 2005)
- ^ Neve’ei at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
- ^ See ISO 639-3, 2008, Change Request Number 2008-012.
- ^ Maddieson, Ian (2013). "Consonant Inventories". World Atlas of Language Structures.
- References from: Musgrave, J. (2007). A grammar of Neve’ei, Vanuatu. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
- ^ a b c d e f Musgrave 2007, p.3
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.12
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.6.
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (table 2.5), p.19
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p.28
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p. 29
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p. 29-30
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.30
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p. 30
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p.31
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p. 30-31
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p. 32
- ^ Musgrave 2007 p. 62
- ^ a b c Musgrave 2007, p.33
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (table 3.5), p.33
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p.36
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.40
- ^ a b c Musgrave 2007, p.41
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p. 44-45
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p. 44
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p. 46
- ^ a b Musgrave 2007, p. 45
- ^ a b c d e f Musgrave 2007, p.54
- ^ a b c d e f g Musgrave 2007, (table 3.11), p.56
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.57
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (table 3.12), p.57
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.51
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.52
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (example 3.56a-d), p.52
- ^ Musgrove 2007, (examples 3.62-3), p.54
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.77-78
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (examples 5.2b, 5.4a), p.78
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.88
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (table 5.5), p.88
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.91
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (example 5.29c), p.91
- ^ Musgrave 2007, p.92
- ^ Musgrove 2007, (table 5.6), p.92
- ^ Musgrave 2007, (example 5.35a-b), p.93
- ^ Musgrove 2007, p.94
- ^ Musgrove 2007, (example 5.43f), p.96